Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2006

01-09-2006 | Original Article

Auditory sequence learning: differential sensitivity to task relevant and task irrelevant sequences

Auteurs: B. Riedel, A. Mike Burton

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Using a serial reaction time task, this study examines whether learning of auditory sequences is possible without a corresponding motor response, i.e., by listening alone. The dual sequence paradigm used by Mayr (in Journal of the Experimental Psychology: Learning memory and cognition 22:350–354, 1996, Experiment 1) was adapted to the auditory domain. Four different actors spoke the same four colour words. These were presented such that speaker identity followed one sequence, and the word spoken followed a different sequence. Subjects were asked to respond (with a key press) to one of these dimensions (identity or word), and ignore the other. Results showed learning for either type of stimulus, but only when it was responded to. No learning of either type of auditory sequence by listening alone was found. The results add evidence to visual implicit learning studies that have failed to find learning of event sequences when spatial or response selection was not an important factor in processing. The findings are discussed in the context of implicit learning as a general and fundamental cognitive process.
Literatuur
go back to reference Buchner, A. & Frensch, P.A. (1997). Sequence learning. Phenomena and models. Psychological Research, 60, 1–3.CrossRef Buchner, A. & Frensch, P.A. (1997). Sequence learning. Phenomena and models. Psychological Research, 60, 1–3.CrossRef
go back to reference Buchner, A., Steffens, M.C., Erdfelder, E. & Rothkegel, R. (1997). A multinomial model to assess fluency and recollection in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 631–663.CrossRef Buchner, A., Steffens, M.C., Erdfelder, E. & Rothkegel, R. (1997). A multinomial model to assess fluency and recollection in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 631–663.CrossRef
go back to reference Buchner, A., Steffens, M.C. & Rothkegel, R. (1998). On the role of fragmentary knowledge in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 251–281. Buchner, A., Steffens, M.C. & Rothkegel, R. (1998). On the role of fragmentary knowledge in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 251–281.
go back to reference Heuer, H., Schmidtke, V. & Kleinsorge, T. (2001). Implicit learning of Sequences of Tasks..Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(4) , 967–983. Heuer, H., Schmidtke, V. & Kleinsorge, T. (2001). Implicit learning of Sequences of Tasks..Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(4) , 967–983.
go back to reference Howard, J.H., Mutter, S.A. & Howard, D.V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5) , 109–1039. Howard, J.H., Mutter, S.A. & Howard, D.V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5) , 109–1039.
go back to reference Jacoby, L.L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.CrossRef Jacoby, L.L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.CrossRef
go back to reference Jimenez, L. & Mendez, C. (1999). Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1) , 236–259. Jimenez, L. & Mendez, C. (1999). Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1) , 236–259.
go back to reference Kelly, S.W. & Burton, A.M. (2001). Learning complex sequences by observation? Psychological Research, 65, 15–23.CrossRefPubMed Kelly, S.W. & Burton, A.M. (2001). Learning complex sequences by observation? Psychological Research, 65, 15–23.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kelly, S.W., Burton, A.M., Riedel, B & Lynch, E. (2003). Sequence learning by observation: evidence for separate mechanisms. British Journal of Psychology, 94 (3), 355–372.CrossRefPubMed Kelly, S.W., Burton, A.M., Riedel, B & Lynch, E. (2003). Sequence learning by observation: evidence for separate mechanisms. British Journal of Psychology, 94 (3), 355–372.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koch, I. & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(4), 863–882. Koch, I. & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(4), 863–882.
go back to reference Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 350–364. Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 350–364.
go back to reference Mondor, T.A. & Bregman, A.S. (1994). Allocating attention to frequency regions. Perception and Psychophysics, 56(3), 268–276.PubMed Mondor, T.A. & Bregman, A.S. (1994). Allocating attention to frequency regions. Perception and Psychophysics, 56(3), 268–276.PubMed
go back to reference Mondor, T.A. & Zatorre, R.J. (1994). Shifting and focusing auditory spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(2), 387–409. Mondor, T.A. & Zatorre, R.J. (1994). Shifting and focusing auditory spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(2), 387–409.
go back to reference Newell, B.R. & Bright, J.E.H. (2002). Well past midnight: calling time on implicit invariant learning? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 185–205.CrossRef Newell, B.R. & Bright, J.E.H. (2002). Well past midnight: calling time on implicit invariant learning? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 185–205.CrossRef
go back to reference Nissen, M.J. & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef Nissen, M.J. & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Perruchet, P. & Pacteau, C. (1990). Synthetic grammar learning: implicit rule abstraction or explicit fragmentary knowledge? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 19, 264–275. Perruchet, P. & Pacteau, C. (1990). Synthetic grammar learning: implicit rule abstraction or explicit fragmentary knowledge? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 19, 264–275.
go back to reference Perruchet, P., Bigand, E. & Benoit-Gonin, F. (1997). The emergence of explicit knowledge during the early phase of learning in sequential reaction time tasks. Psychological Research, 60, 87–97.CrossRef Perruchet, P., Bigand, E. & Benoit-Gonin, F. (1997). The emergence of explicit knowledge during the early phase of learning in sequential reaction time tasks. Psychological Research, 60, 87–97.CrossRef
go back to reference Reber, A.S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 6, 855–863. Reber, A.S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 6, 855–863.
go back to reference Reber, A.S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235. Reber, A.S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235.
go back to reference Reber, A.S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: an essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press. Reber, A.S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: an essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Reber, A.S. (1994). What manner of mind is this? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(3) , 418–419. Reber, A.S. (1994). What manner of mind is this? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(3) , 418–419.
go back to reference Saffran, J.R., Johnson, E.K., Aslin, R.N. & Newport, E.L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 27–52.CrossRefPubMed Saffran, J.R., Johnson, E.K., Aslin, R.N. & Newport, E.L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 27–52.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmidtke, V & Heuer, H. (1997). Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning. Psychological Research, 60, 53–71.CrossRef Schmidtke, V & Heuer, H. (1997). Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning. Psychological Research, 60, 53–71.CrossRef
go back to reference Seger, C.A. (1997). Two forms of sequential implicit learning. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 108–131.CrossRefPubMed Seger, C.A. (1997). Two forms of sequential implicit learning. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 108–131.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shanks, D.R. & Johnston, T. (1999). Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6 ) , 1435–1451. Shanks, D.R. & Johnston, T. (1999). Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6 ) , 1435–1451.
go back to reference Willingham, D.B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory and Cognition, 27, 561–572. Willingham, D.B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory and Cognition, 27, 561–572.
go back to reference Willingham, D.B., Nissen, M.J., Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1047–1060. Willingham, D.B., Nissen, M.J., Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1047–1060.
go back to reference Willingham, D.B., Wells, L.A., Farrell, J.M., Stemwedel, M.E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory and Cognition, 28(3) , 366–375. Willingham, D.B., Wells, L.A., Farrell, J.M., Stemwedel, M.E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory and Cognition, 28(3) , 366–375.
Metagegevens
Titel
Auditory sequence learning: differential sensitivity to task relevant and task irrelevant sequences
Auteurs
B. Riedel
A. Mike Burton
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0226-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2006

Psychological Research 5/2006 Naar de uitgave