Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2008

01-03-2008 | Original Article

Effector-related sequence learning in a bimanual-bisequential serial reaction time task

Auteurs: Michael P. Berner, Joachim Hoffmann

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In a bimanual-bisequential version of the serial reaction time (SRT) task participants performed two uncorrelated key-press sequences simultaneously, one with fingers of the left hand and the other with fingers of the right hand. Participants responded to location-based imperative stimuli. When two such stimuli appeared in each trial, the results suggest independent learning of the two sequences and the occurrence of intermanual transfer. Following extended practice in Experiment 2, transfer of acquired sequence knowledge was not complete. Also in Experiment 2, when only one stimulus appeared in each trial specifying the responses for both hands so that there was no basis for separate stimulus–stimulus or separate response–effect learning, independent sequence learning was again evident, but there was no intermanual transfer at all. These findings suggest the existence of two mechanisms of sequence learning, one hand-related stimulus-based and the other motor-based, with only the former allowing for intermanual transfer.
Voetnoten
1
We would like to thank Willem Verwey for pointing this out.
 
2
One reviewer correctly pointed out that the performance decrements in transfer blocks might not be due to a failure of transfer but rather to interference: first, for the transfer hand the formerly practiced sequence might interfere with the new transferred sequence; second, the transferred sequence might become suppressed because the hand which has practiced this sequence before now has to perform a random sequence. However, the interference account can hardly explain why in transfer blocks of Experiment 1 no interference at all is indicated. In particular, in Experiment 1 the error data suggested nearly complete transfer as the hands which performed the transferred sequence never produced error costs, whereas the hands which performed the random sequence always produced error costs. Therefore we consider the account in terms of transfer at least as plausible as the interference account.
 
3
We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for alerting us to this alternative account.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bapi, R. S., Doya, K., & Harner, A. M. (2000). Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 149–162.CrossRef Bapi, R. S., Doya, K., & Harner, A. M. (2000). Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 149–162.CrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Clegg, B. A., DiGirolamo, G. J., & Keele, S. W. (1998). Sequence learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 275–281.CrossRef Clegg, B. A., DiGirolamo, G. J., & Keele, S. W. (1998). Sequence learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 275–281.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17–30.CrossRef Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17–30.CrossRef
go back to reference Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006). Perceptual or motor learning in SRT tasks with complex sequence structures. Psychological Research, 70, 88–102.PubMedCrossRef Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006). Perceptual or motor learning in SRT tasks with complex sequence structures. Psychological Research, 70, 88–102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 343–350.PubMed Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 343–350.PubMed
go back to reference Frensch, P. A., & Miner C. S. (1995). Zur Rolle des Arbeitsgedächtnisses beim impliziten Sequenzlernen (The role of working memory in implicit sequence learning). Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 42, 545–575. Frensch, P. A., & Miner C. S. (1995). Zur Rolle des Arbeitsgedächtnisses beim impliziten Sequenzlernen (The role of working memory in implicit sequence learning). Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 42, 545–575.
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (1998). Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 9420–9428.PubMed Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (1998). Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 9420–9428.PubMed
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Motor sequence learning with the nondominant left hand. A PET functional imaging study. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 369–378.CrossRef Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Motor sequence learning with the nondominant left hand. A PET functional imaging study. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 369–378.CrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine E. (2002). The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (Attention and Performance XIX, pp. 673–689). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Hazeltine E. (2002). The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (Attention and Performance XIX, pp. 673–689). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Hikosaka, O., Nakamura, K., Sakai, K., & Nakahara, H. (2002). Central mechanisms of motor skill learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 217–222.PubMedCrossRef Hikosaka, O., Nakamura, K., Sakai, K., & Nakahara, H. (2002). Central mechanisms of motor skill learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 217–222.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffmann, J. (2001). Representation of serial order, cognitive psychology of. In N. Smelser, & P. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (Vol. 19, pp. 13171–13174). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. Hoffmann, J. (2001). Representation of serial order, cognitive psychology of. In N. Smelser, & P. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (Vol. 19, pp. 13171–13174). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Koch, I. (1997). Stimulus-response compatibility and sequential learning in the serial reaction time task. Psychological Research, 60, 87–97.CrossRef Hoffmann, J., & Koch, I. (1997). Stimulus-response compatibility and sequential learning in the serial reaction time task. Psychological Research, 60, 87–97.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Koch, I. (1998). Implicit learning of loosely defined structures. In M. A. Stadler, & P. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 161–199). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Hoffmann, J., & Koch, I. (1998). Implicit learning of loosely defined structures. In M. A. Stadler, & P. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 161–199). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Martin, C., & Schilling, A. (2003). Unique transitions between stimuli and responses in SRT tasks: Evidence for the primacy of response predictions. Psychological Research, 67, 160–173.PubMedCrossRef Hoffmann, J., Martin, C., & Schilling, A. (2003). Unique transitions between stimuli and responses in SRT tasks: Evidence for the primacy of response predictions. Psychological Research, 67, 160–173.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (1996). Reiz- und Reaktionsmuster in seriellen Wahlreaktionen [Stimulus and response patterns in serial reaction time tasks]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 43, 40–68.PubMed Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (1996). Reiz- und Reaktionsmuster in seriellen Wahlreaktionen [Stimulus and response patterns in serial reaction time tasks]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 43, 40–68.PubMed
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2005). When obvious covariations are not even learned implicitly. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 449–480.CrossRef Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2005). When obvious covariations are not even learned implicitly. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 449–480.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.PubMedCrossRef Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Howard, J. H., Mutter, S. A., & Howard, D. V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1029–1039.PubMedCrossRef Howard, J. H., Mutter, S. A., & Howard, D. V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1029–1039.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jiménez, L., & Méndez, C. (1999). Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 236–259.CrossRef Jiménez, L., & Méndez, C. (1999). Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 236–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Ivry, R. B., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.PubMedCrossRef Keele, S. W., Ivry, R. B., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30.CrossRef Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30.CrossRef
go back to reference Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 863–882.PubMedCrossRef Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 863–882.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350–364.PubMedCrossRef Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350–364.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 60, 98–112.CrossRef Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 60, 98–112.CrossRef
go back to reference Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Park, J.-H., & Shea, C. H. (2005). Sequence learning: Response structure and effector transfer. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 387–419. Park, J.-H., & Shea, C. H. (2005). Sequence learning: Response structure and effector transfer. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 387–419.
go back to reference Remillard, G. (2003). Pure perceptual-based sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 581–597.PubMedCrossRef Remillard, G. (2003). Pure perceptual-based sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 581–597.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rhodes, B. J., Bullock, D., Verwey, W. B., Averbeck, B. B., & Page, M. P. A. (2004). Learning and production of movement sequences: Behavioral, neurophysiological, and modelling perspectives. Human Movement Science, 23, 699–746.PubMedCrossRef Rhodes, B. J., Bullock, D., Verwey, W. B., Averbeck, B. B., & Page, M. P. A. (2004). Learning and production of movement sequences: Behavioral, neurophysiological, and modelling perspectives. Human Movement Science, 23, 699–746.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Riedel, B., & Burton, A. M. (2006). Auditory sequence learning: Differential sensitivity to task relevant and task irrelevant sequences. Psychological Research, 70, 337–344.PubMedCrossRef Riedel, B., & Burton, A. M. (2006). Auditory sequence learning: Differential sensitivity to task relevant and task irrelevant sequences. Psychological Research, 70, 337–344.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rüsseler, J., Münte, T. F., & Rösler, F. (2002). Influence of stimulus distance in implicit learning of spatial and nonspatial event sequences. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 973–987.PubMedCrossRef Rüsseler, J., Münte, T. F., & Rösler, F. (2002). Influence of stimulus distance in implicit learning of spatial and nonspatial event sequences. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 973–987.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rüsseler, J., & Rösler, F. (2000). Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations. Acta Psychologica, 104, 45–67.PubMedCrossRef Rüsseler, J., & Rösler, F. (2000). Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations. Acta Psychologica, 104, 45–67.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schmidtke, V., & Heuer, H. (1997). Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning. Psychological Research, 60, 53–71.CrossRef Schmidtke, V., & Heuer, H. (1997). Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning. Psychological Research, 60, 53–71.CrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime, version 1.1. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime, version 1.1. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.
go back to reference Stöcker, C., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). The ideomotor principle and motor sequence acquisition: Tone effects facilitate movement chunking. Psychological Research, 68, 126–137.PubMedCrossRef Stöcker, C., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). The ideomotor principle and motor sequence acquisition: Tone effects facilitate movement chunking. Psychological Research, 68, 126–137.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stöcker, C., Sebald, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). The influence of response-effect compatibility in a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 586–703. Stöcker, C., Sebald, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). The influence of response-effect compatibility in a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 586–703.
go back to reference van der Graaf, F. H. C. E., de Jong, B. M., Maguire, R. P., Meiners, L. C., & Leenders, K. L. (2004). Cerebral activation related to skills practice in a double serial reaction time task: Striatal involvement in random-order sequence learning. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 120–131.CrossRef van der Graaf, F. H. C. E., de Jong, B. M., Maguire, R. P., Meiners, L. C., & Leenders, K. L. (2004). Cerebral activation related to skills practice in a double serial reaction time task: Striatal involvement in random-order sequence learning. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 120–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Verwey, W. B., & Clegg, B. A. (2005). Effector dependent sequence learning in the serial RT task. Psychological Research, 69, 242–251.PubMedCrossRef Verwey, W. B., & Clegg, B. A. (2005). Effector dependent sequence learning in the serial RT task. Psychological Research, 69, 242–251.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Verwey, W. B., & Wright, D. L. (2004). Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task. Psychological Research, 68, 64–70.PubMedCrossRef Verwey, W. B., & Wright, D. L. (2004). Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task. Psychological Research, 68, 64–70.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366–375. Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366–375.
go back to reference Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978.CrossRef Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978.CrossRef
go back to reference Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 595–613.PubMedCrossRef Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 595–613.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Effector-related sequence learning in a bimanual-bisequential serial reaction time task
Auteurs
Michael P. Berner
Joachim Hoffmann
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0097-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2008

Psychological Research 2/2008 Naar de uitgave