Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2024

18-08-2023 | Research

Similarities of SNARC, cognitive Simon, and visuomotor Simon effects in terms of response time distributions, hand-stimulus proximity, and temporal dynamics

Auteurs: Lizhu Yan, Yilin Ma, Weibin Yang, Xinrui Xiang, Weizhi Nan

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2024

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) and Simon effects are attributed to the same type of conflict according to dimensional overlap (DO) theory: the congruency of task-irrelevant spatial information and the selected response (e.g., left or right). However, previous studies have yielded inconsistent results regarding the relationship between the two effects, with some studies reporting an interaction while others did not. This discrepancy may be attributed to the use of different types of Simon effects (visuomotor and cognitive Simon effects) in these studies, as the spatial codes associated with these two types of Simon effects are distinct (exogenous and endogenous, respectively). The aim of this study was to address these inconsistencies and gain a better understanding of the similarities and differences in spatial representations generated by spatial location, semantic information, and numerical information. We attempted to classify the relationships among the SNARC and Simon effects. Specifically, the visuomotor Simon, cognitive Simon, and SNARC effects were compared from three perspectives: the response time (RT) distribution, hand-stimulus proximity, and temporal dynamics (with the drift diffusion model; DDM). Regarding RTs, the results showed that the visuomotor Simon effect decreased with increased values of RT bins, while the cognitive Simon and SNARC effects increased. Additionally, the visuomotor Simon effect was the only effect influenced by hand-stimulus proximity, with a stronger effect observed in the hand-proximal condition than in the hand-distal condition. Regarding the DDM results, only the visuomotor Simon effect exhibited a higher drift rate and longer non-decision time in the incompatible condition than in the compatible condition. Conversely, both the SNARC and cognitive Simon effects exhibited an inverse pattern regarding the drift rate and no significant difference in non-decision time between the two conditions. These findings suggest that the SNARC effect is more similar to the cognitive Simon effect than the visuomotor Simon effect, indicating that the endogenous spatial-numerical representation of the SNARC effect might share an underlying processing mechanism with the endogenous spatial–semantic representation of the cognitive Simon effect but not with the exogenous location representation of the visuomotor Simon effect. Our results further demonstrate that the origin of spatial information could impact the classification of conflicts and supplement DO theory.
Literatuur
go back to reference Abrahamse, E., Braem, S., Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2016). Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. Psychological Bulletin, 142(7), 693–728.PubMedCrossRef Abrahamse, E., Braem, S., Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2016). Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. Psychological Bulletin, 142(7), 693–728.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2007). Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. Psychological Review, 114(4), 1076–1086.PubMedCrossRef Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2007). Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. Psychological Review, 114(4), 1076–1086.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Boag, R. J., Strickland, L., Heathcote, A., Neal, A., & Loft, S. (2019). Cognitive control and capacity for prospective memory in complex dynamic environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(12), 2181–2206.PubMedCrossRef Boag, R. J., Strickland, L., Heathcote, A., Neal, A., & Loft, S. (2019). Cognitive control and capacity for prospective memory in complex dynamic environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(12), 2181–2206.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Braem, S., Bugg, J. M., Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J. C., Weissman, D. H., Notebaert, W., & Egner, T. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(9), 769–783.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Braem, S., Bugg, J. M., Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J. C., Weissman, D. H., Notebaert, W., & Egner, T. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(9), 769–783.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)?. Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 5–13. Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)?. Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 5–13.
go back to reference Cao, Y., Cao, X., Yue, Z., & Wang, L. (2017). Temporal and spectral dynamics underlying cognitive control modulated by task-irrelevant stimulus–response learning. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(1), 158–173.CrossRef Cao, Y., Cao, X., Yue, Z., & Wang, L. (2017). Temporal and spectral dynamics underlying cognitive control modulated by task-irrelevant stimulus–response learning. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(1), 158–173.CrossRef
go back to reference Dillon, D. G., Wiecki, T., Pechtel, P., Webb, C., Goer, F., Murray, L., Trivedi, M., Fava, M., McGrath, P. J., Weissman, M., Parsey, R., Kurian, B., Adams, P., Carmody, T., Weyandt, S., Shores-Wilson, K., Toups, M., McInnis, M., Oquendo, M. A., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). A computational analysis of flanker interference in depression. Psychological Medicine, 45(11), 2333–2344.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Dillon, D. G., Wiecki, T., Pechtel, P., Webb, C., Goer, F., Murray, L., Trivedi, M., Fava, M., McGrath, P. J., Weissman, M., Parsey, R., Kurian, B., Adams, P., Carmody, T., Weyandt, S., Shores-Wilson, K., Toups, M., McInnis, M., Oquendo, M. A., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). A computational analysis of flanker interference in depression. Psychological Medicine, 45(11), 2333–2344.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Dufour, A., & Touzalin, P. (2008). Improved visual sensitivity in the perihand space. Experimental Brain Research, 190, 91–98.PubMedCrossRef Dufour, A., & Touzalin, P. (2008). Improved visual sensitivity in the perihand space. Experimental Brain Research, 190, 91–98.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8(12), 12.CrossRef Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8(12), 12.CrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Faulkenberry, T. J. (2018). Computing Bayes factors to measure evidence from experiments: An extension of the BIC approximation. arXiv preprint arXiv:1803.00360 Faulkenberry, T. J. (2018). Computing Bayes factors to measure evidence from experiments: An extension of the BIC approximation. arXiv preprint arXiv:1803.00360
go back to reference Fitousi, D., & Algom, D. (2020). A model for two-digit number processing based on a joint Garner and system factorial technology analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 676–700.PubMedCrossRef Fitousi, D., & Algom, D. (2020). A model for two-digit number processing based on a joint Garner and system factorial technology analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 676–700.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fröber, K., & Lerche, V. (2023). Performance-contingent reward increases the use of congruent distracting information. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 85(3), 905–929.CrossRef Fröber, K., & Lerche, V. (2023). Performance-contingent reward increases the use of congruent distracting information. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 85(3), 905–929.CrossRef
go back to reference Gozli, D. G., Ardron, J., & Pratt, J. (2014). Reduced visual feature binding in the near-hand space. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 76(5), 1308–1317.CrossRef Gozli, D. G., Ardron, J., & Pratt, J. (2014). Reduced visual feature binding in the near-hand space. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 76(5), 1308–1317.CrossRef
go back to reference Greenhouse, S. W., & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24(2), 95–112. Greenhouse, S. W., & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24(2), 95–112.
go back to reference Heathcote, A., Lin, Y.-S., Reynolds, A., Strickland, L., Gretton, M., & Matzke, D. (2019). Dynamic models of choice. Behavior Research Methods, 51(2), 961–985.PubMedCrossRef Heathcote, A., Lin, Y.-S., Reynolds, A., Strickland, L., Gretton, M., & Matzke, D. (2019). Dynamic models of choice. Behavior Research Methods, 51(2), 961–985.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56(4), 261–268.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56(4), 261–268.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S. (1994). The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56, 130–135.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S. (1994). The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56, 130–135.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S., & Lee, J.-W. (1995). Stimulus–response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855–875.PubMed Kornblum, S., & Lee, J.-W. (1995). Stimulus–response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855–875.PubMed
go back to reference Liu, X., Park, Y., Gu, X., & Fan, J. (2010). Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus–response compatibility effects. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72(6), 1710–1720.CrossRef Liu, X., Park, Y., Gu, X., & Fan, J. (2010). Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus–response compatibility effects. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72(6), 1710–1720.CrossRef
go back to reference Liu, T., Wang, D., Wang, C., Xiao, T., & Shi, J. (2022). The influence of reward anticipation on conflict control in children and adolescents: Evidences from hierarchical drift-diffusion model and event-related potentials. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 55, 101118. Liu, T., Wang, D., Wang, C., Xiao, T., & Shi, J. (2022). The influence of reward anticipation on conflict control in children and adolescents: Evidences from hierarchical drift-diffusion model and event-related potentials. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 55, 101118.
go back to reference Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2020). Shared mechanisms underlying the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 84(6), 1655–1667.PubMedCrossRef Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2020). Shared mechanisms underlying the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 84(6), 1655–1667.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2018). BayseFactor: computation of bayes factors for common designs. Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2018). BayseFactor: computation of bayes factors for common designs.
go back to reference Myers, C. E., Interian, A., & Moustafa, A. A. (2022). A practical introduction to using the drift diffusion model of decision-making in cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and health sciences. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1039172.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Myers, C. E., Interian, A., & Moustafa, A. A. (2022). A practical introduction to using the drift diffusion model of decision-making in cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and health sciences. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1039172.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Pellegrino, G., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). Direct evidence from parietal extinction of enhancement of visual attention near a visible hand. Current Biology, 10(22), 1475–1477.CrossRef Pellegrino, G., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). Direct evidence from parietal extinction of enhancement of visual attention near a visible hand. Current Biology, 10(22), 1475–1477.CrossRef
go back to reference Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: a time-course analysis. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 219–227.PubMedCrossRef Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: a time-course analysis. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 219–227.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between stroop and Simon effects. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72(7), 2013–2025.CrossRef Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between stroop and Simon effects. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72(7), 2013–2025.CrossRef
go back to reference Pressigout, A., & Dore-Mazars, K. (2020). How does number magnitude influence temporal and spatial parameters of eye movements? Experimental Brain Research, 238, 101–109.PubMedCrossRef Pressigout, A., & Dore-Mazars, K. (2020). How does number magnitude influence temporal and spatial parameters of eye movements? Experimental Brain Research, 238, 101–109.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Marble, J. G., & Vu, K.-P.L. (2000). Mixing incompatibly mapped location-relevant trials with location-irrelevant trials: effects of stimulus mode on the reverse Simon effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 64, 11–24.PubMedCrossRef Proctor, R. W., Marble, J. G., & Vu, K.-P.L. (2000). Mixing incompatibly mapped location-relevant trials with location-irrelevant trials: effects of stimulus mode on the reverse Simon effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 64, 11–24.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.CrossRef Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.CrossRef
go back to reference Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 446–461.PubMedCrossRef Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 446–461.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Decision diffusion model: current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260–281.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Decision diffusion model: current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260–281.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Reed, C. L., Garza, J. P., & Vyas, D. B. (2018). Feeling but not seeing the hand: occluded hand position reduces the hand proximity effect in ERPs. Consciousness and Cognition, 64, 154–163.PubMedCrossRef Reed, C. L., Garza, J. P., & Vyas, D. B. (2018). Feeling but not seeing the hand: occluded hand position reduces the hand proximity effect in ERPs. Consciousness and Cognition, 64, 154–163.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reed, C. L., Grubb, J. D., & Steele, C. (2006). Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 166–177.PubMed Reed, C. L., Grubb, J. D., & Steele, C. (2006). Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 166–177.PubMed
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Mechanisms in Perception and Action (pp. 494–519). Oxford University Press.CrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Mechanisms in Perception and Action (pp. 494–519). Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 514–523.PubMed Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 514–523.PubMed
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J. C., Cheesman, J., & Besner, D. (2007). Contingency learning without awareness: evidence for implicit control. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(2), 421–435.PubMedCrossRef Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J. C., Cheesman, J., & Besner, D. (2007). Contingency learning without awareness: evidence for implicit control. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(2), 421–435.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Pütz, S. (2021). Mood state and conflict adaptation: an update and a diffusion model analysis. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(1), 322–344.PubMedCrossRef Schuch, S., & Pütz, S. (2021). Mood state and conflict adaptation: an update and a diffusion model analysis. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(1), 322–344.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Servant, M., & Evans, N. J. (2020). A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging in the Flanker Task. Psychology and Aging, 35(6), 831–849.PubMedCrossRef Servant, M., & Evans, N. J. (2020). A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging in the Flanker Task. Psychology and Aging, 35(6), 831–849.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., & Van Der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (statistical Methodology), 64(4), 583–639.MathSciNetCrossRef Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., & Van Der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (statistical Methodology), 64(4), 583–639.MathSciNetCrossRef
go back to reference Suh, J., & Abrams, R. A. (2015). Reduced object-based perception in the near-hand space. Experimental Brain Research, 233(12), 3403–3412.PubMedCrossRef Suh, J., & Abrams, R. A. (2015). Reduced object-based perception in the near-hand space. Experimental Brain Research, 233(12), 3403–3412.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Turner, B. M., Sederberg, P. B., Brown, S. D., & Steyvers, M. (2013). A method for efficiently sampling from distributions with correlated dimensions. Psychological Methods, 18(3), 368–384.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Turner, B. M., Sederberg, P. B., Brown, S. D., & Steyvers, M. (2013). A method for efficiently sampling from distributions with correlated dimensions. Psychological Methods, 18(3), 368–384.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148–174.PubMedCrossRef Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148–174.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Ravenzwaaij, D., Cassey, P., & Brown, S. D. (2018). A simple introduction to Markov Chain Monte-Carlo sampling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 143–154.CrossRef Van Ravenzwaaij, D., Cassey, P., & Brown, S. D. (2018). A simple introduction to Markov Chain Monte-Carlo sampling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 143–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Voss, A., Rothermund, K., & Voss, J. (2004). Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: an empirical validation. Memory & Cognition, 32(7), 1206–1220.CrossRef Voss, A., Rothermund, K., & Voss, J. (2004). Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: an empirical validation. Memory & Cognition, 32(7), 1206–1220.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, X., Du, F., He, X., & Zhang, K. (2014). Enhanced spatial stimulus–response mapping near the hands: the Simon effect is modulated by hand-stimulus proximity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(6), 2252–2265.PubMed Wang, X., Du, F., He, X., & Zhang, K. (2014). Enhanced spatial stimulus–response mapping near the hands: the Simon effect is modulated by hand-stimulus proximity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(6), 2252–2265.PubMed
go back to reference Wang, X., Du, S., Zhang, K., & Du, F. (2021). An auditory hand-proximity effect: the auditory Simon effect is enhanced near the hands. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28, 853–861.CrossRef Wang, X., Du, S., Zhang, K., & Du, F. (2021). An auditory hand-proximity effect: the auditory Simon effect is enhanced near the hands. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28, 853–861.CrossRef
go back to reference Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed
go back to reference White, C. N., Ratcliff, R., & Starns, J. J. (2011). Diffusion models of the flanker task: discrete versus gradual attentional selection. Cognitive Psychology, 63(4), 210–238.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef White, C. N., Ratcliff, R., & Starns, J. J. (2011). Diffusion models of the flanker task: discrete versus gradual attentional selection. Cognitive Psychology, 63(4), 210–238.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: Evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 453–464.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: Evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 453–464.PubMed
go back to reference Xia, T., Li, H., & Wang, L. (2016). Implicitly strengthened task-irrelevant stimulus–response associations modulate cognitive control: evidence from an fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 37(2), 756–772.MathSciNetPubMedCrossRef Xia, T., Li, H., & Wang, L. (2016). Implicitly strengthened task-irrelevant stimulus–response associations modulate cognitive control: evidence from an fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 37(2), 756–772.MathSciNetPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Xia, X., Guo, M., & Wang, L. (2023). Learning of irrelevant stimulus–response associations modulates cognitive control. NeuroImage, 276, 120206.PubMedCrossRef Xia, X., Guo, M., & Wang, L. (2023). Learning of irrelevant stimulus–response associations modulates cognitive control. NeuroImage, 276, 120206.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Xiang, X., Yan, L., Fu, S., Nan, W. (2022). Processing stage flexibility of the SNARC effect: Task relevance or magnitude relevance? Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1022999. Xiang, X., Yan, L., Fu, S., Nan, W. (2022). Processing stage flexibility of the SNARC effect: Task relevance or magnitude relevance? Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1022999.
go back to reference Yan, L., Yang, G., Nan, W., Liu, X., & Fu, S. (2021). The SNARC effect occurs in the response-selection stage. Acta Psychologica, 215(8), 103292.PubMedCrossRef Yan, L., Yang, G., Nan, W., Liu, X., & Fu, S. (2021). The SNARC effect occurs in the response-selection stage. Acta Psychologica, 215(8), 103292.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Similarities of SNARC, cognitive Simon, and visuomotor Simon effects in terms of response time distributions, hand-stimulus proximity, and temporal dynamics
Auteurs
Lizhu Yan
Yilin Ma
Weibin Yang
Xinrui Xiang
Weizhi Nan
Publicatiedatum
18-08-2023
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2024
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01866-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2024

Psychological Research 2/2024 Naar de uitgave