Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2019

22-07-2017 | Original Article

Binding abstract concepts

Auteurs: Tarini Singh, Christian Frings, Birte Moeller

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Binding theories assume that a stimulus and the response made to it are bound together in an event file (Hommel et al., Behav Brain Sci 24(05):849–937, 2001). Such bindings can occur even after single encounters. If the stimulus or parts of its features are repeated within the time frame in which the event file is still intact, the previously integrated response is retrieved. Stimulus–response binding can exist at a perceptual, conceptual or a response selection level (Henson et al., Trends Cogn Sci 18(7):376–384, 2014). The current experiments test whether the observed binding of concepts with responses can be extended from concrete to abstract concepts (detailedness) and whether abstract concepts can retrieve the previous response, in the absence of perceptual repetition. In the present experiment participants responded to a target feature (colour) while the detailedness of the stimulus was irrelevant to the task. The results showed a significant interaction of response relation and detailedness relation, even in the absence of perceptual repetition. This interaction is interpreted as evidence for response-retrieval due to abstract concept repetition. Thus, our data suggest a broader impact of binding mechanism on performance as even abstract concepts can be integrated into event-files and later modulate behaviour.
Literatuur
go back to reference Denkinger, B., & Koutstaal, W. (2009). Perceive-decide-act, perceive-decide-act: How abstract is repetition-related decision learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 742–756.CrossRefPubMed Denkinger, B., & Koutstaal, W. (2009). Perceive-decide-act, perceive-decide-act: How abstract is repetition-related decision learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 742–756.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.CrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Fletcher, B. C., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1978). The changing pattern of perceptual analytic strategies and response selection with practice in a two-choice reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30(3), 417–427.CrossRefPubMed Fletcher, B. C., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1978). The changing pattern of perceptual analytic strategies and response selection with practice in a two-choice reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30(3), 417–427.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frings, C., Moeller, B., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Retrieval of event files can be conceptually mediated. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 75(4), 700–709.CrossRefPubMed Frings, C., Moeller, B., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Retrieval of event files can be conceptually mediated. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 75(4), 700–709.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). To be or not to be … included in an event file: Integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus–response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5), 1209–1227.CrossRefPubMed Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). To be or not to be … included in an event file: Integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus–response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5), 1209–1227.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frings, C., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2007). Distractor repetitions retrieve previous responses to targets. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(10), 1367–1377.CrossRefPubMed Frings, C., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2007). Distractor repetitions retrieve previous responses to targets. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(10), 1367–1377.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus–response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 376–384.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus–response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 376–384.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Holmes, V. M., & Langford, J. (1976). Comprehension and recall of abstract and concrete sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 559–566.CrossRef Holmes, V. M., & Langford, J. (1976). Comprehension and recall of abstract and concrete sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 559–566.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1–2), 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1–2), 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 494–500.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 494–500.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11(4), 483–521.CrossRef Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11(4), 483–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(05), 849–937.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(05), 849–937.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2009). Bindings between stimuli and multiple response codes dominate long-lag repetition priming in speeded classification tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 757–779.CrossRefPubMed Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2009). Bindings between stimuli and multiple response codes dominate long-lag repetition priming in speeded classification tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 757–779.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2011). Stimulus–response bindings code both abstract and specific representations of stimuli: Evidence from a classification priming design that reverses multiple levels of response representation. Memory and Cognition, 39, 1457–1471.CrossRefPubMed Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2011). Stimulus–response bindings code both abstract and specific representations of stimuli: Evidence from a classification priming design that reverses multiple levels of response representation. Memory and Cognition, 39, 1457–1471.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kieras, D. (1978). Beyond pictures and words: Alternative information-processing models for imagery effects in verbal memory. Psychological Bulletin, 85(3), 532–554.CrossRef Kieras, D. (1978). Beyond pictures and words: Alternative information-processing models for imagery effects in verbal memory. Psychological Bulletin, 85(3), 532–554.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, B., & Buchner, A. (2006). Evidence for episodic retrieval of inadequate prime responses in auditory negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 932–943.PubMed Mayr, B., & Buchner, A. (2006). Evidence for episodic retrieval of inadequate prime responses in auditory negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 932–943.PubMed
go back to reference Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2014). Long-term response–stimulus associations can influence distractor-response bindings. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 10(2), 68–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2014). Long-term response–stimulus associations can influence distractor-response bindings. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 10(2), 68–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2017). Overlearned responses hinder SR binding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(1), 1.PubMed Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2017). Overlearned responses hinder SR binding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(1), 1.PubMed
go back to reference Neill, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 1291–1305.CrossRef Neill, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 1291–1305.CrossRef
go back to reference Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(3), 565–576.CrossRef Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(3), 565–576.CrossRef
go back to reference Nelson, D. L., & Schrieber, T. A. (1992). Word concreteness and word structure as independent determinants of recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 237–260.CrossRef Nelson, D. L., & Schrieber, T. A. (1992). Word concreteness and word structure as independent determinants of recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 237–260.CrossRef
go back to reference Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45(3), 255–287.CrossRef Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45(3), 255–287.CrossRef
go back to reference Paivio, A., Walsh, M., & Bons, T. (1994). Concreteness effects on memory: When and why? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1196–1204.CrossRef Paivio, A., Walsh, M., & Bons, T. (1994). Concreteness effects on memory: When and why? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1196–1204.CrossRef
go back to reference Plaut, D. C., & Shallice, T. (1991). Effects of word abstractness in a connectionist model of deep dyslexia. Proceedings of the 13th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 73–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Plaut, D. C., & Shallice, T. (1991). Effects of word abstractness in a connectionist model of deep dyslexia. Proceedings of the 13th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 73–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 482–495.CrossRefPubMed Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 482–495.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R., De Houwer, J., & Besener, D. (2010). Contingency learning and unlearning in the blink of an eye: A resource dependent process. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 235–250.CrossRefPubMed Schmidt, J. R., De Houwer, J., & Besener, D. (2010). Contingency learning and unlearning in the blink of an eye: A resource dependent process. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 235–250.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R., De Houwer, J., & Rotmermund, K. (2016). The parallel episodic processing (PEP) model 2.0: A single computational model of stimulus–response binding, contingency learning, power curves, and mixing costs. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 82–108.CrossRefPubMed Schmidt, J. R., De Houwer, J., & Rotmermund, K. (2016). The parallel episodic processing (PEP) model 2.0: A single computational model of stimulus–response binding, contingency learning, power curves, and mixing costs. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 82–108.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schwanenfluegel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1983). Differential context effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(1), 82–102.CrossRef Schwanenfluegel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1983). Differential context effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(1), 82–102.CrossRef
go back to reference Singh, T., Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2016). Five shades of grey: Generalization in distractor-based retrieval of SR episodes. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 78(8), 2307–2312.CrossRef Singh, T., Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2016). Five shades of grey: Generalization in distractor-based retrieval of SR episodes. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 78(8), 2307–2312.CrossRef
go back to reference Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1625–1640. Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1625–1640.
go back to reference Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
go back to reference Weimer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29, 719–736.CrossRef Weimer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29, 719–736.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Binding abstract concepts
Auteurs
Tarini Singh
Christian Frings
Birte Moeller
Publicatiedatum
22-07-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0897-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2019

Psychological Research 5/2019 Naar de uitgave