Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2010

01-01-2010 | Original Article

Having to identify a target reduces latencies in prosaccades but not in antisaccades

Auteurs: Nathalie Guyader, Jennifer Malsert, Christian Marendaz

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In a princeps study, Trottier and Pratt (2005) showed that saccadic latencies were dramatically reduced when subjects were instructed to not simply look at a peripheral target (reflexive saccade) but to identify some of its properties. According to the authors, the shortening of saccadic reactions times may arise from a top-down disinhibition of the superior colliculus (SC), potentially mediated by the direct pathway connecting frontal/prefrontal cortex structures to the SC. Using a “cue paradigm” (a cue preceded the appearance of the target), the present study tests if the task instruction (Identify vs. Glance) also reduces the latencies of antisaccades (AS), which involve prefrontal structures. We show that instruction reduces latencies for prosaccade but not for AS. An AS requires two processes: the inhibition of a reflexive saccade and the generation of a voluntary saccade. To separate these processes and to better understand the task effect we also test the effect of the task instruction only on voluntary saccades. The effect still exists but it is much weaker than for reflexive saccades. The instruction effect closely depends on task demands in executive resources.
Literatuur
go back to reference Brown, M. R. G., Goltz, H. G., Vilis, T., Ford, K. A., & Everling, S. (2006). Inhibition and generation of saccades: Rapid event-related fMRI of prosaccades, antisaccades, and nogo trials. Neuroimage, 33, 644–659.CrossRefPubMed Brown, M. R. G., Goltz, H. G., Vilis, T., Ford, K. A., & Everling, S. (2006). Inhibition and generation of saccades: Rapid event-related fMRI of prosaccades, antisaccades, and nogo trials. Neuroimage, 33, 644–659.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carpenter, R. H. S. (1988). Movements of the eyes. London: Pion. Carpenter, R. H. S. (1988). Movements of the eyes. London: Pion.
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.CrossRefPubMed Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crevits, L., Vandierendonck, A., Stuyven, E., Verschaete, S., & Wildenbeest, J. (2004). Effect of intention and visual fixation disengagement on prosaccades in Parkinson’s disease patients. Neuropsychologia, 42, 624–632.CrossRefPubMed Crevits, L., Vandierendonck, A., Stuyven, E., Verschaete, S., & Wildenbeest, J. (2004). Effect of intention and visual fixation disengagement on prosaccades in Parkinson’s disease patients. Neuropsychologia, 42, 624–632.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Valois, R. L., & De Valois, K. K. (1988). Spatial Vision. New York: Oxford University Press. De Valois, R. L., & De Valois, K. K. (1988). Spatial Vision. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Dorris, M. C., & Munoz, D. P. (1995). A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73(6), 2558–2562.PubMed Dorris, M. C., & Munoz, D. P. (1995). A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73(6), 2558–2562.PubMed
go back to reference Fischer, B., & Weber, H. (1993). Express saccades and visual attention. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 553–567.CrossRef Fischer, B., & Weber, H. (1993). Express saccades and visual attention. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 553–567.CrossRef
go back to reference Guitton, D., Buchtel, H. A., & Douglas, R. M. (1985). Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 58, 455–472.CrossRef Guitton, D., Buchtel, H. A., & Douglas, R. M. (1985). Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 58, 455–472.CrossRef
go back to reference Ikeda, T., & Hikosaka, O. (2003). Reward-dependant gain and bias of visual responses in primate superior colliculus. Neuron, 39, 693–700.CrossRefPubMed Ikeda, T., & Hikosaka, O. (2003). Reward-dependant gain and bias of visual responses in primate superior colliculus. Neuron, 39, 693–700.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ikeda, T., & Hikosaka, O. (2007). Positive and negative modulation of motor response in primate superior colliculus by reward expectation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98, 3163–3170.CrossRefPubMed Ikeda, T., & Hikosaka, O. (2007). Positive and negative modulation of motor response in primate superior colliculus by reward expectation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98, 3163–3170.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Marendaz, C., Guyader, N., Malsert, J., Peyrin, C., & Charnallet, A. (in revision). Cognitive instruction releases alexic eyes: Having to identify a target causes fast express saccades, even in the case of alexia/agnosia. Neuropsychologia. Marendaz, C., Guyader, N., Malsert, J., Peyrin, C., & Charnallet, A. (in revision). Cognitive instruction releases alexic eyes: Having to identify a target causes fast express saccades, even in the case of alexia/agnosia. Neuropsychologia.
go back to reference Mayfrank, L., Mobashery, M., Kimming, L., & Fischer, B. (1986). The role of fixation and visual attention in the occurrence of express saccades in man. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 235(5), 269–275.CrossRefPubMed Mayfrank, L., Mobashery, M., Kimming, L., & Fischer, B. (1986). The role of fixation and visual attention in the occurrence of express saccades in man. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 235(5), 269–275.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Montagnini, A., & Chelazzi, L. (2005). The urgency to look: Prompt saccades to the benefit of perception. Vision Research, 45, 3391–3401.CrossRefPubMed Montagnini, A., & Chelazzi, L. (2005). The urgency to look: Prompt saccades to the benefit of perception. Vision Research, 45, 3391–3401.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Munoz, P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look away: The anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 218–228.CrossRefPubMed Munoz, P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look away: The anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 218–228.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., De Jong, R., Kok, A., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2000). Inhibitory inefficiency and failures of intention activation: Age-related decline in the control of saccadic eye movements. Psychology and Aging, 15, 635–647.CrossRefPubMed Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., De Jong, R., Kok, A., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2000). Inhibitory inefficiency and failures of intention activation: Age-related decline in the control of saccadic eye movements. Psychology and Aging, 15, 635–647.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. H., Milea, D., & Muri, R. M. (2004). Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex. Current Opinion in Neurology, 17(1), 17–25.CrossRefPubMed Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. H., Milea, D., & Muri, R. M. (2004). Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex. Current Opinion in Neurology, 17(1), 17–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. H., Muri, R. M., Nyffeler, T., & Milea, D. (2005). The role of the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039, 239–251.CrossRefPubMed Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. H., Muri, R. M., Nyffeler, T., & Milea, D. (2005). The role of the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039, 239–251.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1992). Eye movements and scene perception. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46(3), 342–376. Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1992). Eye movements and scene perception. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46(3), 342–376.
go back to reference Reddi, B. A. J., Asrress, K. N., & Carpenter, R. H. S. (2003). Accuracy, information and response time in saccadic decision task. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(5), 3538–3546.CrossRefPubMed Reddi, B. A. J., Asrress, K. N., & Carpenter, R. H. S. (2003). Accuracy, information and response time in saccadic decision task. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(5), 3538–3546.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Reddi, B. A. J., & Carpenter, R. H. S. (2000). The influence of urgency on decision time. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 827–830.CrossRefPubMed Reddi, B. A. J., & Carpenter, R. H. S. (2000). The influence of urgency on decision time. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 827–830.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effect of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency of saccadic eye movements. J Opt Soc Am, 57, 1024–1029.CrossRefPubMed Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effect of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency of saccadic eye movements. J Opt Soc Am, 57, 1024–1029.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schiller, P. H., True, S. D., & Conway, J. L. (1980). Deficits in eye movements following frontal eye field and superior colliculus ablations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 44, 1175–1189.PubMed Schiller, P. H., True, S. D., & Conway, J. L. (1980). Deficits in eye movements following frontal eye field and superior colliculus ablations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 44, 1175–1189.PubMed
go back to reference Séré, B., Marendaz, C., & Hérault, J. (2000). Non-homogeneous resolution of images of natural scenes. Perception, 29, 1403–1412.CrossRefPubMed Séré, B., Marendaz, C., & Hérault, J. (2000). Non-homogeneous resolution of images of natural scenes. Perception, 29, 1403–1412.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shadmehr, R., & Krakauer, J. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 359–381.CrossRef Shadmehr, R., & Krakauer, J. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 359–381.CrossRef
go back to reference Sumner, P., Nachev, P., Castor-Perry, S., Isenman, H., & Kennard, C. (2006). Which visual pathways cause fixation-related inhibition? Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 1527–1536.CrossRefPubMed Sumner, P., Nachev, P., Castor-Perry, S., Isenman, H., & Kennard, C. (2006). Which visual pathways cause fixation-related inhibition? Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 1527–1536.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Trottier, L., & Pratt, J. (2005). Visual processing of targets can reduce saccadic latencies. Vision Research, 45, 1349–1354.CrossRefPubMed Trottier, L., & Pratt, J. (2005). Visual processing of targets can reduce saccadic latencies. Vision Research, 45, 1349–1354.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Having to identify a target reduces latencies in prosaccades but not in antisaccades
Auteurs
Nathalie Guyader
Jennifer Malsert
Christian Marendaz
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0218-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2010

Psychological Research 1/2010 Naar de uitgave