Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

20-03-2020 | Original Article

Phonological encoding is free from orthographic influence: evidence from a picture variant of the phonological Stroop task

Auteurs: Sachiko Kinoshita, Rinus G. Verdonschot

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The phonological Stroop task, in which the participant names the color of written distractors, is being used increasingly to study the phonological encoding process in speech production. A brief review of experimental paradigms used to study the phonological encoding process indicated that currently it is not known whether the onset overlap benefit (faster color naming when the distractor shares the onset segment with the color name) in a phonological Stroop task is due to phonology or orthography. The present paper investigated this question using a picture variant of the phonological Stroop task. Participants named a small set of line drawings of animals (e.g., camel) with a pseudoword distractor printed on it. Picture naming was facilitated when the distractor shared the onset segment with the picture name regardless of orthographic overlap (CUST–camel = KUST–camel < NUST–camel). We conclude that the picture variant of the phonological Stroop task is a useful tool to study the phonological encoding process, free of orthographic influence.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Starreveld and La Heij’s (2017) paper was in direct opposition to Dell’Aqua, Job, Peressotti and Pascali (2007) who titled their paper “The picture–word interference effect is not a Stroop effect”. In brief, Starreveld and La Heij noted that the results observed by Dell’Aqua et al. and taken as evidence for the dissociation between the two tasks have not been replicated in two later studies, and that the difference is likely to have been due to the methodological differences between the two tasks as they are standardly used. In particular, in the classic Stroop task, but not in the PWI task, only few targets selected from a single semantic category (colors) are used, and the distractors are also drawn from this category. Readers are referred to Starreveld and La Heij (2017) for further detail.
 
2
Note that the orthographic (graphemic) overlap is not the same as letter overlap. Specifically, the vowel segment in “seal” is pronounced /i:/ and orthographically represented by the grapheme “ea”, and not “e” (/ɛ/) as in SELP. We deemed SELP, SELM, and SELG (for seal) as acceptable for this reason.
 
3
This pattern of finding (CUST = KUST < NUST when naming “camel”) has since been replicated (Kinoshita & Mills, 2020). That study further found no difference between the three distractor conditions (CUST = KUST = NUST) when the response was a manual key press response and did not involve a speech response, consistent with the claim that the effect of onset overlap benefit originates in the phonological encoding process.
 
4
In contrast, picture targets used by Lupker (1982) were selected from a children’s coloring book, and the names of some of the pictures (e.g., “fire”) may have been more ambiguous, which may have contributed to the greater role of the name retrieval process.
 
5
In this context, we remind the readers that others (e.g., Geng et al., 2014; Shitova et al., 2016) have also noted a substantial reduction in picture naming latency when a small set of pictures is used repeatedly.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Alario, X. F., Perre, L., Castel, C., & Ziegler, J. C. (2007). The role of orthography in speech production revisited. Cognition, 102, 464–545.CrossRef Alario, X. F., Perre, L., Castel, C., & Ziegler, J. C. (2007). The role of orthography in speech production revisited. Cognition, 102, 464–545.CrossRef
go back to reference Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2018). Lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using 'Eigen' and S4. Version 1.1-17. Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2018). Lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using 'Eigen' and S4. Version 1.1-17.
go back to reference Bowers, J. S., Vigliocco, G., & Haan, R. (1998). Orthographic, phonological, and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1705–1719.PubMed Bowers, J. S., Vigliocco, G., & Haan, R. (1998). Orthographic, phonological, and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1705–1719.PubMed
go back to reference Coltheart, M., Woollams, A., Kinoshita, S., & Perry, C. (1999). A position-sensitive Stroop effect: Further evidence for a left-to-right component in print-to-speech conversion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 456–463.CrossRef Coltheart, M., Woollams, A., Kinoshita, S., & Perry, C. (1999). A position-sensitive Stroop effect: Further evidence for a left-to-right component in print-to-speech conversion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 456–463.CrossRef
go back to reference Damian, M., & Bowers, J. (2003). Effects of orthography on speech production in a form-preparation paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 119–132.CrossRef Damian, M., & Bowers, J. (2003). Effects of orthography on speech production in a form-preparation paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 119–132.CrossRef
go back to reference Dell’Acqua, R., Job, R., Peressotti, F., & Pascali, A. (2007). The picture–word interference effect is not a Stroop effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 717–722.CrossRef Dell’Acqua, R., Job, R., Peressotti, F., & Pascali, A. (2007). The picture–word interference effect is not a Stroop effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 717–722.CrossRef
go back to reference Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Influence of prime lexicality, frequency, and pronounceability on the masked onset priming effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1813–1837.CrossRef Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Influence of prime lexicality, frequency, and pronounceability on the masked onset priming effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1813–1837.CrossRef
go back to reference Forster, K. I., & Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form- priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory & Language, 30, 1–25.CrossRef Forster, K. I., & Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form- priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory & Language, 30, 1–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, 35, 116–124.CrossRef Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, 35, 116–124.CrossRef
go back to reference Geng, J., Schnur, T. T., & Janssen, N. (2014). Relative speed of processing affects interference in Stroop and picture–word interference paradigms: Evidence from the distractor frequency effect. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 1100–1114.CrossRef Geng, J., Schnur, T. T., & Janssen, N. (2014). Relative speed of processing affects interference in Stroop and picture–word interference paradigms: Evidence from the distractor frequency effect. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 1100–1114.CrossRef
go back to reference Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446.CrossRef Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446.CrossRef
go back to reference Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press. Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press.
go back to reference Kinoshita, S. (2000). The left-to-right nature of the masked onset priming effect in naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 133–141.CrossRef Kinoshita, S. (2000). The left-to-right nature of the masked onset priming effect in naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 133–141.CrossRef
go back to reference Kureta, Y., Fushimi, T., & Tatsumi, I. F. (2006). The functional unit in phonological encoding: Evidence for moraic representation in native Japanese speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1102–1119.PubMed Kureta, Y., Fushimi, T., & Tatsumi, I. F. (2006). The functional unit in phonological encoding: Evidence for moraic representation in native Japanese speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1102–1119.PubMed
go back to reference Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–38. Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–38.
go back to reference Lupker, S. J. (1982). The role of phonetic and orthographic similarity in picture–word interference. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 349–376.CrossRef Lupker, S. J. (1982). The role of phonetic and orthographic similarity in picture–word interference. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 349–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, A. S. (1990). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 524–545.CrossRef Meyer, A. S. (1990). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 524–545.CrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, A. S. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69–89.CrossRef Meyer, A. S. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69–89.CrossRef
go back to reference Mousikou, P., Rastle, K., Besner, D., & Coltheart, M. (2015). The locus of serial processing in reading aloud: Orthography-to-phonology computation or speech planning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 41, 1076–1099. Mousikou, P., Rastle, K., Besner, D., & Coltheart, M. (2015). The locus of serial processing in reading aloud: Orthography-to-phonology computation or speech planning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 41, 1076–1099.
go back to reference O’Seaghdha, P. G., Chen, J.-Y., & Chen, T.-M. (2010). Proximate units in word production: Phonological encoding begins with syllables in Mandarin Chinese but with segments in English. Cognition, 115, 282–302.CrossRef O’Seaghdha, P. G., Chen, J.-Y., & Chen, T.-M. (2010). Proximate units in word production: Phonological encoding begins with syllables in Mandarin Chinese but with segments in English. Cognition, 115, 282–302.CrossRef
go back to reference Qu, Q., & Damian, M. (2019). Orthographic effects in Mandarin spoken language production. Memory & Cognition, 47, 326–334.CrossRef Qu, Q., & Damian, M. (2019). Orthographic effects in Mandarin spoken language production. Memory & Cognition, 47, 326–334.CrossRef
go back to reference Roelofs, A. (2004). Seriality of phonological encoding in naming objects and reading their names. Memory & Cognition, 32, 212–222.CrossRef Roelofs, A. (2004). Seriality of phonological encoding in naming objects and reading their names. Memory & Cognition, 32, 212–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Roelofs, A. (2006). The influence of spelling on phonological encoding in word reading, object naming, and word generation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 33–37.CrossRef Roelofs, A. (2006). The influence of spelling on phonological encoding in word reading, object naming, and word generation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 33–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Schiller, N. O. (2004). The onset effect in word naming. Journal of Memory & Language, 50, 477–490.CrossRef Schiller, N. O. (2004). The onset effect in word naming. Journal of Memory & Language, 50, 477–490.CrossRef
go back to reference Schiller, N. O. (2007). Phonology and orthography in reading aloud. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 460–465.CrossRef Schiller, N. O. (2007). Phonology and orthography in reading aloud. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 460–465.CrossRef
go back to reference Schiller, N. O. (2008). The masked onset priming effect in picture naming. Cognition, 106, 952–962.CrossRef Schiller, N. O. (2008). The masked onset priming effect in picture naming. Cognition, 106, 952–962.CrossRef
go back to reference Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174–215. Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174–215.
go back to reference Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W. (2017). Picture-word interference is a Stroop effect: A theoretical analysis and new empirical findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 721–733.CrossRef Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W. (2017). Picture-word interference is a Stroop effect: A theoretical analysis and new empirical findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 721–733.CrossRef
go back to reference Verdonschot, R. G., & Kinoshita, S. (2018). Mora or more? The phonological unit of Japanese word production in the Stroop color naming task. Memory & Cognition, 46, 410–425.CrossRef Verdonschot, R. G., & Kinoshita, S. (2018). Mora or more? The phonological unit of Japanese word production in the Stroop color naming task. Memory & Cognition, 46, 410–425.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Phonological encoding is free from orthographic influence: evidence from a picture variant of the phonological Stroop task
Auteurs
Sachiko Kinoshita
Rinus G. Verdonschot
Publicatiedatum
20-03-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01315-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2021

Psychological Research 3/2021 Naar de uitgave