Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2021

25-10-2019 | Original Article

Gaze interaction: anticipation-based control of the gaze of others

Auteurs: Eva Riechelmann, Tim Raettig, Anne Böckler, Lynn Huestegge

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Gaze control is an important component of social communication, e.g. to direct someone’s attention. While previous research on gaze interaction has mainly focused on the gaze recipient by asking how humans respond to perceived gaze (gaze cueing), we address the actor’s point of view by asking how actors control their own eye movements to trigger a gaze response in others. Specifically, we investigate whether gaze responses of a (virtual) interaction partner are anticipated and thereby affect oculomotor control. Building on a pre-established paradigm for addressing anticipation-based motor control in non-social contexts, participants were instructed to alternately look at two faces on the screen, which consistently responded to the participant’s gaze with either direct or averted gaze. We tested whether this gaze response of the targeted face is already anticipated prior to the participant’s eye movement by displaying a task-irrelevant visual stimulus (prior to the execution of the target saccade), which was either congruent, incongruent, or unrelated to the subsequently perceived gaze. In addition to schematic and photographic faces, we included conditions involving changes in non-social objects. Overall, we observed congruency effects (as an indicator of anticipation of the virtual other’s gaze response to one’s own gaze) for both social and non-social stimuli, but only when the perceived changes were sufficiently salient. Temporal dynamics of the congruency effects were comparable for social and non-social stimuli, suggesting that similar mechanisms underlie anticipation-based oculomotor control. The results support recent theoretical claims emphasizing the role of anticipation-based action control in social interaction.
Voetnoten
1
A stronger, more natural form of controlling another's gaze with our own gaze would probably be a setting in which a participant freely chooses to move his/her eyes (instead of being instructed by means of an auditory imperative stimulus) in order to guide the gaze of another person into a certain direction or to a certain object. However, the present study was based on a previous established paradigm and designed to address anticipation-based oculomotor control under maximally controlled conditions.
 
2
Note that we recruited 105 participants, but excluded data of four participants with an unusual high error rate (> 30% in at least one cell of the design). Additionally, the data of one participant was removed from analysis due to an eye tracking error.
 
3
For the sake of completeness, we also report the respective analyses for Experiments 1, 3 and 5. In Experiment 1 (upright face stimuli), neither the effect of experiment half, F(1, 19) = 2.57, p = 0.125, ƞ p 2  = 0.12, nor the three-way interaction of congruency, SOA, and experiment half, F(2, 38) = 1.28, p = 0.290, ƞ p 2  = 0.06, or any other relevant interaction revealed significant results (all Fs < 1). In Experiment 3 (inverted face stimuli) participants responded faster in the first versus second half of the experiment, F(1, 19) = 11.77, p = 0.003, ƞ p 2  = 0.38. None of the other relevant interactions were significant, neither the interaction of congruency and experiment half, F(2, 38) = 1.85, p = 0.172, ƞ p 2  = 0.08, nor the three-way interaction of congruency, direction, and experiment half, F(2, 38) = 1.66, p = 0.204, ƞ p 2  = 0.08, or the interaction of congruency, SOA, and experiment half, F(2, 38) = 2.36, p = 0.108, ƞ p 2  = 0.11. The four-way interaction was not significant, F < 1. In Experiment 5 (scrambled face stimuli) no effect of experiment half was present, F(1, 19) = 2.30, p = 0.146, ƞ p 2  = 0.11. The four-way interaction was significant, F(2, 38) = 3.54, p = 0.039, ƞ p 2  = 0.16. None of the remaining relevant interaction effects was significant (all Fs < 1, except for the three-way interaction of congruency, SOA, and experiment half, F(2, 38) = 2.96, p = 0.064, ƞ p 2  = 0.14).
 
4
Please note that none of the statistical comparisons revealed significant results (all Fs < 1) when conducting a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors task-irrelevant stimulus (direct gaze vs. averted gaze direction in Experiments 1 and 3; central vs. lateral effect direction in Experiment 5) and congruency (congruent vs. incongruent) separately for Experiments 1, 3, and 5.
 
5
Note that we only report statistical comparisons involving the factor experiment for the between-experiment comparisons.
 
6
We additionally addressed our claim that overall saliency of the gaze is reduced for photographic versus non-photographic stimuli empirically: In two short additional experiments, we compared the detection rate of direct vs. averted (left/right) gaze for photographic (upright and inverted) face stimuli to the detection rate of direct vs. averted (left/right) gaze for schematic face and central vs. lateral effects for abstract stimuli using the stimulus material from the current study. Participants either saw a stimulus presented for 35 ms at the screen center followed by a random pattern mask (scrambled version of the preceding stimulus) (Experiment A), or saw a printout of the stimuli (stimulus width × height: 2.0 cm × 2.6 cm) at a fixed viewing distance that was large enough to prevent perfect stimulus classification (3.35 m, Experiment B). Participants had to indicate the (gaze) orientation of the presented stimulus (left vs. direct/central vs. right, using randomized stimulus placement). The results confirmed our hypothesis that (gaze) orientation saliency is higher for non-photographic stimuli compared to photographic stimuli, as detection rate was higher for the abstract and schematic face stimuli compared to the photographic upright and inverted stimuli, t(19) = 6.85, p < 0.001, d = 1.53 (in Experiment A), and t(19) = 6.51, p < 0.001, d = 1.46 (in Experiment B).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A. B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action-effect associations: Automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action-effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(6), 1701–1712. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000022.CrossRefPubMed Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A. B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action-effect associations: Automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action-effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(6), 1701–1712. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​xlm0000022.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 661–674.CrossRef Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 661–674.CrossRef
go back to reference Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens [The apparatus of the will]. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73. Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens [The apparatus of the will]. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73.
go back to reference Hayward, D. A., Voorhies, W., Morris, J. L., Capozzi, F., & Ristic, J. (2017). Staring reality in the face: A comparison of social attention across laboratory and real world measures suggests little common ground. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(3), 212–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000117.CrossRefPubMed Hayward, D. A., Voorhies, W., Morris, J. L., Capozzi, F., & Ristic, J. (2017). Staring reality in the face: A comparison of social attention across laboratory and real world measures suggests little common ground. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(3), 212–225. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​cep0000117.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heider, F. (1977). Psychologie der interpersonalen Beziehungen [The psychology of interpersonal relations] (G. Deffner, Trans.). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett. (Original work published 1958) Heider, F. (1977). Psychologie der interpersonalen Beziehungen [The psychology of interpersonal relations] (G. Deffner, Trans.). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett. (Original work published 1958)
go back to reference Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik, und Mathematik. [Psychology as a science newly founded on experience, metaphysicy, and mathematics]. Königsberg: Unzer. Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik, und Mathematik. [Psychology as a science newly founded on experience, metaphysicy, and mathematics]. Königsberg: Unzer.
go back to reference James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.
go back to reference Sprague, N., & Ballard, D. (2003). Eye movements for reward maximization. In S. Thrun, L. K. Saul, & B. Scholkopf (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (pp. 1467–1474). Boston, MA: MIT Press. Sprague, N., & Ballard, D. (2003). Eye movements for reward maximization. In S. Thrun, L. K. Saul, & B. Scholkopf (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (pp. 1467–1474). Boston, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Tipples, J. (2002). Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 314–318.CrossRef Tipples, J. (2002). Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 314–318.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Gaze interaction: anticipation-based control of the gaze of others
Auteurs
Eva Riechelmann
Tim Raettig
Anne Böckler
Lynn Huestegge
Publicatiedatum
25-10-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01257-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2021

Psychological Research 1/2021 Naar de uitgave