Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2010

01-05-2010 | Original Article

Between-person effects on attention and action: Joe and Fred revisited

Auteurs: Spencer J. Hayes, Steve Hansen, Digby Elliott

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Previous study indicates that target–target inhibition of return (IOR) is not restricted to a single nervous system. Specifically, watching another person perform a goal-directed aiming movement engages similar inhibitory processes on a subsequent aiming attempt as if having performed the preceding movement oneself. This between-person effect has been attributed to the mirror neuron system. In the study reported here, we replicated this finding and examined the relative importance of automatic stimulus alerting events and action–observation by dissociating these two influences. This was done by having two people alternately perform sets of two aiming trials to the same equally probable targets. Under some experimental conditions, one or both of the performers moved to a non-illuminated target. In this way, we dissociated the stimulus and observed event under some between-person conditions. Although IOR was greatest when the stimulus and observed events were compatible, both contributed to the between-person inhibitory processes slowing the responses (Experiment 1). The impact of observing another person perform an aiming movement appears to have more to do with realizing a particular spatial goal than seeing the biological motion associated with achieving that goal (Experiment 2). Findings that both the illumination of a visual target signal and the observation of another person’s action engage similar attention–action processes are consistent with action-based accounts of visual selective attention.
Literatuur
go back to reference Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.PubMed Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.PubMed
go back to reference Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180. Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180.
go back to reference Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.CrossRefPubMed Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rossi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.CrossRefPubMed Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rossi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grierson, L. E. M., Welsh, T. N., Hansen, S., Hodges, N. J., Hayes, S., Lyons, J. L., et al. (2008). The response activation model and cross-modal facilitation and inhibition of return: A trajectory analysis. The Open Psychology Journal, 1, 35–41.CrossRef Grierson, L. E. M., Welsh, T. N., Hansen, S., Hodges, N. J., Hayes, S., Lyons, J. L., et al. (2008). The response activation model and cross-modal facilitation and inhibition of return: A trajectory analysis. The Open Psychology Journal, 1, 35–41.CrossRef
go back to reference Hansen, S., & Elliott, D. (2005). Nomadic inhibition of attention and motor responses. Human Movement Science, 24, 744–759.CrossRefPubMed Hansen, S., & Elliott, D. (2005). Nomadic inhibition of attention and motor responses. Human Movement Science, 24, 744–759.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hansen, S., Lyons, J. L., & Keetch, K. M. (2006). Attentional and motor response priming in a bimanual task. Motor Control, 10, 290–299. Hansen, S., Lyons, J. L., & Keetch, K. M. (2006). Attentional and motor response priming in a bimanual task. Motor Control, 10, 290–299.
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Ashford, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2008). Goal-directed imitation: The means to an end. Acta Psychologica, 127, 407–415.CrossRefPubMed Hayes, S. J., Ashford, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2008). Goal-directed imitation: The means to an end. Acta Psychologica, 127, 407–415.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.CrossRefPubMed Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13, 522–525.CrossRefPubMed Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13, 522–525.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klein, R. M., & MacInnes, J. (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10, 346–352.CrossRef Klein, R. M., & MacInnes, J. (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10, 346–352.CrossRef
go back to reference Lyons, J., Glazebrook, C. M., Keetch, K. M., Dhillon, V., & Elliott, D. (2006). Influence of endogenous and exogenous orientations of attention on inhibition of return in a cross-modal target-target aiming task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 219–228.CrossRefPubMed Lyons, J., Glazebrook, C. M., Keetch, K. M., Dhillon, V., & Elliott, D. (2006). Influence of endogenous and exogenous orientations of attention on inhibition of return in a cross-modal target-target aiming task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 219–228.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Maylor, E. A., & Hockey, R. (1985). Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 777–787.CrossRefPubMed Maylor, E. A., & Hockey, R. (1985). Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 777–787.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Milgram, P. (1987). A spectacle-mounted liquid-crystal tachistoscope. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 19, 449–456. Milgram, P. (1987). A spectacle-mounted liquid-crystal tachistoscope. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 19, 449–456.
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–554). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–554). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., et al. (1996). Localisation of grasp representations in humans by PET—1: Observation vs execution. Experimental Brain Research, 111, 246–252.CrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., et al. (1996). Localisation of grasp representations in humans by PET—1: Observation vs execution. Experimental Brain Research, 111, 246–252.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B. M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 231–265). Cambridge: MIT Press. Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B. M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 231–265). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., & Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: Evidence for action-centred attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 891–905.CrossRefPubMed Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., & Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: Evidence for action-centred attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 891–905.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tremblay, L., Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2005). Between-trial inhibition and facilitation in goal-directed aiming: Manual and spatial asymmetries. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 79–88.CrossRef Tremblay, L., Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2005). Between-trial inhibition and facilitation in goal-directed aiming: Manual and spatial asymmetries. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 79–88.CrossRef
go back to reference van Schie, H. T., Koelewijn, T., Jensen, O., Oostenveld, R., Maris, E., & Bekkering, H. (2008a). Evidence for fast, low-level motor resonance at action observation. Social Neuroscience, 3, 213–228.CrossRefPubMed van Schie, H. T., Koelewijn, T., Jensen, O., Oostenveld, R., Maris, E., & Bekkering, H. (2008a). Evidence for fast, low-level motor resonance at action observation. Social Neuroscience, 3, 213–228.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van Schie, H. T., van Waterschoot, B. M., & Bekkering, H. (2008b). Understanding action beyond imitation: Reversed compatibility effects of action observation in imitation and joint action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1493–1500.CrossRefPubMed van Schie, H. T., van Waterschoot, B. M., & Bekkering, H. (2008b). Understanding action beyond imitation: Reversed compatibility effects of action observation in imitation and joint action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1493–1500.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2004a). Movement trajectories in the presence of a distracting stimulus: Evidence for a response activation model of selective reaching. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 1031–1057.PubMed Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2004a). Movement trajectories in the presence of a distracting stimulus: Evidence for a response activation model of selective reaching. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 1031–1057.PubMed
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2004b). Effects of response priming and inhibition on movement planning and execution. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 200–211.CrossRefPubMed Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2004b). Effects of response priming and inhibition on movement planning and execution. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 200–211.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2005). The effects of response priming on the planning and execution of goal-directed movements in the presence of a distracting stimulus. Acta Psychologica, 119, 123–142.CrossRefPubMed Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2005). The effects of response priming on the planning and execution of goal-directed movements in the presence of a distracting stimulus. Acta Psychologica, 119, 123–142.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., Elliott, D., Anson, J. G., Dhillon, V. P., Weeks, D. J., Lyons, J. L., et al. (2005). Does Joe influence Fred’s action? Inhibition of return across different nervous systems. Neuroscience Letters, 385, 99–104.CrossRefPubMed Welsh, T. N., Elliott, D., Anson, J. G., Dhillon, V. P., Weeks, D. J., Lyons, J. L., et al. (2005). Does Joe influence Fred’s action? Inhibition of return across different nervous systems. Neuroscience Letters, 385, 99–104.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., Elliott, D., & Weeks, D. J. (1999). Hand deviations toward distractors: Evidence for response competition. Experimental Brain Research, 127, 207–212.CrossRef Welsh, T. N., Elliott, D., & Weeks, D. J. (1999). Hand deviations toward distractors: Evidence for response competition. Experimental Brain Research, 127, 207–212.CrossRef
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., Lyons, J. L., Weeks, D. J., Anson, J. G., Chua, R., Mendoza, J., et al. (2007). Within- and between nervous system inhibition of return: Observation is as good as performance. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 950–956.PubMed Welsh, T. N., Lyons, J. L., Weeks, D. J., Anson, J. G., Chua, R., Mendoza, J., et al. (2007). Within- and between nervous system inhibition of return: Observation is as good as performance. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 950–956.PubMed
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., Ray, M., Weeks, D. J., Dewey, D., & Elliott, D. (2009). Does Joe influence Fred’s action? Not if Fred has autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1248, 141–148.CrossRefPubMed Welsh, T. N., Ray, M., Weeks, D. J., Dewey, D., & Elliott, D. (2009). Does Joe influence Fred’s action? Not if Fred has autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1248, 141–148.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Between-person effects on attention and action: Joe and Fred revisited
Auteurs
Spencer J. Hayes
Steve Hansen
Digby Elliott
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0250-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2010

Psychological Research 3/2010 Naar de uitgave