Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2010

01-01-2010 | Original Article

In hindsight, life flows from left to right

Auteurs: Julio Santiago, Antonio Román, Marc Ouellet, Nieves Rodríguez, Pilar Pérez-Azor

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Three experiments investigated the mental representation of meaningful event sequences. Experiment 1 used extended (5 min long) naturalistic scenes excerpted from commercial movies. Experiments 2 and 3 presented everyday activities by means of sequences of six photographs. All experiments found both left–right and distance effects in an order decision task, suggesting that when contemplated in hindsight, experienced events unfold along a left-to-right analogical mental line. Present results are discussed in the context of the mental representation of other kinds of ordinal sequences, and other left–right effects reported in non-ordinal domains.
Voetnoten
1
The counterbalancing factor was not included in the design. Although there could be interactions with this factor, they would be of no theoretical value. Counterbalancing assures that order-practice effects are controlled for. Sometimes it is useful to include it as a factor in the analyses to take off some error variance and improve the power of the analysis, but it is not necessary in the present experiments, as all ANOVAs already provide significant results for the factors of interest.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Altmann, L. J. P., Saleem, A., Kendall, D., Heilman, K. M., & Rothi, L. J. G. (2006). Orthographic directionality and thematic role illustration in English and Arabic. Brain and Language, 97, 306–316.CrossRefPubMed Altmann, L. J. P., Saleem, A., Kendall, D., Heilman, K. M., & Rothi, L. J. G. (2006). Orthographic directionality and thematic role illustration in English and Arabic. Brain and Language, 97, 306–316.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Barrett, A. M., Kim, M., Crucian, G. P., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). Spatial bias: Effects of early reading direction on Korean subjects. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1003–1012.CrossRefPubMed Barrett, A. M., Kim, M., Crucian, G. P., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). Spatial bias: Effects of early reading direction on Korean subjects. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1003–1012.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Kerzel, D. (2004). A Simon Effect With Stationary Moving Stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 39–55.CrossRefPubMed Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Kerzel, D. (2004). A Simon Effect With Stationary Moving Stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 39–55.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bush, L. K., Hess, U., & Wolford, G. (1993). Transformations for within-subject designs: A Monte-Carlo investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 566–579.CrossRefPubMed Bush, L. K., Hess, U., & Wolford, G. (1993). Transformations for within-subject designs: A Monte-Carlo investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 566–579.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Butterworth, B. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet (pp. 9–34). Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Butterworth, B. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet (pp. 9–34). Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: Bisecting horizontal and vertical number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2989–3000.CrossRefPubMed Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: Bisecting horizontal and vertical number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2989–3000.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102, 101–117.CrossRefPubMed Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102, 101–117.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593.CrossRefPubMed Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chatterjee, A. (2001). Language and space: Some interactions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 55–61.CrossRefPubMed Chatterjee, A. (2001). Language and space: Some interactions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 55–61.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chatterjee, A., Maher, L. M., Gonzalez-Rothi, L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Asyntactic thematic role assignment: The use of a temporal-spatial strategy. Brain and Language, 49, 125–139.CrossRefPubMed Chatterjee, A., Maher, L. M., Gonzalez-Rothi, L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Asyntactic thematic role assignment: The use of a temporal-spatial strategy. Brain and Language, 49, 125–139.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chatterjee, A., Southwood, M. H., & Basilico, D. (1999). Verbs, events and spatial representations. Neuropsychologia, 37, 395–402.CrossRefPubMed Chatterjee, A., Southwood, M. H., & Basilico, D. (1999). Verbs, events and spatial representations. Neuropsychologia, 37, 395–402.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Doricci, F., Guariglia, P., Gasparini, M., & Tomaiuolo, F. (2005). Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1663–1665.CrossRef Doricci, F., Guariglia, P., Gasparini, M., & Tomaiuolo, F. (2005). Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1663–1665.CrossRef
go back to reference Fias, W., & Fischer, M. H. (2005). Spatial Representation of Numbers. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 43–54). Hove: Psychology Press. Fias, W., & Fischer, M. H. (2005). Spatial Representation of Numbers. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 43–54). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 555–556.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 555–556.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87, B87–B95.CrossRefPubMed Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87, B87–B95.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2004). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organised: Evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 40, 171–172.CrossRefPubMed Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2004). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organised: Evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 40, 171–172.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.
go back to reference Longo, M. R., & Lourenço, S. F. (2007). Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1400–1407.CrossRefPubMed Longo, M. R., & Lourenço, S. F. (2007). Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1400–1407.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lorch, R. F., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 16, 149–157.CrossRef Lorch, R. F., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 16, 149–157.CrossRef
go back to reference Maas, A., & Russo, A. (2003). Directional bias in the mental representation of spatial events: Nature or culture? Psychological Science, 14, 296–301.CrossRef Maas, A., & Russo, A. (2003). Directional bias in the mental representation of spatial events: Nature or culture? Psychological Science, 14, 296–301.CrossRef
go back to reference Maher, L. M., Chatterjee, A., Gonzalez-Rothi, L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Agrammatic sentence production: The use of a temporal-spatial strategy. Brain and Language, 49, 105–124.CrossRefPubMed Maher, L. M., Chatterjee, A., Gonzalez-Rothi, L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Agrammatic sentence production: The use of a temporal-spatial strategy. Brain and Language, 49, 105–124.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.CrossRefPubMed Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: convergent evidence from aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cogn Sci, 30, 1–49. Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: convergent evidence from aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cogn Sci, 30, 1–49.
go back to reference Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Funes, M. J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2008) Thinking about time makes attention move to the sides (submitted) Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Funes, M. J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2008) Thinking about time makes attention move to the sides (submitted)
go back to reference Price, M. C., & Mentzoni, R. A. (2008). Where is January? The month-SNARC effect in sequence-form synaesthetes. Cortex, 44, 890–907.CrossRefPubMed Price, M. C., & Mentzoni, R. A. (2008). Where is January? The month-SNARC effect in sequence-form synaesthetes. Cortex, 44, 890–907.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Priftis, K., Zorzi, M., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umiltà, C. (2006). Explicit vs. implicit processing of representational space in neglect: Dissociations in accessing the mental number line. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 680–688.CrossRefPubMed Priftis, K., Zorzi, M., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umiltà, C. (2006). Explicit vs. implicit processing of representational space in neglect: Dissociations in accessing the mental number line. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 680–688.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: a general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416–442.CrossRefPubMed Proctor, R., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: a general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416–442.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rinaldi, M. C., & Pizzamiglio, L. (2006). When space merges into language. Neuropsychologia, 44, 556–565.CrossRefPubMed Rinaldi, M. C., & Pizzamiglio, L. (2006). When space merges into language. Neuropsychologia, 44, 556–565.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512–516. Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512–516.
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.
go back to reference Stoianov, I., Kramer, P., Umiltá, C., & Zorzi, M. (2008). Visuospatial priming of the mental number line. Cognition, 106, 770–779.CrossRefPubMed Stoianov, I., Kramer, P., Umiltá, C., & Zorzi, M. (2008). Visuospatial priming of the mental number line. Cognition, 106, 770–779.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Funes, M. J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30, 745–757.CrossRef Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Funes, M. J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30, 745–757.CrossRef
go back to reference Tversky, B. (2004). Narratives of space, time, and life. Mind and Language, 19, 380–392.CrossRef Tversky, B. (2004). Narratives of space, time, and life. Mind and Language, 19, 380–392.CrossRef
go back to reference Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 515–557.CrossRef Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 515–557.CrossRef
go back to reference Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A. & Shallice, T. (in press) An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition. Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A. & Shallice, T. (in press) An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition.
go back to reference Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483–488.CrossRefPubMed Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483–488.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weger, U. & Pratt, J. (in press) Time flies like an arrow: Shifting spatial attention in response to adverbs of time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. Weger, U. & Pratt, J. (in press) Time flies like an arrow: Shifting spatial attention in response to adverbs of time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
go back to reference Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 3–21.CrossRefPubMed Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 3–21.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zacks, J. M., Tversky, B., & Iyer, G. (2001). Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 29–58.CrossRef Zacks, J. M., Tversky, B., & Iyer, G. (2001). Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 29–58.CrossRef
go back to reference Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the REVERSED SNARC effect in English, Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 165–190.CrossRef Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the REVERSED SNARC effect in English, Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 165–190.CrossRef
go back to reference Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138–139.CrossRefPubMed Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138–139.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umiltà, C. (2006). The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: Evidence from neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1061–1067.CrossRefPubMed Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umiltà, C. (2006). The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: Evidence from neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1061–1067.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
In hindsight, life flows from left to right
Auteurs
Julio Santiago
Antonio Román
Marc Ouellet
Nieves Rodríguez
Pilar Pérez-Azor
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0220-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2010

Psychological Research 1/2010 Naar de uitgave