Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2021

27-09-2019 | Original Article

Working memory affects anticipatory behavior during implicit pattern learning

Auteurs: Srdan Medimorec, Petar Milin, Dagmar Divjak

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We investigated the relation between implicit sequence learning and individual differences in working memory (WM) capacity. Participants performed an oculomotor version of the serial reaction time (SRT) task and three computerized WM tasks. Implicit learning was measured using anticipation measures only, as they represent strong indicators of learning. Our results demonstrate that anticipatory behavior in the SRT task changes as a function of WM capacity, such that it increases with decreased WM capacity. On the other hand, WM capacity did not affect the overall number of correct anticipations in the task. In addition, we report a positive relation between WM capacity and the number of consecutive correct anticipations (or chunks), and a negative relation between WM capacity and the overall number of errors, indicating different learning strategies during implicit sequence learning. The results of the current study are theoretically important, because they demonstrate that individual differences in WM capacity could account for differences in learning processes, and ultimately change individuals’ anticipatory behavior, even when learning is implicit, without intention and awareness.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
More complex probabilistic sequences could likewise increase task reliability (Stark-Inbar, Raza, Taylor, & Ivry, 2016; West, Vadillo, Shanks, & Hulme, 2018).
 
2
Note that including a set of additional predictors (i.e., age, gender, and education) did not improve the models (for more details and an additional control model containing a measure of executive function using a subset of participants, see Supplementary Material, Tables S3 and S4).
 
3
Because our sample size was relatively small and the size of the effects of interest could not be inferred from previous research, we addressed the potential power and parametrization bias issues by conducting a corresponding set of analyses using Bayesian estimation. Note that the estimates from the frequentist and Bayesian models were largely comparable. Where there are differences in statistical significance of the estimates, we signal them in text. Further details about Bayesian analyses are provided in the Supplementary Material. For the full model frequentist and Bayesian estimate comparisons, see Tables S5–S11.
 
4
In the no-interaction model, there was a marginally significant effect of Block, χ2 (1) = 3.80, p = 0.051, such that correct anticipations increased in Block 6. Using Bayesian approach, the effect of Block was statistically significant (as indicated by the 95% credible interval), estimate = 0.23, credible interval  = [0.001, 0.45].
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I. C. (2012). Statistical learning is related to reading ability in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 36, 286–304.PubMed Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I. C. (2012). Statistical learning is related to reading ability in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 36, 286–304.PubMed
go back to reference Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48.
go back to reference Bo, J., Borza, V., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Age-related declines in visuospatial working memory correlate with deficits in explicit motor sequence learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 2744–2754.PubMedPubMedCentral Bo, J., Borza, V., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Age-related declines in visuospatial working memory correlate with deficits in explicit motor sequence learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 2744–2754.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Bo, J., Jennett, S., & Seidler, R. (2012). Differential working memory correlates for implicit sequence performance in young and older adults. Experimental Brain Research, 221, 467–477.PubMed Bo, J., Jennett, S., & Seidler, R. (2012). Differential working memory correlates for implicit sequence performance in young and older adults. Experimental Brain Research, 221, 467–477.PubMed
go back to reference Bo, J., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Visuospatial working memory capacity predicts the organization of acquired explicit motor sequences. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 3116–3125.PubMedPubMedCentral Bo, J., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Visuospatial working memory capacity predicts the organization of acquired explicit motor sequences. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 3116–3125.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Caljouw, S. R., Veldkamp, R., & Lamoth, C. J. C. (2016). Implicit and explicit learning of a sequential postural weight-shifting task in young and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 733.PubMedPubMedCentral Caljouw, S. R., Veldkamp, R., & Lamoth, C. J. C. (2016). Implicit and explicit learning of a sequential postural weight-shifting task in young and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 733.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235–253. Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235–253.
go back to reference Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114, 356–371.PubMed Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114, 356–371.PubMed
go back to reference Dale, R., Duran, N. D., & Morehead, J. R. (2012). Prediction during statistical learning, and implications for the implicit/explicit divide. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 196–209.PubMedPubMedCentral Dale, R., Duran, N. D., & Morehead, J. R. (2012). Prediction during statistical learning, and implications for the implicit/explicit divide. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 196–209.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Daltrozzo, J., Emerson, S. N., Deocampo, J., Singh, S., Freggens, M., Branum-Martin, L., & Conway, C. M. (2017). Visual statistical learning is related to natural language ability in adults: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 166, 40–51.PubMedPubMedCentral Daltrozzo, J., Emerson, S. N., Deocampo, J., Singh, S., Freggens, M., Branum-Martin, L., & Conway, C. M. (2017). Visual statistical learning is related to natural language ability in adults: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 166, 40–51.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition, 48, 71–99.PubMed Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition, 48, 71–99.PubMed
go back to reference Erickson, L. C., & Thiessen, E. D. (2015). Statistical learning of language: Theory, validity, and predictions of a statistical learning account of language acquisition. Developmental Review, 37, 66–108. Erickson, L. C., & Thiessen, E. D. (2015). Statistical learning of language: Theory, validity, and predictions of a statistical learning account of language acquisition. Developmental Review, 37, 66–108.
go back to reference Feldman, J., Kerr, B., & Streissguth, A. P. (1995). Correlational analyses of procedural and declarative learning performance. Intelligence, 20, 87–114. Feldman, J., Kerr, B., & Streissguth, A. P. (1995). Correlational analyses of procedural and declarative learning performance. Intelligence, 20, 87–114.
go back to reference Fischer, H., & Holt, D. V. (2017). When high working memory capacity is and is not beneficial for predicting nonlinear processes. Memory and Cognition, 45, 404–412.PubMed Fischer, H., & Holt, D. V. (2017). When high working memory capacity is and is not beneficial for predicting nonlinear processes. Memory and Cognition, 45, 404–412.PubMed
go back to reference Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
go back to reference Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95–110.PubMed Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95–110.PubMed
go back to reference Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Stefaniak, N., Lejeune, C., Desmottes, L., & Meulemans, T. (2013). Procedural learning in specific language impairment: Effects of sequence complexity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 264–271.PubMed Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Stefaniak, N., Lejeune, C., Desmottes, L., & Meulemans, T. (2013). Procedural learning in specific language impairment: Effects of sequence complexity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 264–271.PubMed
go back to reference Galea, J. M., Albert, N. B., Ditye, T., & Miall, R. C. (2010). Disruption of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates the consolidation of procedural skills. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1158–1164.PubMedPubMedCentral Galea, J. M., Albert, N. B., Ditye, T., & Miall, R. C. (2010). Disruption of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates the consolidation of procedural skills. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1158–1164.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Guzmán, F. J. (2018). The influence of personality and working memory capacity on implicit learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 2603–2614. Guzmán, F. J. (2018). The influence of personality and working memory capacity on implicit learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 2603–2614.
go back to reference Harrison, T. L., Shipstead, Z., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Why is working memory capacity related to matrix reasoning tasks? Memory and Cognition, 43, 389–396.PubMed Harrison, T. L., Shipstead, Z., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Why is working memory capacity related to matrix reasoning tasks? Memory and Cognition, 43, 389–396.PubMed
go back to reference Hasson, U., Cashdollar, N., Weisz, N., & Ruhnau, P. (2016). The role of working memory in the probabilistic inference of future sensory events. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 2955–2969. Hasson, U., Cashdollar, N., Weisz, N., & Ruhnau, P. (2016). The role of working memory in the probabilistic inference of future sensory events. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 2955–2969.
go back to reference Hunt, R. H., & Aslin, R. N. (2001). Statistical learning in a serial reaction time task: Access to separable statistical cues by individual learners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 658–680. Hunt, R. H., & Aslin, R. N. (2001). Statistical learning in a serial reaction time task: Access to separable statistical cues by individual learners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 658–680.
go back to reference Janacsek, K., & Nemeth, D. (2013). Implicit sequence learning and working memory: Correlated or complicated? Cortex, 49, 2001–2006.PubMed Janacsek, K., & Nemeth, D. (2013). Implicit sequence learning and working memory: Correlated or complicated? Cortex, 49, 2001–2006.PubMed
go back to reference Janacsek, K., & Nemeth, D. (2015). The puzzle is complicated: When should working memory be related to implicit sequence learning, and when should it not? (Response to Martini et al.). Cortex, 64, 411–412.PubMed Janacsek, K., & Nemeth, D. (2015). The puzzle is complicated: When should working memory be related to implicit sequence learning, and when should it not? (Response to Martini et al.). Cortex, 64, 411–412.PubMed
go back to reference Jimenez, L., & Vazquez, G. A. (2005). Sequence learning under dual-task conditions: Alternatives to a resource-based account. Psychological Research, 69, 352–368.PubMed Jimenez, L., & Vazquez, G. A. (2005). Sequence learning under dual-task conditions: Alternatives to a resource-based account. Psychological Research, 69, 352–368.PubMed
go back to reference Jongbloed-Pereboom, M., Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W. G., & Steenbergen, B. (2019). Explicit and implicit motor sequence learning in children and adults; The role of age and visual working memory. Human Movement Science, 64, 1–11.PubMed Jongbloed-Pereboom, M., Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W. G., & Steenbergen, B. (2019). Explicit and implicit motor sequence learning in children and adults; The role of age and visual working memory. Human Movement Science, 64, 1–11.PubMed
go back to reference Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience sampling study of working memory and executive control in everyday life. Psychological Science, 18, 614–621.PubMed Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience sampling study of working memory and executive control in everyday life. Psychological Science, 18, 614–621.PubMed
go back to reference Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116, 321–340.PubMed Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116, 321–340.PubMed
go back to reference Kennerley, S. W., Sakai, K., & Rushworth, M. F. (2004). Organization of action sequences and the role of the pre-SMA. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 978–993.PubMed Kennerley, S. W., Sakai, K., & Rushworth, M. F. (2004). Organization of action sequences and the role of the pre-SMA. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 978–993.PubMed
go back to reference Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48, 171–184.PubMed Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48, 171–184.PubMed
go back to reference Kidd, E., & Arciuli, J. (2016). Individual differences in statistical learning predict children’s comprehension of syntax. Child Development, 87, 184–193.PubMed Kidd, E., & Arciuli, J. (2016). Individual differences in statistical learning predict children’s comprehension of syntax. Child Development, 87, 184–193.PubMed
go back to reference Kinder, A., Rolfs, M., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Sequence learning at optimal stimulus–response mapping: Evidence from a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 203–209.PubMed Kinder, A., Rolfs, M., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Sequence learning at optimal stimulus–response mapping: Evidence from a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 203–209.PubMed
go back to reference Marcus, D. J., Karatekin, C., & Markiewicz, S. (2006). Oculomotor evidence of sequence learning on the serial reaction time task. Memory and Cognition, 34, 420–432.PubMed Marcus, D. J., Karatekin, C., & Markiewicz, S. (2006). Oculomotor evidence of sequence learning on the serial reaction time task. Memory and Cognition, 34, 420–432.PubMed
go back to reference Martini, M., Furtner, M. R., & Sachse, P. (2013). Working memory and its relation to deterministic sequence learning. PLoS One, 8, e56166.PubMedPubMedCentral Martini, M., Furtner, M. R., & Sachse, P. (2013). Working memory and its relation to deterministic sequence learning. PLoS One, 8, e56166.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Masters, R. S. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how. The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343–358. Masters, R. S. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how. The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343–358.
go back to reference Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 314–324.PubMed Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 314–324.PubMed
go back to reference Medimorec, S., Mander, C., & Risko, E. F. (2018). Memory demands in linguistic compensation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 1234–1239.PubMed Medimorec, S., Mander, C., & Risko, E. F. (2018). Memory demands in linguistic compensation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 1234–1239.PubMed
go back to reference Meissner, S. N., Keitel, A., Südmeyer, M., & Pollok, B. (2016). Implicit motor sequence learning and working memory performance changes across the adult life span. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 89. Meissner, S. N., Keitel, A., Südmeyer, M., & Pollok, B. (2016). Implicit motor sequence learning and working memory performance changes across the adult life span. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 89.
go back to reference Milin, P., Divjak, D., & Baayen, R. H. (2017). A learning perspective on individual differences in skilled reading: Exploring and exploiting orthographic and semantic discrimination cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 1730–1751.PubMed Milin, P., Divjak, D., & Baayen, R. H. (2017). A learning perspective on individual differences in skilled reading: Exploring and exploiting orthographic and semantic discrimination cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 1730–1751.PubMed
go back to reference Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2012). Statistical learning and language: An individual differences study. Language Learning, 62, 302–331. Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2012). Statistical learning and language: An individual differences study. Language Learning, 62, 302–331.
go back to reference Nemeth, D., Janacsek, K., Polner, B., & Kovacs, Z. A. (2013). Boosting human learning by hypnosis. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 801–805.PubMed Nemeth, D., Janacsek, K., Polner, B., & Kovacs, Z. A. (2013). Boosting human learning by hypnosis. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 801–805.PubMed
go back to reference Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28. Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28.
go back to reference Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32. Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.
go back to reference Sakai, K., Kitaguchi, K., & Hikosaka, O. (2003). Chunking during human visuomotor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 152, 229–242.PubMed Sakai, K., Kitaguchi, K., & Hikosaka, O. (2003). Chunking during human visuomotor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 152, 229–242.PubMed
go back to reference Schvaneveldt, R. R., & Gomez, R. L. (1998). Attention and probabilistic sequence learning. Psychological Research, 61, 175–190. Schvaneveldt, R. R., & Gomez, R. L. (1998). Attention and probabilistic sequence learning. Psychological Research, 61, 175–190.
go back to reference Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2012). Generalized lessons about sequence learning from the study of the serial reaction time task. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 165–178.PubMedPubMedCentral Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2012). Generalized lessons about sequence learning from the study of the serial reaction time task. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 165–178.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Shafto, C. L., Conway, C. M., Field, S. L., & Houston, D. M. (2012). Visual sequence learning in infancy: Domain-general and domain-specific associations with language. Infancy, 17, 247–271. Shafto, C. L., Conway, C. M., Field, S. L., & Houston, D. M. (2012). Visual sequence learning in infancy: Domain-general and domain-specific associations with language. Infancy, 17, 247–271.
go back to reference Shea, C. H., Park, J. H., & Braden, H. W. (2006). Age-related effects in sequential motor learning. Physical Therapy, 86, 478–488.PubMed Shea, C. H., Park, J. H., & Braden, H. W. (2006). Age-related effects in sequential motor learning. Physical Therapy, 86, 478–488.PubMed
go back to reference Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1061–1069.PubMed Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1061–1069.PubMed
go back to reference Stadler, M. A., & Frensch, P. A. (1998). Handbook of implicit learning. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Stadler, M. A., & Frensch, P. A. (1998). Handbook of implicit learning. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Stafford, T., & Dewar, M. (2014). Tracing the trajectory of skill learning with a very large sample of online game players. Psychological Science, 25, 511–518.PubMed Stafford, T., & Dewar, M. (2014). Tracing the trajectory of skill learning with a very large sample of online game players. Psychological Science, 25, 511–518.PubMed
go back to reference Stark-Inbar, A., Raza, M., Taylor, J. A., & Ivry, R. B. (2016). Individual differences in implicit motor learning: Task specificity in sensorimotor adaptation and sequence learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 117, 412–428.PubMedPubMedCentral Stark-Inbar, A., Raza, M., Taylor, J. A., & Ivry, R. B. (2016). Individual differences in implicit motor learning: Task specificity in sensorimotor adaptation and sequence learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 117, 412–428.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Torriero, S., Oliveri, M., Koch, G., Gerfo, E. L., Salerno, S., Petrosini, L., & Caltagirone, C. (2007). Cortical networks of procedural learning: Evidence from cerebellar damage. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1208–1214.PubMed Torriero, S., Oliveri, M., Koch, G., Gerfo, E. L., Salerno, S., Petrosini, L., & Caltagirone, C. (2007). Cortical networks of procedural learning: Evidence from cerebellar damage. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1208–1214.PubMed
go back to reference Tóth, B., Janacsek, K., Takács, Á., Kóbor, A., Zavecz, Z., & Nemeth, D. (2017). Dynamics of EEG functional connectivity during statistical learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 144, 216–229.PubMed Tóth, B., Janacsek, K., Takács, Á., Kóbor, A., Zavecz, Z., & Nemeth, D. (2017). Dynamics of EEG functional connectivity during statistical learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 144, 216–229.PubMed
go back to reference Turk-Browne, N. B., Scholl, B. J., Chun, M. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2008). Neural evidence of statistical learning: Efficient detection of visual regularities without awareness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1934–1945. Turk-Browne, N. B., Scholl, B. J., Chun, M. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2008). Neural evidence of statistical learning: Efficient detection of visual regularities without awareness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1934–1945.
go back to reference Unsworth, N. (2017). Examining the dynamics of strategic search from long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 135–153. Unsworth, N. (2017). Examining the dynamics of strategic search from long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 135–153.
go back to reference Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Individual differences in working memory capacity and learning: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Memory and Cognition, 33, 213–220.PubMed Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Individual differences in working memory capacity and learning: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Memory and Cognition, 33, 213–220.PubMed
go back to reference Vakil, E., Bloch, A., & Cohen, H. (2017). Anticipation measures of sequence learning: Manual versus coulometer versions of the serial reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 579–589. Vakil, E., Bloch, A., & Cohen, H. (2017). Anticipation measures of sequence learning: Manual versus coulometer versions of the serial reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 579–589.
go back to reference Virag, M., Janacsek, K., Horvath, A., Bujdoso, Z., Fabo, D., & Nemeth, D. (2015). Competition between frontal lobe functions and implicit sequence learning: Evidence from the long-term effects of alcohol. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 2081–2089.PubMed Virag, M., Janacsek, K., Horvath, A., Bujdoso, Z., Fabo, D., & Nemeth, D. (2015). Competition between frontal lobe functions and implicit sequence learning: Evidence from the long-term effects of alcohol. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 2081–2089.PubMed
go back to reference von Bastian, C. C., Locher, A., & Ruflin, M. (2013). Tatool: A java-based open-source programming framework for psychological studies. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 108–115. von Bastian, C. C., Locher, A., & Ruflin, M. (2013). Tatool: A java-based open-source programming framework for psychological studies. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 108–115.
go back to reference Weitz, D., O’Shea, G., Zook, N., & Needham, W. (2011). Working memory and sequence learning in the Hebb digits task: Awareness is predicted by individual differences in operation span. The American Journal of Psychology, 124, 49–62.PubMed Weitz, D., O’Shea, G., Zook, N., & Needham, W. (2011). Working memory and sequence learning in the Hebb digits task: Awareness is predicted by individual differences in operation span. The American Journal of Psychology, 124, 49–62.PubMed
go back to reference West, G., Vadillo, M. A., Shanks, D. R., & Hulme, C. (2018). The procedural learning deficit hypothesis of language learning disorders: We see some problems. Developmental Science, 21, e12552. West, G., Vadillo, M. A., Shanks, D. R., & Hulme, C. (2018). The procedural learning deficit hypothesis of language learning disorders: We see some problems. Developmental Science, 21, e12552.
go back to reference Wilkinson, L., & Shanks, D. R. (2004). Intentional control and implicit sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 354–369.PubMed Wilkinson, L., & Shanks, D. R. (2004). Intentional control and implicit sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 354–369.PubMed
go back to reference Wymbs, N. F., Bassett, D. S., Mucha, P. J., Porter, M. A., & Grafton, S. T. (2012). Differential recruitment of the sensorimotor putamen and frontoparietal cortex during motor chunking in humans. Neuron, 74, 936–946.PubMedPubMedCentral Wymbs, N. F., Bassett, D. S., Mucha, P. J., Porter, M. A., & Grafton, S. T. (2012). Differential recruitment of the sensorimotor putamen and frontoparietal cortex during motor chunking in humans. Neuron, 74, 936–946.PubMedPubMedCentral
Metagegevens
Titel
Working memory affects anticipatory behavior during implicit pattern learning
Auteurs
Srdan Medimorec
Petar Milin
Dagmar Divjak
Publicatiedatum
27-09-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01251-w

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2021

Psychological Research 1/2021 Naar de uitgave