Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2010

01-01-2010 | Original Article

Adaptation to a direction-dependent visuomotor gain in the young and elderly

Auteurs: Mathias Hegele, Herbert Heuer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Consistent with the widely accepted notion of separate specification of movement amplitude and direction, it has been argued that there is also a categorical difference between adaptation to novel visuomotor rotations and to novel visuomotor gains. In line with this view, ageing seems to affect rotation and gain adaptation differently in that age-related impairments are consistently found for the former, but not for the latter. In this study we ask whether the contrasting findings could also be ascribed to differences in the level of difficulty of gain and rotation adaptation tasks, respectively. In order to increase the difficulty of gain adaptation, younger and older participants had to adapt to a direction-dependent gain transformation. Results revealed direction-dependent adaptation in both groups. More importantly, we replicated the typical findings of age-related impairments of adaptation, but not of aftereffects, that were previously only reported for rotation adaptation. Younger participants also showed superior explicit knowledge regarding the novel visuomotor mapping as compared to the older participants. We show that this knowledge was used by younger participants to selectively augment adaptive shifts. Finally, our findings suggest that the difficulty of the novel visuomotor transformation and, related to this, the involvement of explicit knowledge in adaptation is critical for age-related changes to show up, but not the type of adaptation task, rotation and gain adaptation, respectively.
Voetnoten
1
The dependent variables used in the studies differed. Whereas Heuer and Hegele assessed the directional errors at the end of visually open-loop movements, Seidler measured directional errors only at the time of peak velocity (and found no age-related group differences) and final movement errors in terms of the total distance from movement end point to visual target. The latter measure includes amplitude as well as directional errors and might thus be fallible to systematic differences in movement amplitude between the age groups (Heuer & Hegele, 2007). Thus it does not reflect genuine adaptation to directional errors as induced by the visuomotor rotation.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2001a). Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input. Experimental Brain Research, 139, 248–253.CrossRef Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2001a). Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input. Experimental Brain Research, 139, 248–253.CrossRef
go back to reference Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2001b). Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: Different mechanisms for small versus large rotations. Experimental Brain Research, 140, 407–410.CrossRef Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2001b). Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: Different mechanisms for small versus large rotations. Experimental Brain Research, 140, 407–410.CrossRef
go back to reference Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2003). Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 128–132.CrossRef Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2003). Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 128–132.CrossRef
go back to reference Amthauer, R. (1953). I–S-T Intelligenz-Struktur-test. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Amthauer, R. (1953). I–S-T Intelligenz-Struktur-test. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Bock, O. (1992). Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensory-motor discordance: Evidence for direction-independent gain control. Behavioral Brain Research, 51, 41–50.CrossRef Bock, O. (1992). Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensory-motor discordance: Evidence for direction-independent gain control. Behavioral Brain Research, 51, 41–50.CrossRef
go back to reference Bock, O. (2005). Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 259–263.CrossRef Bock, O. (2005). Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 259–263.CrossRef
go back to reference Bock, O., & Arnold, K. (1992). Motor control prior to movement onset: Preparatory mechanisms for pointing at visual targets. Experimental Brain Research, 90, 209–216.CrossRef Bock, O., & Arnold, K. (1992). Motor control prior to movement onset: Preparatory mechanisms for pointing at visual targets. Experimental Brain Research, 90, 209–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Bock, O., & Burghoff, M. (1997). Visuo-motor adaptation: Evidence for a distributed amplitude control system. Behavioral Brain Research, 89, 267–273.CrossRef Bock, O., & Burghoff, M. (1997). Visuo-motor adaptation: Evidence for a distributed amplitude control system. Behavioral Brain Research, 89, 267–273.CrossRef
go back to reference Bock, O., & Eversheim, U. (2000). The mechanisms of movement preparation: A precuing study. Behavioural Brain Research, 108, 85–90.CrossRefPubMed Bock, O., & Eversheim, U. (2000). The mechanisms of movement preparation: A precuing study. Behavioural Brain Research, 108, 85–90.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bock, O., & Girgenrath, M. (2006). Relationship between sensorimotor adaptation and cognitive functions in younger and older subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 400–406.CrossRef Bock, O., & Girgenrath, M. (2006). Relationship between sensorimotor adaptation and cognitive functions in younger and older subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 400–406.CrossRef
go back to reference Bock, O., & Schneider, S. (2002). Sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 761–767.CrossRefPubMed Bock, O., & Schneider, S. (2002). Sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 761–767.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buch, E. R., Young, S., & Contreras-Vidal, J. L. (2003). Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. Learning and Memory, 10, 55–63.CrossRefPubMed Buch, E. R., Young, S., & Contreras-Vidal, J. L. (2003). Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. Learning and Memory, 10, 55–63.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cunningham, H. A. (1989). Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 493–506.CrossRefPubMed Cunningham, H. A. (1989). Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 493–506.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Desmurget, M., Jordan, M. I., Prablanc, C., & Jeannerod, M. (1997). Constrained and unconstrained movements involve different control strategies. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 1644–1650.PubMed Desmurget, M., Jordan, M. I., Prablanc, C., & Jeannerod, M. (1997). Constrained and unconstrained movements involve different control strategies. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 1644–1650.PubMed
go back to reference Desmurget, M., Prablanc, C., Jordan, M. I., & Jeannerod, M. (1999). Are reaching movement planned to be straight and invariant in extrinsic space: Kinematic comparison between compliant and unconstrained motions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 981–1020.CrossRef Desmurget, M., Prablanc, C., Jordan, M. I., & Jeannerod, M. (1999). Are reaching movement planned to be straight and invariant in extrinsic space: Kinematic comparison between compliant and unconstrained motions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 981–1020.CrossRef
go back to reference Favilla, M., Hening, W., & Ghez, C. (1989). Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. VI: Independent specification of response amplitude and direction. Experimental Brain Research, 75, 280–294.CrossRef Favilla, M., Hening, W., & Ghez, C. (1989). Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. VI: Independent specification of response amplitude and direction. Experimental Brain Research, 75, 280–294.CrossRef
go back to reference Flanagan, J. R., & Rao, A. K. (1995). Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: Evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 2174–2178.PubMed Flanagan, J. R., & Rao, A. K. (1995). Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: Evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 2174–2178.PubMed
go back to reference Fraser, H. B., Khaitovich, P., Plotkin, J. B., Pääbo, S., & Eisen, M. B. (2005). Aging and gene expression in the primate brain. PLoS Biology, 3, 1653–1661.CrossRef Fraser, H. B., Khaitovich, P., Plotkin, J. B., Pääbo, S., & Eisen, M. B. (2005). Aging and gene expression in the primate brain. PLoS Biology, 3, 1653–1661.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1991). WIlled action and the prefrontal cortex in man: A study with PET. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 244, 241–246.CrossRefPubMed Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1991). WIlled action and the prefrontal cortex in man: A study with PET. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 244, 241–246.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Georgopoulos, A. P., Schwartz, A. B., & Kettner, R. E. (1986). Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science, 233, 1416–1419.CrossRefPubMed Georgopoulos, A. P., Schwartz, A. B., & Kettner, R. E. (1986). Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science, 233, 1416–1419.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ghez, C., Favilla, M., Ghilardi, M. F., Gordon, J., Bermejo, R., & Pullman, S. (1997). Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements nd isometric force trajectories. Experimental Brain Research, 115, 217–233.CrossRef Ghez, C., Favilla, M., Ghilardi, M. F., Gordon, J., Bermejo, R., & Pullman, S. (1997). Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements nd isometric force trajectories. Experimental Brain Research, 115, 217–233.CrossRef
go back to reference Ghilardi, M. F., Ghez, C., Dhawan, V., Moeller, J., Mentis, M., Nakamura, T., et al. (2000). Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning. Brain Research, 871, 127–145.CrossRefPubMed Ghilardi, M. F., Ghez, C., Dhawan, V., Moeller, J., Mentis, M., Nakamura, T., et al. (2000). Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning. Brain Research, 871, 127–145.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1994). Accuracy of planar reaching movements. 1: Independence of direction and extent variability. Experimental Brain Research, 99, 97–111.CrossRef Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1994). Accuracy of planar reaching movements. 1: Independence of direction and extent variability. Experimental Brain Research, 99, 97–111.CrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2007). Learning new visuo-motor gains at early and late working age. Ergonomics, 90, 979–1003.CrossRef Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2007). Learning new visuo-motor gains at early and late working age. Ergonomics, 90, 979–1003.CrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2008a). Adaptation to visuo-motor rotations in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 190–202.CrossRefPubMed Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2008a). Adaptation to visuo-motor rotations in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 190–202.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2008b). Constraints on visuo-motor adaptation depend on the type of visual feedback during practice. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 101–110.CrossRef Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2008b). Constraints on visuo-motor adaptation depend on the type of visual feedback during practice. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 101–110.CrossRef
go back to reference Hollerbach, J. M., & Atkeson, C. G. (1987). Inferring limb coordination strategies from trajectory kinematics. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 181–194. Hollerbach, J. M., & Atkeson, C. G. (1987). Inferring limb coordination strategies from trajectory kinematics. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 181–194.
go back to reference Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1970). Conditions under which the mean square ratios in repeated measures designs have exact F-distributions. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 65, 1582–1589.CrossRef Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1970). Conditions under which the mean square ratios in repeated measures designs have exact F-distributions. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 65, 1582–1589.CrossRef
go back to reference Imamizu, H., Miyauchi, S., Tamada, T., Sasaki, Y., Takino, R., Putz, B., et al. (2000). Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature, 403, 192–195.CrossRefPubMed Imamizu, H., Miyauchi, S., Tamada, T., Sasaki, Y., Takino, R., Putz, B., et al. (2000). Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature, 403, 192–195.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Imamizu, H., & Shimojo, S. (1995). The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: Implications from intermanual transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 719–733.CrossRefPubMed Imamizu, H., & Shimojo, S. (1995). The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: Implications from intermanual transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 719–733.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Inoue, K., Kawashima, R., Satoh, K., Kinomura, S., Sugiura, M., Goto, R., et al. (2000). A PET study of visuomotor learning under optical rotation. Neuroimage, 11, 505–516.CrossRefPubMed Inoue, K., Kawashima, R., Satoh, K., Kinomura, S., Sugiura, M., Goto, R., et al. (2000). A PET study of visuomotor learning under optical rotation. Neuroimage, 11, 505–516.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Inoue, M., Suhara, T., Sudo, Y., Okubo, Y., Yasuno, F., Kishimoto, T., et al. (2001). Age-related reduction of extrastriatal dopamine D2 receptors measured by PET. Life Sciences, 69, 1079–1084.CrossRefPubMed Inoue, M., Suhara, T., Sudo, Y., Okubo, Y., Yasuno, F., Kishimoto, T., et al. (2001). Age-related reduction of extrastriatal dopamine D2 receptors measured by PET. Life Sciences, 69, 1079–1084.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ishihara, S. (2005). Ishihara’s tests for colour deficiency. London: H.K. Lewis. Ishihara, S. (2005). Ishihara’s tests for colour deficiency. London: H.K. Lewis.
go back to reference Jakobson, L. S., & Goodale, M. A. (1989). Trajectories of reaches to prismatically-displaced targets: evidence for “automatic” visuomotor recalibration. Experimental Brain Research, 78, 575–587.CrossRef Jakobson, L. S., & Goodale, M. A. (1989). Trajectories of reaches to prismatically-displaced targets: evidence for “automatic” visuomotor recalibration. Experimental Brain Research, 78, 575–587.CrossRef
go back to reference Kaasinen, V., Vilkman, H., Hietala, J., Nagren, K., Helenius, H., Olsson, H., et al. (2005). Age-related dopamine D2/D3 receptor loss in extrastriatal regions of the human brain. Neurobiology of Aging, 21, 683–688.CrossRef Kaasinen, V., Vilkman, H., Hietala, J., Nagren, K., Helenius, H., Olsson, H., et al. (2005). Age-related dopamine D2/D3 receptor loss in extrastriatal regions of the human brain. Neurobiology of Aging, 21, 683–688.CrossRef
go back to reference Ketcham, C. J., Seidler, R. D., Van Gemmert, A. W. A., & Stelmach, G. E. (2002). Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target-size, and movement amplitude. Journal of Gerontology: The Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, P54–P64. Ketcham, C. J., Seidler, R. D., Van Gemmert, A. W. A., & Stelmach, G. E. (2002). Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target-size, and movement amplitude. Journal of Gerontology: The Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, P54–P64.
go back to reference Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. . F., Mentis, M., Barnes, A., Veytsman, M., Eidelberg, D., et al. (2004). Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 924–933.CrossRefPubMed Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. . F., Mentis, M., Barnes, A., Veytsman, M., Eidelberg, D., et al. (2004). Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 924–933.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krakauer, J. W., Pine, Z. M., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (2000). Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8916–8924.PubMed Krakauer, J. W., Pine, Z. M., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (2000). Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8916–8924.PubMed
go back to reference Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikström, S. (2001). Aging cognition: From neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 479–486.CrossRef Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikström, S. (2001). Aging cognition: From neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 479–486.CrossRef
go back to reference Magescas, F., & Prablanc, C. (2006a). A joint-centered model accounts for movement curvature and spatial variability. Neuroscience Letters, 403, 114–118.CrossRefPubMed Magescas, F., & Prablanc, C. (2006a). A joint-centered model accounts for movement curvature and spatial variability. Neuroscience Letters, 403, 114–118.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Magescas, F., & Prablanc, C. (2006b). Automatic drive of limb motor plasticity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 75–83.CrossRefPubMed Magescas, F., & Prablanc, C. (2006b). Automatic drive of limb motor plasticity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 75–83.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Malfait, N., & Ostry, D. J. (2004). Is interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation a cognitive response to the sudden introduction of load? Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 8084–8089.CrossRefPubMed Malfait, N., & Ostry, D. J. (2004). Is interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation a cognitive response to the sudden introduction of load? Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 8084–8089.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McNay, E. C., & Willingham, D. B. (1998). Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptuomotor recalibration, with aging. Learning and Memory, 4, 411–420.CrossRefPubMed McNay, E. C., & Willingham, D. B. (1998). Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptuomotor recalibration, with aging. Learning and Memory, 4, 411–420.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Messier, J., & Kalaska, J. F. (1997). Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements. Experimental Brain Research, 115, 469–478.CrossRef Messier, J., & Kalaska, J. F. (1997). Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements. Experimental Brain Research, 115, 469–478.CrossRef
go back to reference Michel, C., Pisella, L., Prablanc, C., Rode, G., & Rossetti, Y. (2007). Enhancing visuomotor adaptation by reducing error signals: Single-step (aware) versus multiple-step (unaware) exposure to wedge prisms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 341–350.CrossRefPubMed Michel, C., Pisella, L., Prablanc, C., Rode, G., & Rossetti, Y. (2007). Enhancing visuomotor adaptation by reducing error signals: Single-step (aware) versus multiple-step (unaware) exposure to wedge prisms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 341–350.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475–487.CrossRefPubMed Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475–487.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Palluel-Germain, R., Boy, F., Orliaguet, J. -P., & Coello, Y. (2004). Visual and motor constraints on trajectory planning in pointing movements. Neuroscience Letters, 372, 235–239.CrossRefPubMed Palluel-Germain, R., Boy, F., Orliaguet, J. -P., & Coello, Y. (2004). Visual and motor constraints on trajectory planning in pointing movements. Neuroscience Letters, 372, 235–239.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pennel, I., Coello, Y., & Orliaguet, J. -P. (2002). Frame of reference and adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task. Ergonomics, 45, 1047–1077.CrossRefPubMed Pennel, I., Coello, Y., & Orliaguet, J. -P. (2002). Frame of reference and adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task. Ergonomics, 45, 1047–1077.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pine, Z. M., Krakauer, J. W., Gordon, J., & Ghez, C. (1996). Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. Neuroreport, 7, 2357–2361.CrossRefPubMed Pine, Z. M., Krakauer, J. W., Gordon, J., & Ghez, C. (1996). Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. Neuroreport, 7, 2357–2361.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Vu, K. L., & Pick, D. F. (2005). Aging and response selection in spatial choice tasks. Human Factors, 47, 250–270.CrossRefPubMed Proctor, R. W., Vu, K. L., & Pick, D. F. (2005). Aging and response selection in spatial choice tasks. Human Factors, 47, 250–270.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Redding, G., Rossetti, Y., & Wallace, B. (2005). Applications of prism adaptation: A tutorial in theory and method. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 431–444.CrossRefPubMed Redding, G., Rossetti, Y., & Wallace, B. (2005). Applications of prism adaptation: A tutorial in theory and method. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 431–444.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Redding, G., & Wallace, B. (1996). Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 379–394.CrossRefPubMed Redding, G., & Wallace, B. (1996). Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 379–394.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm direction and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 444–474.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm direction and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 444–474.CrossRef
go back to reference Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 68–87.CrossRef Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 68–87.CrossRef
go back to reference Schoppe, K. J. (1974). Das MLS-Gerät. Ein neuer Testapparat zur Messung feinmotorischer Leistungen [The MLS box: A new test apparatus to measure fine motor performance]. Diagnostica, 20, 43–46. Schoppe, K. J. (1974). Das MLS-Gerät. Ein neuer Testapparat zur Messung feinmotorischer Leistungen [The MLS box: A new test apparatus to measure fine motor performance]. Diagnostica, 20, 43–46.
go back to reference Scott, S. H., & Kalaska, J. F. (1995). Changes in motor cortex activity during reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm postures. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2563–2567.PubMed Scott, S. H., & Kalaska, J. F. (1995). Changes in motor cortex activity during reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm postures. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2563–2567.PubMed
go back to reference Seidler, R. D. (2006). Differential effects of age on sequence learning and sensorimotor adaptation. Brain Research Bulletin, 70, 337–346.CrossRefPubMed Seidler, R. D. (2006). Differential effects of age on sequence learning and sensorimotor adaptation. Brain Research Bulletin, 70, 337–346.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Seidler, R. D., & Stelmach, G. E. (1995). Reduction in sensorimotor control with age. Quest, 47, 386–394. Seidler, R. D., & Stelmach, G. E. (1995). Reduction in sensorimotor control with age. Quest, 47, 386–394.
go back to reference Szafran, J. (1951). Changes with age and with exclusion of vision in performance at an aiming test. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 111–118.CrossRef Szafran, J. (1951). Changes with age and with exclusion of vision in performance at an aiming test. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 111–118.CrossRef
go back to reference Teeken, J. C., Adam, J. J., Paas, F. G. W. C., van Boxtel, M. P. J., Houx, P. J., & Jolles, J. (1996). Effects of age and gender on discrete and reciprocal aiming movements. Psychology and Aging, 11, 195–198.CrossRefPubMed Teeken, J. C., Adam, J. J., Paas, F. G. W. C., van Boxtel, M. P. J., Houx, P. J., & Jolles, J. (1996). Effects of age and gender on discrete and reciprocal aiming movements. Psychology and Aging, 11, 195–198.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tewes, U. (1991). Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (Rev. Ed.). Toronto: Huber. Tewes, U. (1991). Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (Rev. Ed.). Toronto: Huber.
go back to reference Todorov, E. (2000). Direct cortical control of muscle activation in voluntary arm movements: A model. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 391–398.CrossRefPubMed Todorov, E. (2000). Direct cortical control of muscle activation in voluntary arm movements: A model. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 391–398.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Uhlarik, J. J. (1973). Role of cognitive factors on adaptation to prismatic displacement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 98, 223–232.CrossRefPubMed Uhlarik, J. J. (1973). Role of cognitive factors on adaptation to prismatic displacement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 98, 223–232.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vercruyssen, M. (1997). Movement control and speed of behavior. In A. D. Fisk & W. A. Rogers (Eds.), Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 55–86). San Diego: Academic Press. Vercruyssen, M. (1997). Movement control and speed of behavior. In A. D. Fisk & W. A. Rogers (Eds.), Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 55–86). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Vindras, P., & Viviani, P. (1998). Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 569–591.CrossRefPubMed Vindras, P., & Viviani, P. (1998). Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 569–591.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vindras, P., & Viviani, P. (2002). Altering the visuomotor gain: Evidence that motor plans deal with vector quantities. Experimental Brain Research, 147, 280–295.CrossRef Vindras, P., & Viviani, P. (2002). Altering the visuomotor gain: Evidence that motor plans deal with vector quantities. Experimental Brain Research, 147, 280–295.CrossRef
go back to reference Volkow, N. D., Gur, R. C., Wang, G. -J., Fowler, J. S., Moberg, P. J., Ding, Y. -S., et al. (1998). Association between decline in brain dopmine activity with age and cognitive and motor impairment in healthy individuals. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 344–349.PubMed Volkow, N. D., Gur, R. C., Wang, G. -J., Fowler, J. S., Moberg, P. J., Ding, Y. -S., et al. (1998). Association between decline in brain dopmine activity with age and cognitive and motor impairment in healthy individuals. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 344–349.PubMed
go back to reference Welch, R. B. (1978). Perceptual modification: Adapting to altered sensory environments. New York: Academic Press. Welch, R. B. (1978). Perceptual modification: Adapting to altered sensory environments. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1981). Signal, noise, performance, and age. Human Factors, 23, 97–109.PubMed Welford, A. T. (1981). Signal, noise, performance, and age. Human Factors, 23, 97–109.PubMed
go back to reference West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–292.CrossRefPubMed West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–292.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.CrossRefPubMed Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., & Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 338–347.CrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., & Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 338–347.CrossRef
go back to reference Yan, J. H., Thomas, J. R., & Stelmach, G. E. (1998). Aging and rapid aiming arm movement control. Experimental Aging Research, 24, 155–168.CrossRefPubMed Yan, J. H., Thomas, J. R., & Stelmach, G. E. (1998). Aging and rapid aiming arm movement control. Experimental Aging Research, 24, 155–168.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Adaptation to a direction-dependent visuomotor gain in the young and elderly
Auteurs
Mathias Hegele
Herbert Heuer
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0221-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2010

Psychological Research 1/2010 Naar de uitgave