Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 8/2021

Open Access 05-02-2021 | Original Paper

Implicit Semantic Processing of Linguistic and Non-linguistic Stimuli in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Auteurs: Emme O’Rourke, Emily L. Coderre

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 8/2021

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

While many individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) experience difficulties with language processing, non-linguistic semantic processing may be intact. We examined neural responses to an implicit semantic priming task by comparing N400 responses—an event-related potential related to semantic processing—in response to semantically related or unrelated pairs of words or pictures. Adults with ASD showed larger N400 responses than typically developing adults for pictures, but no group differences occurred for words. However, we also observed complex modulations of N400 amplitude by age and by level of autistic traits. These results offer important implications for how groups are delineated and compared in autism research.
Opmerkingen

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
The rate of diagnosis for autism spectrum disorders (ASD) has increased over the last decade, with the most recent estimate to be one in every 54 children with the diagnosis (Center for Disease Control and Prevention 2020). This group of neurodevelopmental disorders is characterized by deficits in social communication and restricted and repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Association 2013). Although the severity of autistic traits can vary between individuals both with and without an ASD diagnosis, many individuals with ASD experience difficulties with language, specifically in higher-level functions such as semantic processing: understanding of the meaning of a stimulus, regardless of the mode of presentation (e.g., word, picture, or sound) (Coderre et al. 2017). Difficulties with language processing in individuals with ASD may be particularly tied, at least in part, to difficulties with semantic integration, which involves not just accessing the semantics of stimuli but assimilating meanings together to arrive at a holistic understanding or a “gist”.
One way of quantifying semantic integration is through event related potentials (ERPs), which are derived from the electroencephalogram (EEG). In previous research with typically developing (TD) populations, the N400 component of the ERP, a negative-going deflection peaking approximately 400 ms after stimulus presentation, has been linked to semantic processing and integration (Kutas and Hillyard 1980; Lau et al. 2008), although a number of other functional interpretations exist (e.g., Brouwer et al. 2012; see Kutas and Federmeier 2011 for a broader discussion). The amplitude of the N400 component is typically reduced when a sentence-final word is semantically congruent with the preceding context of the sentence. Consider the sentences, “the bird has yellow feathers” and “the bird has yellow shoes.” The word ‘feathers’ is semantically congruent with the beginning of the sentence, while ‘shoes’ is incongruent. The difference in N400 amplitude between the congruent and incongruent conditions here is termed the “N400 effect.” The same modulation of N400 amplitude occurs for single words when the two words are related (e.g., bird-FEATHERS) compared to when they are unrelated (e.g., bird-SHOES).
Several studies have reported that individuals with ASD show reduced or absent N400 effects for language compared to TD individuals, suggesting impairments in semantic integration (Braeutigam et al. 2008; Dunn et al. 1999; Dunn and Bates 2005; McCleery et al. 2010; Pijnacker et al. 2010; Strandburg et al. 1993; Verbaten et al. 1991). These findings have been explained in light of the theory of weak central coherence (Frith 1989), which proposes that individuals with ASD have a tendency to focus on details at the expense of the broader picture. Therefore difficulties with language processing in ASD may arise from a reduced use of semantic context to process the meanings of words. Difficulties with semantic integration have been documented for pairs of single words (e.g. Dunn et al. 1999; McCleery et al. 2010) and are likely compounded with increasing complexity such as in sentences (e.g. Braeutigam et al. 2008; Pijnacker et al. 2010) or narratives (Coderre et al. 2018).
In contrast to these difficulties with semantic integration of language, there have also been reports that semantic integration of non-verbal stimuli like pictures or sounds is intact in individuals with ASD (Kamio and Toichi 2000; McCleery et al. 2010), which might suggest a language-specific difficulty with semantic integration. However, these previous studies used cross-modal stimuli (i.e. combinations of words and pictures), making it difficult to determine whether a true language-specific difficulty with semantic processing exists in this population. In previous work, we performed the first within-modality test of semantic integration in linguistic and non-linguistic modalities (Coderre et al. 2017). In a semantic priming ERP paradigm, adults with ASD and TD adults were shown pairs of semantically related or unrelated words or pictures. As reported by prior literature, there were no differences in N400 effect magnitudes (i.e. amplitude differences between related and unrelated pairs) for picture stimuli, suggesting intact semantic processing of non-linguistic information in individuals with ASD. Surprisingly, both groups also showed similar magnitudes of N400 effects in response to word stimuli, which contradicts previous reports of impaired semantic processing of linguistic stimuli in individuals with ASD (Braeutigam et al. 2008; Dunn and Bates 2005; Dunn et al. 1999; McCleery et al. 2010; Pijnacker et al. 2010; Strandburg et al. 1993; Verbaten et al. 1991). We speculated in this previous study that this surprising result could have arisen from the use of an explicit task: participants were asked to judge whether each pair of pictures or words was related or unrelated. Although individuals with ASD are thought to have a reduced tendency to process information in its larger context coherence (Frith 1989), explicit instruction to attend to the semantic relationships between stimuli may overcome this tendency (Koldewyn et al. 2013). Therefore the use of an explicit task may have introduced compensatory strategies in ASD adults, allowing them to overcome their natural tendencies and leading to successful semantic integration in both modalities.
If this is the case, then an implicit semantic integration task, in which attention is not overtly drawn to the semantic relationships between stimuli, may be more likely to reveal a language-specific deficit in individuals with ASD. We explore this possibility in the current study, which aims to replicate our previous work (Coderre et al. 2017) with an implicit semantic priming task to determine whether semantic processing difficulties are observed when attention is not drawn to the semantic relatedness of stimuli. Adults with and without ASD viewed words and pictures that were either semantically related or unrelated and were told to monitor for “catch” trials (a string of consonants or a smiley face). That is, participants viewed pairs of related and unrelated stimuli but were not explicitly told about or instructed to attend to the relatedness of the stimuli. In this way, this type of paradigm should pick up on more implicit, “automatic” semantic processing. This type of implicit paradigm has been used in work with children with and without ASD (McCleery et al. 2010) and has previously shown to elicit N400 effects for TD participants in the absence of an explicit task. Therefore we expected that it would provide a sensitive measure of the degree to which semantic integration was triggered in the absence of overt instruction.
We predicted that both groups would show similar N400 effects for sematic processing of pictures, replicating previous findings that semantic processing of non-linguistic stimuli is intact in individuals with ASD. In contrast, if individuals with ASD have a reduced tendency to automatically and implicitly process the semantic properties of language, we would expect reduced N400 responses to words compared to TD individuals.

Methods

Participants

One group consisted of 20 adults with ASD, ages 18–54 (M = 27.8, SD = 9.5). The clinical diagnosis of autism or ASD (according to the DSM-IV or DSM-5, depending on the recency of the diagnosis/evaluation) was confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Second Edition (ADOS-2) (Lord et al. 2012). A second group consisted of 20 typically developing (TD) adults ages 19–49 (M = 25.2, SD = 7.57). The groups did not differ on age; however, the ASD group had significantly lower verbal and non-verbal IQ, as measured by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition (KBIT-2; Kaufman and Kaufman 2004). There was also a trend toward lower receptive vocabulary knowledge in the ASD group, as measured by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Fourth Edition (PPVT-4; Dunn and Dunn 2007). Finally, the groups also differed significantly on working memory, as assessed by a digit span task. All participants also completed the Autism Quotient (AQ), a self-report measure of autistic traits that is applicable for the general population (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). As expected, the groups differed significantly on AQ scores, with higher scores in the TD group. Full participant demographics can be found in Table 1.
Table 1
Participant characteristics for the TD and ASD groups
 
TD group (n = 20)
ASD group (n = 20)
Group difference
Age
25.2 (19–49)
27.8 (18–54)
t(36.3) = − 0.96, p = 0.34
PPVT
115 (98–135)
108 (80–132)
t(33.2) = 1.89, p = 0.067
K-BIT
 Verbal
118 (101–141)
107 (71–135)
t(33.3) = 2.54, p < 0.05
 Non-verbal
112 (74–132)
101 (79–120)
t(37.5) = 2.38, p < 0.05
Autism Quotient
15.5 (5–37)
30.1 (17–43)
t(37.5) = -6.28, p < 0.001
Digit span
12 (7–16)
10 (4–16)
t(36.6) = 2.99, p < 0.01
WRATa
 Word reading
111 (107–126)
108 (87–135)
t(18.4) = 0.70, p = 0.49
 Sentence comprehension
119 (108–130)
104 (82–131)
t(16.8) = 2.95, p < 0.01
ADOS Module 4
 Social + communication totalb
NA
9.3 (6–16)
NA
 SA + RBB totalc
12.3 (4–7)
 Calibrated severity score (CSS)d
6.6 (3–10)
Means and ranges are reported for each measure. The ‘group difference’ column shows the results of independent-samples t-tests on each measure. Asterisks indicate statistically significant results (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001)
aBased on scores from 9/20 TD participants and 13/20 ASD participants
bThe Social + communication total score is taken from the original algorithm for Module 4 of the ADOS-2, which is included in the scoring forms
cSA social affect, RBB restricted and repetitive behaviors. The SA + RBB total score is based off of a revised algorithm for Module 4 (Hus and Lord 2014), which was not included in the original release of the ADOS-2
dIn generating Calibrated Severity Scores (CSS) for Module 4, only participants ages 9–39 were included by Hus and Lord (2014). No calibrated scores were provided for participants older than 40, which means that technically we cannot calculate CSS for our older participants. In our sample, three participants were over 40. When considering all participants under age 40 (n = 17), the average CSS was 6.9 (range 4–10)
All participants were able to read to an adequate degree to perform the word version of the task. All participants independently read the consent form and completed other questionnaires administered as part of this study and did not indicate any difficulty with understanding. We also obtained standardized measures of word reading and sentence comprehension (as assessed via the Wide Range Achievement Test, version 4; WRAT-4 Wilkinson and Robertson 2006) as part of a different study; although not all participants completed this assessment, the results from this task also confirmed that participants were able to read. As shown in Table 1, there were no differences in word reading scores between TD and ASD participants. The ASD group did show significantly lower scores on the sentence comprehension subtest compared to TD participants (p < 0.01), as would be expected based on the previously mentioned challenges with comprehension in this group. However, the scores for ASD participants for both word reading and sentence comprehension were all within the “normal” range (all above 70, i.e. within 2 standard deviations, SDs), confirming that their reading abilities were sufficiently advanced to read and understand the words presented as part of this paradigm.
All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants were recruited through newspaper advertisements, email announcements, and fliers at the University of Vermont and the Burlington, VT community. All procedures were approved by the University of Vermont Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before testing. Participants were monetarily compensated for their participation.

Stimuli and Procedure

The experimental paradigm used the same pairs of related and unrelated words and pictures as in our previous study (Coderre et al. 2017). Semantic relatedness was assessed using latent semantic analysis (LSA; mean LSA value for related pairs = 0.58; mean LSA value for unrelated pairs = 0.03). See Coderre et al. (2017) for more details regarding stimuli development.
To create an “implicit” paradigm, an additional 16% of trials (16 trials per block) were included as “catch trials” in which one stimulus was either a smiley face (for picture blocks) or a consonant string (for word blocks); see Fig. 1. Participants were asked to press a button on a button box whenever they saw a smiley face or a consonant string). Such catch trials have been used in prior research (McCleery et al. 2010) to ensure that participants attend to all stimuli. Note that although it includes a task, this paradigm assesses implicit semantic integration because participants are not asked to attend to the semantic relationship between the stimuli.
The implicit semantic priming task consisted of four blocks of picture pairs and four blocks of word pairs (25 related pairs, 25 unrelated pairs, 16 catch trials per block) for a total of 100 trials in each of the four stimuli types (picture related, picture unrelated, word related, word unrelated) plus 64 catch trials in each modality. Participants performed all four blocks of one modality followed by all four blocks of the other, counterbalanced across participants.
Stimuli were presented using E-Prime version 2.0.10.356. On each trial, a red fixation cross was presented for 400 ms, followed by the first word or picture for 1000 ms; an inter-stimulus blank screen for 300 ms; and the second word or picture for 1000 ms. Following the offset of the second stimulus a blank screen was presented for 400 ms, followed by a black fixation cross presented at an inter-trial interval ranging from 1000 to 1400 ms (mean 1200 ms).
Stimuli were presented on a Dell 21.5″ LCD monitor with a resolution of 1920 × 1080. Participants sat approximately 24″ away from the computer screen. Picture size ranged from 2.25 to 4″ in height and 2.5 to 4″ in width, yielding a visual angle between 5 and 9 degrees. Word stimuli were presented in size 28 Courier New font and ranged from 0.6 to 2.25″ in width and 0.25″ in height, yielding a visual angle between 1 and 5 degrees horizontally and 0.6 degrees vertically. During the semantic priming task, EEG data were recorded at 500 Hz using a 128-channel Geodesics Sensor net and NetStation version 5.3. Impedences were kept under 50 kΩ wherever possible. The entire experimental session lasted approximately 1.5 h including consenting, paperwork, EEG net application, and testing.

Data Preprocessing

Data were preprocessed using EEGlab version 14.1.1 and Matlab 2017a. The data were filtered using a 0.1–50 Hz bandpass filter to remove very high- or low-frequency noise in the signal not related to brain activity. The data were then re-referenced using an average reference transform in order to express the voltage at each channel with respect to the average voltage across all channels (Dien 1998). The cleaned continuous data were then segmented into epochs time-locked to the onset of the second stimulus. Segments extended from 1400 ms before to 1000 ms after the second stimulus (in order to capture the response to the first stimulus, presented at -1300 ms). Eye movement artifacts were identified and removed from the data using independent component analysis (ICA). Prior to ICA decomposition, the mean of each trial was removed (Groppe et al. 2009) and the data were reduced to 32 dimensions. After ICA decomposition, eye movements, blinks, muscle artifacts, and other noise components were visually identified and manually removed from the data.

Statistical Analyses

ERP amplitude was evaluated at 9 regions across the scalp taken from the 10–20 distribution. Clusters were centered around F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, and P4 (Fig. 2). Since catch trials were relatively rare (16%) they likely elicited a P300 component that could confound N400 effects; therefore only experimental (i.e. non-catch) trials were included in analyses.
Because the N400 may occur at slightly different latencies between groups or modalities (e.g. Hamm et al. 2002; McPherson and Holcomb 1999; West and Holcomb 2002), we chose not to restrict our analyses to one predefined time window. We defined long time windows of interest based on visual inspection and then performed repeated-measure ANOVAs in 100 ms windows. ANOVAs were performed on the unrelated-related difference waves within each modality. Group (TD/ASD) was included as a between-subjects factor and site (frontal/central/parietal) and laterality (left/midline/right) were included as within-subjects factors. Because of group differences in the demographic variables, verbal and non-verbal K-BIT scores, PPVT scores, and digit span scores were included as covariates in all ANOVAs.
Although verbal and non-verbal IQ, receptive language scores, and digit span scores were included as covariates in the statistical analyses, there is the possibility that other demographic variables may have modulated ERP effects. Therefore in secondary analyses, we reran the ANOVAs described above with two modifications: using AQ score as a continuous variable instead of TD/ASD groups and also including age as a continuous variable. As can be seen in Table 1, our participants had quite a range of AQ scores, with notable overlap between the groups. One benefit of the AQ measure is that it can provide an estimate of autistic traits in the general population (Ruzich et al. 2015), since TD participants completed it as well. This assessment also provides a continuous measure of autistic traits, which allows us to explore finer-grained modulations of how autistic traits affect ERP responses compared to dichotomous grouping by the presence of an ASD diagnosis. In addition, as we have suggested elsewhere (Coderre et al. 2017), semantic integration abilities may also be mitigated by age; since we had a fairly wide range of ages in the current sample, we examined whether this variable might affect ERP responses, as well as how it might interact with AQ score. In summary, the ANOVAs for these secondary analyses were run on the unrelated-related difference waves in each modality and included AQ score and age as continuous variables, and site (frontal/central/parietal) and laterality (left/midline/right) as within-subjects factors, with interaction terms between all factors. Verbal and non-verbal K-BIT scores, PPVT scores, and digit span scores were included as covariates. These ANOVAs were run in the same analysis windows as specified above (100 ms windows from 200 to 800 ms). To interpret interactions between AQ score and age, scatterplots were created with regression lines for low-AQ and high-AQ participants (as determined by a median split: median AQ score = 21.5).

Results

Pictures

Visual inspection of the ERP waveforms for the picture condition suggested that in the TD group (Fig. 3), the unrelated condition was more negative than the related condition over frontal sites from approximately 400–500 ms. This more frontal N400 effect is commonly seen for visual stimuli (Ganis et al. 1996; Hamm et al. 2002; McPherson and Holcomb 1999; Sitnikova et al. 2008), in contrast to the more centro-parietal scalp distribution of the N400 in response to linguistic stimuli (Kutas and Federmeier 2011). This effect was flipped over parietal sites, such that the unrelated condition was more positive than the related condition. The ASD group showed a similar pattern (Fig. 4), with unrelated conditions more negative than related conditions over frontal sites from approximately 200–400 ms over right frontal sites, with a more sustained effect from 200 to 1000 ms over left and midline frontal clusters. These effects can also be seen in Fig. 5, which plots the unrelated-related difference waves and topographic plots for each group.
To compare group effects in the picture condition, we ran repeated-measures ANOVAs in 100 ms time windows from 200 to 800 ms after presentation of the second stimulus. Full results are presented in Table 2; only significant (p < 0.05) main effects of group or interactions with group are discussed below.
Table 2
F-values for the repeated-measures ANOVAs on unrelated-related difference waves in each analysis window for the picture condition, with a between-subjects factor of group (TD, ASD) and within-subjects factors of site (frontal, central, parietal) and laterality (left, midline, right)
Main effect or interaction
200–300 ms
300–400 ms
400–500 ms
500–600 ms
600–700 ms
700–800 ms
PPVT
4.01
2.56
5.38 *
7.63 *
7.21 *
7.02 *
KBIT verbal
0.01
0.30
1.28
0.11
0.01
0.20
KBIT nonverbal
1.11
0.04
0.03
0.25
 < 0.01
0.19
Digit span
0.55
0.76
0.56
1.58
0.58
0.92
Site
12.61 ***
8.31 ***
7.96 ***
5.03 **
4.83 *
1.12
Laterality
0.88
1.70
1.75
1.11
0.79
0.87
Group
6.59 *
2.22
2.33
1.43
1.96
1.36
Group × site
2.72
3.56 *
0.90
0.22
0.61
0.32
Group × laterality
0.15
0.17
0.21
1.03
0.58
2.07
Site × laterality
0.41
0.30
0.34
0.29
0.32
0.18
Group × site × laterality
1.30
1.67
1.01
0.99
2.30
3.07 *
Verbal and non-verbal K-BIT, PPVT, and digit span scores were included as covariates. Asterisks indicate statistically significant results (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Main effects of group and interactions with group are highlighted in bold
From 200 to 300 ms there was a significant main effect of group (F(1,34) = 6.59, p < 0.05) such that, over all electrode clusters, the ASD difference wave (M = − 0.07, SD = 0.55) was more negative than the TD difference wave (M = 0.04, SD = 0.51), indicating larger N400 effects in the ASD group than in the TD group (Fig. 5). To explore whether the relatedness effect was significant in either group individually, we also ran a repeated-measures ANOVA in each group with levels of condition (related/unrelated), site (frontal/central/parietal), and laterality (left/midline/right) in this time window. In the TD group there were no significant main effects of or interactions with condition (all p’s > 0.11). However, in the ASD group there was a significant interaction of condition and site (F(2,38) = 11.17, p < 0.001) such that over frontal sites the unrelated condition was more negative than the related condition, reflecting the more frontal N400 found for pictures (Ganis et al. 1996; Hamm et al. 2002; McPherson and Holcomb 1999; Sitnikova et al. 2008), whereas this effect was flipped over parietal sites, such that related was more negative than unrelated, reflecting the flipped dipole. Therefore the ASD group showed a significant N400 effect from 200 to 300 ms, particularly over frontal sites, but the TD group did not.
From 300 to 400 ms there was a significant interaction of group and site (F(2,76) = 3.56, p < 0.05). This arose from a significant effect of group at frontal sites (F(1,114) = 8.79, p < 0.01) such that the ASD difference wave (M = − 0.35, SD = 0.60) was more negative than the TD difference wave (M = − 0.07, SD = 0.52), indicating larger N400 effects in the ASD group than in the TD group (Fig. 5). To explore whether the relatedness effect was significant in either group individually, we also ran a repeated-measures ANOVA in each group with levels of condition, site, and laterality in this time window. In the TD group there were no significant main effects of or interactions with condition (all p’s > 0.32). However, in the ASD group there was a significant interaction of condition and site (F(2,38) = 8.54, p < 0.001) such that over frontal sites the unrelated condition was more negative than the related condition, whereas this effect was flipped at parietal sites. Therefore the ASD group showed a significant N400 effect over frontal sites from 300 to 400 ms. whereas the TD group did not.
From 700 to 800 ms there was a significant interaction of group, site, and laterality (F(4,152) = 3.07, p < 0.05) which arose from a trend of a main effect of group at frontal midline sites, i.e. the Fz cluster (F(1,34) = 3.01, p = 0.09). At this electrode cluster, the ASD difference wave (M = − 0.28, SD = 0.53) was more negative than the TD difference wave (M = − 0.02, SD = 0.47), indicating larger N400 effects in the ASD group than in the TD group. To explore whether the relatedness effect was significant in either group individually, we also ran a repeated-measures ANOVA in each group with levels of condition, site, and laterality in this time window. In the TD group there were no main effects of or interactions with condition (all p’s > 0.21). However, in the ASD group there was a significant interaction of condition and laterality (F(2,38) = 3.36, p < 0.05), which arose from trends of an effect of condition at midline (F(1,19) = 3.25, p = 0.09) and right hemisphere sites (F(1,19) = 3.94, p = 0.06). Over midline clusters the unrelated condition was more negative (M = 0.04, SD = 0.95) than the related condition (M = 0.11, SD = 0.84) whereas this effect was flipped over right hemisphere clusters, in which the related condition (M = − 0.06, SD = 0.88) was more negative than the related condition (M = 0.05, SD = 0.88. Therefore the ASD group showed a significant relatedness effect at midline electrode clusters, particularly at frontal sites, but a flipped effect over right-hemisphere clusters, whereas the TD group did not show any modulations of amplitude by relatedness.
As described above (see Statistical Analyses section), we also ran secondary analyses exploring how degree of autistic traits and age, and their potential interactions, might affect the results. To do so we ran repeated-measures ANOVAs on the unrelated-related difference waves in each of the specified analysis windows; AQ score and age were included as continuous variables, site and laterality were included as within-subjects variables, and verbal and non-verbal K-BIT scores, PPVT scores, and digit span scores were included as covariates. Visualization scatterplots were created to interpret interactions with age and AQ. The full results can be found in Table 3; only significant (p < 0.05) main effects of and interactions with AQ and age are discussed below.
Table 3
F-values for the repeated-measures ANOVAs on unrelated-related difference waves in each analysis window for the picture condition, with AQ score and age as continuous variables and within-subjects factors of site (frontal, central, parietal) and laterality (left, midline, right)
Main effect or interaction
200–300 ms
300–400 ms
400–500 ms
500–600 ms
600–700 ms
700–800 ms
PPVT
3.73
2.46
5.21 *
8.25 **
7.49 *
7.44 *
KBIT verbal
0.01
0.29
1.24
0.12
0.01
0.21
KBIT nonverbal
1.03
0.04
0.03
0.28
 < 0.01
0.20
Digit span
0.51
0.73
0.54
1.71
0.60
0.97
Age
0.54
0.50
0.46
0.77
0.20
 < 0.01
AQ
0.22
0.21
0.40
1.21
0.44
0.27
Age × AQ
5.03 *
2.10
2.33
4.36 *
4.76 *
5.15 *
Site
11.61 ***
7.47 ***
7.52 **
4.92 **
4.63 *
1.19
Age × site
1.11
1.01
0.62
1.35
0.51
4.12 *
AQ × site
0.20
0.05
0.04
0.03
 < 0.01
0.13
Age × AQ × site
0.18
0.31
0.09
0.01
0.51
0.22
Laterality
0.94
1.76
1.77
1.06
0.74
0.83
Age × laterality
4.12 *
2.60
2.05
0.07
0.20
0.61
AQ × laterality
0.16
0.07
0.35
0.13
0.04
1.33
Age × AQ × laterality
0.54
1.03
0.13
1.08
0.25
0.66
Site × laterality
0.41
0.30
0.34
0.29
0.31
0.19
Age × site × laterality
1.07
0.78
0.45
0.26
0.27
0.33
AQ × site × laterality
0.77
0.93
0.94
1.25
1.80
2.51 *
Age × AQ × site × laterality
0.88
1.02
1.68
2.19
1.43
2.67 *
Verbal and non-verbal K-BIT, PPVT, and digit span scores were included as covariates. Asterisks indicate statistically significant results (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Main effects of and interactions with AQ and age are highlighted in bold
From 200 to 300 ms there was an interaction of age and laterality (F(2,72) = 4.12, p < 0.05). However, on follow-up, there were no main effects of AQ at any laterality.
From 200 to 300 ms, 500–600 ms, and 600–700 ms, there were interactions of age and AQ (all p’s < 0.05). In all time windows, visualization scatterplots indicated that increasing age was associated with larger N400 effects (such that unrelated conditions were more negative than related conditions) in participants with lower AQ scores, but the opposite trend (such that increasing age was associated with smaller N400 effects) happened in those with high AQ scores.
From 700 to 800 ms there was an interaction of age, AQ, site, and hemisphere (F(4,144) = 2.66, p < 0.05), arising from an interaction of age and AQ at left and midline central sites, i.e. the C3 (F(1,32) = 4.77, p < 0.05) and Cz clusters (F(1,32) = 7.20, p < 0.05). Visualization scatterplots at each electrode cluster (Fig. 6) showed that at both sites, increasing age was associated with larger N400 effects (such that unrelated conditions were more negative than related conditions) in the low AQ group. This effect was flipped in high AQ participants, such that increasing age was associated with smaller N400 effects.
To summarize the findings in the picture modality, when grouping by TD/ASD designations the ASD group showed a significantly larger N400 effect (i.e. a more negative unrelated-related difference wave) in response to pictures than the TD group, particularly over frontal sites, from 200 to 400 ms and from 700 to 800 ms. When including AQ score (as a measure of autistic traits in the general population) and age as continuous variables in the analysis models, we also found complex interactions between these factors and the magnitude of the N400 effect. At both early (i.e. 200–300 ms) and late (500–800 ms) time windows, those with lower levels of autistic traits (i.e. lower AQ scores) showed increasing sensitivity to semantic relatedness of picture stimuli (i.e. larger N400 effect magnitude) with increasing age, whereas those with higher levels of autistic traits showed decreasing sensitivity to semantic relatedness. In most time windows, this effect was generalized over all electrode sites; from 700 to 800 ms, this effect was most prominent over left and midline central electrodes.

Words

Visual inspection of the ERP waveforms for the word condition suggested that in the TD group (Fig. 7) the unrelated condition was more negative than the related condition at central and right-hemisphere central sites from approximately 400–500 ms and at left frontal sites from approximately 500–700 ms. There was also an opposite effect, such that the unrelated condition was more positive than the related condition, at left parietal sites from approximately 500–600 ms and at right parietal sites from approximately 850–950 ms. In the ASD group (Fig. 8), the unrelated condition was more negative than the related condition at left-hemisphere sites, particularly over frontal scalp, from approximately 300–600 ms. This effect was flipped in the right hemisphere, particularly at right parietal sites, from approximately 300–500 ms.
To compare group effects in the word condition, we ran repeated-measures ANOVAs in 100 ms time windows from 200 to 800 ms after presentation of the second stimulus (Table 4). However, there were no significant main effects of group or interactions with group in any time windows. This suggests that in the word modality there were no significant differences in N400 effects between the groups. This effect can also be seen in Fig. 9, which plots the unrelated-related difference waves and topographic plots for each group.
Table 4
F-values for the repeated-measures ANOVAs on unrelated-related difference waves in each analysis window for the word condition, with a between-subjects factor of group (TD, ASD) and within-subjects factors of site (frontal, central, parietal) and laterality (left, midline, right)
Main effect or interaction
200–300 ms
300–400 ms
400–500 ms
500–600 ms
600–700 ms
700–800 ms
PPVT
 < 0.01
4.63 *
1.94
0.13
4.19 *
3.92
KBIT verbal
0.03
0.08
0.38
0.80
1.71
1.49
KBIT nonverbal
0.06
0.06
0.02
0.76
0.07
1.07
Digit span
2.99
0.03
 < 0.01
 < 0.01
0.73
0.27
Site
0.08
0.33
0.26
0.29
0.86
0.02
Laterality
5.03 **
6.41 **
2.24
1.47
0.26
0.29
Group
0.04
0.28
0.02
 < 0.01
0.87
0.61
Group × site
0.14
0.86
0.33
0.39
0.36
0.41
Group × laterality
1.37
1.43
1.90
0.44
0.36
0.01
Site × laterality
0.90
1.09
1.41
1.41
0.99
1.42
Group × site × laterality
0.41
1.10
1.07
1.35
1.37
0.79
Verbal and non-verbal K-BIT, PPVT, and digit span scores were included as covariates. Asterisks indicate statistically significant results (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Main effects of group and interactions with group are highlighted in bold
To explore whether the unrelated-related condition effects were significant in either group individually, we also ran repeated-measures ANOVAs in each group with levels of condition (related/unrelated), site (frontal/central/parietal), and laterality (left/midline/right) in each time window. In the TD group there were no significant main effects of or interactions with condition in any time window. In the ASD group there were significant interactions of condition by hemisphere from 300 to 400 and 400 to 500 ms (all p’s < 0.05), which arose from a significant N400 effect (unrelated more negative than related) at left hemisphere sites from 300 to 500, with this effect flipped over right hemisphere sites from 300 to 400 ms. Therefore the ASD group showed an N400 effect in response to words from 300 to 500 ms but the TD group did not, although the groups did not differ significantly during direct comparisons.
As described above, we also ran secondary analyses exploring how autistic traits and age, and their potential interactions, might affect results. To do so we ran repeated-measures ANOVAs on the unrelated-related difference waves in each of the specified analysis windows; AQ score and age were included as continuous variables, site and laterality were included as within-subjects variables, and verbal and non-verbal K-BIT scores, PPVT scores, and digit span scores were included as covariates. The full results can be found in Table 5; only significant (p < 0.05) main effects of and interactions with AQ and age are discussed below.
Table 5
F-values for the repeated-measures ANOVAs on unrelated-related difference waves in each analysis window for the word condition, with AQ score and age as continuous variables and within-subjects factors of site (frontal, central, parietal) and laterality (left, midline, right)
Main effect or interaction
200–300 ms
300–400 ms
400–500 ms
500–600 ms
600–700 ms
700–800 ms
PPVT
 < 0.01
4.50 *
1.88
0.13
4.06
3.86
KBIT verbal
0.03
0.08
0.37
0.78
1.67
1.47
KBIT nonverbal
0.05
0.06
0.01
0.74
0.07
1.05
Digit span
2.85
0.03
 < 0.01
 < 0.01
0.71
0.27
Age
0.09
0.50
0.16
0.86
0.69
0.86
AQ
0.31
0.23
0.80
0.06
 < 0.01
0.04
Age × AQ
 < 0.01
0.54
0.01
0.03
1.07
1.25
Site
0.08
0.34
0.26
0.29
0.85
0.02
Age × site
2.49
0.97
0.07
0.84
1.03
1.30
AQ × site
0.15
0.30
0.38
0.97
0.33
0.11
Age × AQ × site
0.16
1.79
2.80
0.16
0.33
0.06
Laterality
4.77 *
6.20 **
2.14
1.44
0.25
0.28
Age × laterality
0.02
0.53
0.42
1.21
0.79
0.57
AQ × laterality
0.45
1.40
1.65
0.02
0.01
0.02
Age × AQ × laterality
0.82
0.19
0.07
0.41
0.02
0.01
Site × laterality
0.96
1.16
1.53
1.51
1.09
1.48
Age × site × laterality
1.75
1.83
1.64
2.19
1.11
1.06
AQ × site × laterality
1.26
1.33
1.21
1.93
2.71 *
0.71
Age × AQ × site × laterality
1.87
2.53 *
3.57 **
2.18
3.54 **
2.76 *
Verbal and non-verbal K-BIT, PPVT, and digit span scores were included as covariates. Asterisks indicate statistically significant results (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Main effects of and interactions with AQ and age are highlighted in bold
From 300 to 500 and 600 to 800 there were significant interactions of age, AQ, site, and laterality.
From 300 to 400 ms, this interaction (F(4,144) = 2.53, p < 0.05) arose from an interaction of age, AQ, and site at midline lateralities (F(2,72) = 3.32, p < 0.05). However, on follow-up, there were no interactions of age and AQ at any site (all p’s > 0.20).
From 400 to 500 ms, the interaction of age, AQ, site, and laterality (F(4,144) = 3.57, p < 0.01) arose from an interaction of age and AQ at left and midline clusters (all p’s < 0.05). In the left hemisphere, follow-up tests showed no interactions of age and AQ at any site (all p’s > 0.20). At midline lateralities, there was a trend of an interaction of age and AQ at frontal sites (F(1,32) = 3.45, p = 0.07), i.e. the Fz cluster. Visualization scatterplots (Fig. 10) showed that at the Fz cluster, participants with lower AQ scores showed decreasing N400 effects (such that unrelated conditions were less negative than related conditions) with increasing age, whereas this was not as strong as for those with higher AQ scores. However, this effect was flipped over parietal sites, particularly the P3 and Pz clusters, such that those with lower AQ scores showed increasing N400 effects with age whereas this relationship was less pronounced for those with higher AQ scores. Because the N400 effect to linguistic stimuli tends to be more centro-parietal (Kutas and Federmeier 2011), and we indeed observed an N400 effect at central sites in the TD group (although not statistically significant), we interpret this parietal relationship as the “typical” N400 effect. Therefore age and AQ seem to interact in similar ways as in the picture condition, with increasing age associated with larger N400 effects (though with different scalp distributions reflecting modality differences in the N400 effect) in those with lower AQ scores, whereas this effect is not as strong in those with higher AQ scores.
From 600 to 700 ms, the interaction of age, AQ, site, and laterality (F(4,144) = 3.54, p < 0.01) arose from an interaction of age and AQ at right frontal sites, i.e. the F4 cluster (F(1,32) = 3.29, p = 0.08). Visualization scatterplots (Fig. 11) showed that at the F4 cluster, participants with lower AQ scores showed increasing N400 effects (such that unrelated conditions were more negative than related conditions) with increasing age, whereas the opposite pattern occurred for those with higher AQ scores. Over most central and parietal clusters, the high and low AQ participants showed similar patterns such that increasing age was associated with larger N400 effects, although this effect was not as strong in the participants with higher AQ scores.
From 700 to 800 ms, there was an interaction of age, AQ, site, and laterality (F(4,144) = 2.76, p < 0.05). However, on follow-up testing there were no significant interactions of age and AQ at any site or laterality.
To summarize the findings in the word modality, when grouping by TD/ASD designations we observed no significant group differences in N400 magnitude. This is somewhat surprising and contradicts previous literature showing that individuals with ASD experience reduced or absent N400 effects to linguistic stimuli. However, when including AQ score and age in the analysis models, we found significant interactions between these variables in the N400 window (400–500 ms). Specifically, in participants with less severe autistic symptoms (i.e. lower AQ scores), sensitivity to semantic relatedness (i.e. N400 effect magnitude) increased with age over centro-parietal sites (reflecting the typical N400 scalp distribution in response to linguistic stimuli; Kutas and Federmeier 2011). This effect was flipped over frontal sites, such that lower AQ scores were associated with decreasing N400 magnitude with age, likely reflecting the flipped dipole of the N400. However, in participants with higher levels of autistic traits, this sensitivity to semantic relatedness was not as strongly modulated by age and did not show as much topographic variation, with fairly similar effects found over all sites. A similar pattern was also observed in a slightly later window from 600 to 700 ms, with the most significant effects coming out over right frontal electrodes such that in participants with lower levels of autistic traits, increasing age was associated with larger N400 effects, whereas this relationship was flipped over centro-parietal sites.

Discussion

This study built on previous work by our group (Coderre et al. 2017) by examining implicit semantic integration in visual and linguistic modalities in individuals with ASD. Adults with and without ASD viewed words and pictures that were either semantically related or unrelated during concurrent EEG recording. Our primary ERP component of interest was the N400 component, an index of semantic processing. We predicted similar N400 effects between groups for sematic processing of pictures, which would support previous findings that semantic processing of non-linguistic stimuli is intact in individuals with ASD. In contrast, we predicted smaller N400 effects for the ASD group for semantic processing of words, which would suggest a reduced tendency to automatically and implicitly process the semantic properties of language in this population.
In the picture condition, both groups appeared to show sustained N400 effects over frontal sites. This longer and more frontally-distributed N400 effect is characteristic of visual stimuli (Coderre et al. 2017, 2018; Cohn et al. 2012; West and Holcomb 2002), so is in line with previous literature. In group comparisons, the ASD group showed significantly larger N400 effects than the TD group, particularly over frontal sites and between 200–400 ms. Although we had predicted that the groups would not differ on the magnitude of their N400 effects for visual stimuli, suggesting intact semantic processing for non-linguistic information as has been reported previously (Coderre et al. 2017; Kamio and Toichi 2000; McCleery et al. 2010), this result of larger N400 effects for the ASD group actually suggests that visuo-semantic processing may be enhanced in individuals with ASD compared to TD individuals. This interpretation would be in line with previous studies suggesting that lower-level perceptual processing is enhanced in individuals with ASD (O’Riordan et al. 2001; O’Riordan and Plaisted 2001) and would fit in with claims in the educational literature that visual supports can be useful in improving reading comprehension in individuals with ASD (Gately 2008; Nguyen et al. 2015; Styslinger 2012). However, we offer this interpretation with the caveat that our potential finding of enhanced visuo-semantic processing in individuals with ASD appears in response to single pictures; comprehension of more complex stimuli like narratives may still be difficult for individuals with ASD, even in visual modalities (Coderre 2020; Coderre et al. 2018).
In the word condition, both groups appeared to show small N400 effects, from 400 to 500 ms over central scalp in the TD group and from 300 to 600 ms over frontal scalp in the ASD group. However, the groups did not differ on the magnitude of their N400 effects, as evidenced by the lack of significant group effects in the overall ANOVAs. Although at first glance this appears to support our previous finding (Coderre et al. 2017) that there were no differences in the magnitude of the N400 effect between ASD and TD groups, upon additional examination in each group individually we actually found the opposite effect as predicted: the ASD group showed a statistically significant N400 effect over left-hemisphere sites from 300 to 500 ms, but the TD group did not show any significant effects of relatedness.
The fact that our TD group did not show an N400 effect in the word modality is curious. One possibility for this finding is that the implicit task we employed here may be very subtle, and not strong enough to elicit reliable N400 effects. For example, visual inspection of the ERP waveforms in Fig. 7 suggested that a small N400 was present, although not statistically significant. However, this type of implicit paradigm has been used in other studies previously (McCleery et al. 2010) and is not dissimilar from many other semantic priming paradigms that do not require overt attention to semantic relatedness. For instance, many semantic priming paradigms require participants to make a lexical decision on the target word and present the prime so fast that it is considered “unconscious” semantic priming (McNamara 2005; Neely 1991). Another possibility may be that, given the sparsity of required responses, participants were not attending to every stimulus. However, behavioral performance on the catch trials was high (average hit rate = 97%), indicating that participants were monitoring the stimuli appropriately and attending to the task. Additional insights may come from our secondary analyses investigating autistic traits as a continuous measure.

Effects of AQ and Age

One possibility for the lack of significant group differences in the word condition when splitting by TD/ASD designation is that we were not adequately capturing the full spectrum of variation in autistic traits. In secondary analyses, we included AQ score (a self-report measure of autistic traits in the general population) in our model, as well as possible interactions of AQ score and age. Although we originally classified our participants according to the presence of an ASD diagnosis, our participant sample included a fairly wide range of AQ scores that spanned diagnostic groups. The continuous nature of this measure (with higher scores indicating higher degrees of autistic traits) also allowed for finer-grained explorations of how autistic traits might affect N400 amplitudes. Age was also included because of the fairly wide age range in our sample and because we had previously postulated that semantic integration abilities may also be mitigated by age, particularly in ASD participants (Coderre et al. 2017). Inclusion of these variables in our analyses revealed complex interactions of age and level of autistic traits that modulated N400 effects in interesting ways.
In the word condition, we observed interactions of age and AQ score in the N400 window (300–500 ms) such that in participants with less severe autistic symptoms (i.e. lower AQ scores), sensitivity to semantic relatedness (i.e. N400 effect magnitude over centro-parietal sites) increased with age. Although the N400 ERP component has been found to get smaller and shift its peak later in older age (Kutas and Iragui 1998), its magnitude is typically greater in adulthood than in early adolescence (Benau et al. 2011), which fits with the age range in our participant sample and the findings in our data. Interestingly, we also observed that although increasing age was associated with greater N400 amplitude, in participants with higher levels of autistic traits (i.e. higher AQ scores) this sensitivity to semantic relatedness was not as strongly modulated by age.
These interactions with age in the word condition, such that younger ages were associated with smaller N400 effects (in participants with both high and low AQ scores, although this effect was stronger in low AQ participants), fits in with an interpretation offered by Coderre et al. (2017), who proposed that individuals with ASD develop compensatory mechanisms as they grow older to make up for their difficulties with semantic processing of language. By this interpretation, Coderre et al. also suggested that larger impairments in semantic processing should be observable in children compared to adults with ASD, since children may have underdeveloped compensatory mechanisms. The relationship observed here between age and N400 effect suggests the same interpretation: that younger age should be associated with smaller N400 effects to language in participants with ASD.
In the picture condition, we observed interactions of age and AQ score at both early (i.e. 200–300 ms) and late (i.e. 500–800 ms) time windows, such that, similar to the pattern observed in the word condition, lower levels of autistic traits (i.e. lower AQ scores) were associated with increasing sensitivity to semantic relatedness of picture stimuli (i.e. larger N400 effect magnitude) with increasing age. In those with higher levels of autistic traits, however, an opposite effect was found between picture and word conditions: for pictures, increasing age was associated with smaller N400 effects, whereas for words, increasing age was associated with larger N400 effects. This could suggest a trade-off effect: that as individuals with more severe autistic symptoms get older, evaluating semantic relatedness gets easier for words but harder for pictures. If, as we have proposed, individuals who struggle with semantic integration develop compensatory strategies over the course of their development, the increased attention and cognitive demands required by such mechanisms may lead to detrimental effects on other aspects of semantic processing, such as in the visual domain. This trade-off effect may be explained by the historic importance of foundational literacy (e.g., reading, writing, and mathematics) in the education system. Although recent trends have shown schools starting to expand students’ understanding of other types of literacy (e.g., social-emotional or health), and although emerging research is showing many similar cognitive processes underlying literacy in different modalities such as language and sequential images (e.g. Coderre et al. 2018; Cohn et al. 2012), foundational literacy has always been the key focus of education. If students are learning and practicing how to fluently decode, comprehend, and express their understanding of words, it would make sense that as adults it is an easier skill than understanding pictures. If this skill is practiced more frequently, it may explain why adults have an easier time understanding the stimuli presented in the word form and not the picture. While this interpretation is speculative, our findings of complex relationships between age, autistic traits, and semantic processing in picture and language modalities offers rich opportunities for future research.

Additional Considerations

Interestingly, the fact that we observed similar patterns of interactions of age and AQ score for both word and picture conditions—such that age modulated N400 effects more strongly in those with lower AQ scores than those with higher AQ scores—suggests that higher levels of autistic traits may be associated with increased difficulty in semantic integration regardless of modality. This finding contradicts the proposal made by others in the literature suggesting that individuals with ASD have a language-specific deficit in semantic integration (Kamio and Toichi 2000; McCleery et al. 2010). In contrast, it suggests that semantic processing difficulties are characteristic of autistic traits, and that these difficulties also extend to non-linguistic modalities. This suggests a domain-general impairment in semantic integration, in line with conclusions made by other studies investigating higher-level comprehension abilities such as narrative processing (Coderre 2020; Coderre et al. 2018). Importantly, the fact that we were able to pick up this relationship between level of autistic traits and semantic processing even with relatively simplistic stimuli—pairs of single pictures and words—suggests that more severe symptoms may be associated with difficulties in integrating even these simple stimuli. In other words, whereas semantic processing difficulties may become most apparent in those with milder symptoms at the sentence or narrative comprehension level and not at the single-word level (in line with proposals of autism as a disorder of more complex information processing: Minshew et al. 1997; Minshew and Goldstein 1998), our findings of relationships between symptom severity and N400 amplitude suggests that individuals who are more severely affected may also have more trouble with simpler processing such as integrating the meanings of single words and pictures. Such difficulties at lower levels of semantic processing would no doubt be compounded at more challenging levels of comprehension such as sentences and narratives.
An additional important point to mention is the utility of the AQ in examining continuous measures of autistic traits across participants. Whereas the “gold standard” diagnostic measure of autism, the ADOS, can be used to obtain severity scores, it is typically only administered to participants in the predefined ASD group; due to the time it takes to administer, it is not often obtained for TD participants. However, the AQ is a useful measure because it is a fairly short self-report survey and can be used to assess autistic symptomology in the general population, including in TD participants (Ruzich et al. 2015). As demonstrated in our data, the AQ also provides a continuous measure of autistic symptomology which can elucidate complex and interesting modulations of results: insights which may not be possible when simply grouping participants into TD/ASD groups. Indeed, in the current data, our initial analyses comparing TD/ASD groups yielded limited insight into the nature of semantic processing of word stimuli, yet the inclusion of AQ score in analyses brought out many interesting findings. In our current conceptualization of autism as a spectrum disorder, with many individuals self-diagnosed (Lewis 2016), delineating participants according to the presence of a diagnosis may be losing its potency. For instance, a recent meta-analysis demonstrated that the effect sizes in psychology studies comparing ASD and TD populations have diminished in recent years (Rødgaard et al. 2019), likely because of changes to the diagnostic criteria and a blurring of the lines between what is “normal” and what is not. Our findings support this claim and suggest that, in the future of autism research, levels of autistic traits in the general population may be more significant in understanding language processing than whether an individual holds a diagnosis.
A final point to mention is the potential of implicit stimuli to investigate semantic processing in individuals across the autism spectrum, including in those with more limited language or verbal skills. One benefit to the current implicit paradigm is that differences in N400 effects can be observed even in the absence of overt behavioral responses. This potentially opens up investigations of semantic processing to extend beyond participants who are able to understand task directions and make overt behavioral responses to indicate their understanding of semantic relatedness, as we have explored in prior studies (Coderre et al. 2019). Because the current paradigm uses written words, even investigations of implicit processing would require participants to be able to read, restricting the sample population who can take part in this research. However, future investigations using a similar paradigm with auditory words could be used, which would expand the population of individuals with ASD who could participate. Overall, we believe that both ERPs and implicit paradigms hold great potential for exploring language processing in individuals across the entire autism spectrum, including those who have little-to-no functional language.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the current study investigated semantic integration of word and picture stimuli using an implicit semantic priming task. We observed complex interactions between age and autistic traits, such that those with higher levels of autistic symptomatology demonstrated weaker modulations of the N400 ERP component with age. This trend was consistent across both picture and word modalities, suggesting a domain-general difficulty in semantic processing in those with higher levels of autistic traits. Our results point to the utility of using continuous measures of autistic traits rather than simply grouping by the presence of a diagnosis of ASD, which holds significant implications for how we conceptualize and compare groups in autism research and also may be potentially beneficial information for practitioners in the autism field. With recent changes to diagnostic criteria and increasingly more individuals self-diagnosing or foregoing formal diagnoses altogether, the conceptualization of autistic traits as being present to varying degrees in the general population serves to underscore the message of the modern neurodiversity movement: that these variations are normal, rather than deficits.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Elizabeth O’Donnell for her help with data collection. This research was supported by the College of Nursing and Health Sciences Research Incentive Grant from the University of Vermont. Preliminary analyses of these data were presented at the 2018 meeting of the American Speech Hearing Association Convention in Boston, Massachusetts.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
go back to reference APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association. APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.CrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Benau, E. M., Morris, J., & Couperus, J. W. (2011). Semantic processing in children and adults: Incongruity and the N400. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40(3), 225–239.CrossRef Benau, E. M., Morris, J., & Couperus, J. W. (2011). Semantic processing in children and adults: Incongruity and the N400. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40(3), 225–239.CrossRef
go back to reference Braeutigam, S., Swithenby, S. J., & Bailey, A. J. (2008). Contextual integration the unusual way: A magnetoencephalographic study of responses to semantic violation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1026–1036.CrossRef Braeutigam, S., Swithenby, S. J., & Bailey, A. J. (2008). Contextual integration the unusual way: A magnetoencephalographic study of responses to semantic violation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1026–1036.CrossRef
go back to reference Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., & Hoeks, J. (2012). Getting real about Semantic Illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension. Brain Research, 1446, 127–143.CrossRef Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., & Hoeks, J. (2012). Getting real about Semantic Illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension. Brain Research, 1446, 127–143.CrossRef
go back to reference Center for Disease Control and Prevention, C. (2020). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016. MMWR, 69(4). Center for Disease Control and Prevention, C. (2020). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016. MMWR, 69(4).
go back to reference Coderre, E. L. (2020). Dismantling the “visual ease assumption”: A review of visual narrative processing in clinical populations. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(1), 224–225.CrossRef Coderre, E. L. (2020). Dismantling the “visual ease assumption”: A review of visual narrative processing in clinical populations. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(1), 224–225.CrossRef
go back to reference Coderre, E. L., Chernenok, M., Gordon, B., & Ledoux, K. (2017). Linguistic and non-linguistic semantic processing in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: An ERP study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(3), 795–812.CrossRef Coderre, E. L., Chernenok, M., Gordon, B., & Ledoux, K. (2017). Linguistic and non-linguistic semantic processing in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: An ERP study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(3), 795–812.CrossRef
go back to reference Coderre, E. L., Chernenok, M., O’Grady, J., Bosley, L., Gordon, B., & Ledoux, K. (2019). Implicit measures of receptive vocabulary knowledge in individuals with Level 3 autism. Cognitve and Behavioral Neurology, 32(2), 95–119.CrossRef Coderre, E. L., Chernenok, M., O’Grady, J., Bosley, L., Gordon, B., & Ledoux, K. (2019). Implicit measures of receptive vocabulary knowledge in individuals with Level 3 autism. Cognitve and Behavioral Neurology, 32(2), 95–119.CrossRef
go back to reference Coderre, E. L., Cohn, N., Slipher, S. K., Chernenok, M., Ledoux, K., & Gordon, B. (2018). Visual and linguistic narrative comprehension in autism spectrum disorders: Neural evidence for modality-independent impairments. Brain and Language, 186, 44–59.CrossRef Coderre, E. L., Cohn, N., Slipher, S. K., Chernenok, M., Ledoux, K., & Gordon, B. (2018). Visual and linguistic narrative comprehension in autism spectrum disorders: Neural evidence for modality-independent impairments. Brain and Language, 186, 44–59.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohn, N., Paczynski, M., Jackendoff, R., Holcomb, P. J., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2012). (Pea)nuts and bolts of visual narrative: Structure and meaning in sequential image comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 65, 1–38.CrossRef Cohn, N., Paczynski, M., Jackendoff, R., Holcomb, P. J., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2012). (Pea)nuts and bolts of visual narrative: Structure and meaning in sequential image comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 65, 1–38.CrossRef
go back to reference Dien, J. (1998). Issues in the application of the average reference: Review, critiques, and recommendation. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 30(1), 34–43.CrossRef Dien, J. (1998). Issues in the application of the average reference: Review, critiques, and recommendation. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 30(1), 34–43.CrossRef
go back to reference Dunn, L., & Dunn, D. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary tests (4th ed.). In American Guidance Service. American Guidance Service. Dunn, L., & Dunn, D. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary tests (4th ed.). In American Guidance Service. American Guidance Service.
go back to reference Dunn, M. A., & Bates, J. C. (2005). Developmental change in neutral processing of words by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 361–376.CrossRef Dunn, M. A., & Bates, J. C. (2005). Developmental change in neutral processing of words by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 361–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Dunn, M. A., Gaughan, H., Jr., Kreuzer, J., & Kurtzberg, D. (1999). Electrophysiologic correlates of semantic classification in autistic and normal children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(1), 79–99.CrossRef Dunn, M. A., Gaughan, H., Jr., Kreuzer, J., & Kurtzberg, D. (1999). Electrophysiologic correlates of semantic classification in autistic and normal children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(1), 79–99.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Blackwell. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Blackwell.
go back to reference Ganis, G., Kutas, M., & Sereno, M. I. (1996). The search for “common sense”: An electrophysiological study of the comprehension of words and pictures in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(2), 89–106.CrossRef Ganis, G., Kutas, M., & Sereno, M. I. (1996). The search for “common sense”: An electrophysiological study of the comprehension of words and pictures in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(2), 89–106.CrossRef
go back to reference Gately, S. E. (2008). Facilitating reading comprehension for students on the autism spectrum. Teaching Exceptional Children, 40(3), 40–45.CrossRef Gately, S. E. (2008). Facilitating reading comprehension for students on the autism spectrum. Teaching Exceptional Children, 40(3), 40–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Groppe, D. M., Makeig, S., & Kutas, M. (2009). Identifying reliable independent components via split-half comparisons. NeuroImage, 45(4), 1199–1211.CrossRef Groppe, D. M., Makeig, S., & Kutas, M. (2009). Identifying reliable independent components via split-half comparisons. NeuroImage, 45(4), 1199–1211.CrossRef
go back to reference Hamm, J. P., Johnson, B. W., & Kirk, I. J. (2002). Comparison of the N300 and N400 ERPs to picture stimuli in congruent and incongruent contexts. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 1339–1350.CrossRef Hamm, J. P., Johnson, B. W., & Kirk, I. J. (2002). Comparison of the N300 and N400 ERPs to picture stimuli in congruent and incongruent contexts. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 1339–1350.CrossRef
go back to reference Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). The autism diagnostic observation schedule, module 4: Revised algorithm and standardized severity scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(8), 1996–2012.CrossRef Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). The autism diagnostic observation schedule, module 4: Revised algorithm and standardized severity scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(8), 1996–2012.CrossRef
go back to reference Kamio, Y., & Toichi, M. (2000). Dual access to semantics in autism: Is pictorial access superior to verbal access? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(7), 859–867.CrossRef Kamio, Y., & Toichi, M. (2000). Dual access to semantics in autism: Is pictorial access superior to verbal access? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(7), 859–867.CrossRef
go back to reference Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test (2nd ed.). American Guidance Service. Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test (2nd ed.). American Guidance Service.
go back to reference Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2329–2340.CrossRef Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2329–2340.CrossRef
go back to reference Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.CrossRef Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.CrossRef
go back to reference Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain Potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.CrossRef Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain Potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.CrossRef
go back to reference Kutas, M., & Iragui, V. (1998). The N400 in a semantic categorization task across 6 decades. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 108(5), 456–471.CrossRef Kutas, M., & Iragui, V. (1998). The N400 in a semantic categorization task across 6 decades. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 108(5), 456–471.CrossRef
go back to reference Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (De)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920–933.CrossRef Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (De)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920–933.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewis, L. F. (2016). Exploring the experience of self-diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 30(5), 575–580.CrossRef Lewis, L. F. (2016). Exploring the experience of self-diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 30(5), 575–580.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part 1): Modules 1–4. Western Psychological Services. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part 1): Modules 1–4. Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference McCleery, J. P., Ceponiene, R., Burner, K. M., Townsend, J., Kinnear, M., & Schreibman, L. (2010). Neural correlates of verbal and nonverbal semantic integration in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(3), 277–286.CrossRef McCleery, J. P., Ceponiene, R., Burner, K. M., Townsend, J., Kinnear, M., & Schreibman, L. (2010). Neural correlates of verbal and nonverbal semantic integration in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(3), 277–286.CrossRef
go back to reference McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic priming: Perspectives from memory and word recognition. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRef McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic priming: Perspectives from memory and word recognition. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRef
go back to reference McPherson, W. B., & Holcomb, P. J. (1999). An electrophysiological investigation of semantic priming with pictures of real objects. Psychophysiology, 36, 53–65.CrossRef McPherson, W. B., & Holcomb, P. J. (1999). An electrophysiological investigation of semantic priming with pictures of real objects. Psychophysiology, 36, 53–65.CrossRef
go back to reference Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1998). Autism as a disorder of complex information processing. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4, 129–136.CrossRef Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1998). Autism as a disorder of complex information processing. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4, 129–136.CrossRef
go back to reference Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.CrossRef Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.CrossRef
go back to reference Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in vidual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. Basic Processes in Reading: Visual Word Recognition, 11, 264–336. Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in vidual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. Basic Processes in Reading: Visual Word Recognition, 11, 264–336.
go back to reference Nguyen, N. N., Leytham, P., Schaefer Whitby, P., & Gelfer, J. I. (2015). Reading comprehension and autism in the primary general education classroom. The Reading Teacher, 69(1), 71–76.CrossRef Nguyen, N. N., Leytham, P., Schaefer Whitby, P., & Gelfer, J. I. (2015). Reading comprehension and autism in the primary general education classroom. The Reading Teacher, 69(1), 71–76.CrossRef
go back to reference O’Riordan, M. A., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(4), 961–979.CrossRef O’Riordan, M. A., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(4), 961–979.CrossRef
go back to reference O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–730.PubMed O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–730.PubMed
go back to reference Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Exceptions and anomalies: An ERP study on context sensitivity in autism. Neuropsychologia, 48(10), 2940–2951.CrossRef Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Exceptions and anomalies: An ERP study on context sensitivity in autism. Neuropsychologia, 48(10), 2940–2951.CrossRef
go back to reference Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Watson, P., Auyeung, B., Ring, H., et al. (2015). Measuring autistic traits in the general population: A systematic review of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Molecular Autism, 6(2), 1–12. Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Watson, P., Auyeung, B., Ring, H., et al. (2015). Measuring autistic traits in the general population: A systematic review of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Molecular Autism, 6(2), 1–12.
go back to reference Sitnikova, T., Holcomb, P. J., Kiyonaga, K., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2008). Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(11), 2037–2057.CrossRef Sitnikova, T., Holcomb, P. J., Kiyonaga, K., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2008). Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(11), 2037–2057.CrossRef
go back to reference Strandburg, R. J., Marsh, J. T., Brown, W. S., Asarnow, R. F., Guthrie, D., & Higa, J. (1993). Event-related potentials in high-functioning adult autistics: Linguistic and nonlinguistic visual information processing tasks. Neuropsychologia, 31(5), 413–434.CrossRef Strandburg, R. J., Marsh, J. T., Brown, W. S., Asarnow, R. F., Guthrie, D., & Higa, J. (1993). Event-related potentials in high-functioning adult autistics: Linguistic and nonlinguistic visual information processing tasks. Neuropsychologia, 31(5), 413–434.CrossRef
go back to reference Styslinger, M. E. (2012). Making meaning: Strategies for literacy learning. Phi Delta Kappan, 94(4), 40–45.CrossRef Styslinger, M. E. (2012). Making meaning: Strategies for literacy learning. Phi Delta Kappan, 94(4), 40–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Verbaten, M. N., Roelofs, J. W., van Engeland, H., Kenemans, J. K., & Slangen, J. L. (1991). Abnormal visual event-related potentials of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21(4), 449–470.CrossRef Verbaten, M. N., Roelofs, J. W., van Engeland, H., Kenemans, J. K., & Slangen, J. L. (1991). Abnormal visual event-related potentials of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21(4), 449–470.CrossRef
go back to reference West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2002). Event-related potentials during discourse-level semantic integration of complex pictures. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 363–375.CrossRef West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2002). Event-related potentials during discourse-level semantic integration of complex pictures. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 363–375.CrossRef
go back to reference Wilkinson, G., & Robertson, G. (2006). Wide range achievement test (4th ed.). Psychological Assessment Resources. Wilkinson, G., & Robertson, G. (2006). Wide range achievement test (4th ed.). Psychological Assessment Resources.
Metagegevens
Titel
Implicit Semantic Processing of Linguistic and Non-linguistic Stimuli in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Auteurs
Emme O’Rourke
Emily L. Coderre
Publicatiedatum
05-02-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 8/2021
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04736-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2021

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 8/2021 Naar de uitgave