Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2016

13-06-2015 | Original Article

Dissociating conscious expectancies from automatic-link formation in an electrodermal conditioning paradigm

Auteurs: Pierre Perruchet, Laurent Grégoire, Kevin Aerts, Bénédicte Poulin-Charronnat

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The key point of a paradigm initially proposed by Perruchet (Pavlov J Biol Sci 20:163–170, 1985) to dissociate conscious expectancies from automatic-link formation in classical conditioning settings is the use of a partial reinforcement schedule, in which the unconditioned stimulus (US) follows the conditioned stimulus (CS) only half of the time on average. Given (pseudo) randomization, the whole sequence comprises runs of CS alone and runs of CS–US pairs of various lengths. When the preceding run goes from a long sequence of CS alone to a long sequence of CS–US pairs (via shorter sequences), associative strength should grow up, whereas conscious expectancy should decrease. Earlier studies have shown that, in most cases, conditioned performance parallels associative strength. As an exception, however, a few reports suggest that conditioned electrodermal responses (EDRs) would follow predicted changes in US expectancies. This paper presents an experiment that replicates this outcome. However, when the performances from a control group were taken as a baseline to control for response habituation, corrected conditioned EDRs were shown to follow associative strength. This suggests that the atypical pattern of conditioned EDRs in the Perruchet paradigm would be due to the fact that EDRs are more sensitive to habituation than responses involved in other associative learning settings. These results further challenge the recent “propositional” view of conditioning, which stipulates that conditioned responses in humans are the consequence of participants’ conscious inferences about the relationships between the CS and the US, which would lead the CS to generate conscious expectancy for the US.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The single effect that is reported as significant is an interaction between two factors with two modalities, which are labeled as [CS alone; 1, 2] and [CS–US; 1, 2] in Fig. 2, respectively.
 
2
It is worth pointing out that the control group was strikingly different from the control groups routinely involved in conditioning settings. Control participants are usually assigned to conditions that prevent associative learning, to assess whether, by comparison, conditioning actually occurs in the experimental group. By contrast, removing CS-alone trials from the original paradigm leads to turn the 50 % reinforcement ratio into a 100 % reinforcement ratio, with the unusual consequence that conditioned performance should be better in the control group than in the experimental group and hence, that subtracting the former from the latter should return negative scores.
 
3
The fact that the values are in the negative range reflects nothing else than the advantage of total reinforcement (control group) over partial reinforcement (experimental group). Only the relations between the points of the curve reflect the size of the effect of the preceding runs, which is of concern here.
 
4
Planned comparisons are sometimes claimed to be inappropriate when the overall F is not significant. However, a number of experts in statistics have convincingly argued that this general advice was unwarranted (e.g., Howell, 2010; Ryan, 1959; Wilcox, 1987).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Barrett, L. C., & Livesey, E. J. (2010). Dissociations between expectancy and performance in simple and two-choice reaction-time tasks: a test of associative and nonassociative explanations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 3, 864–877. doi:10.1037/a0019403. Barrett, L. C., & Livesey, E. J. (2010). Dissociations between expectancy and performance in simple and two-choice reaction-time tasks: a test of associative and nonassociative explanations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 3, 864–877. doi:10.​1037/​a0019403.
go back to reference Burns, B. D., & Corpus, B. (2004). Randomness and inductions from streaks: “Gambler’s fallacy” versus “Hot hand”. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 179–184. doi:10.3758/BF03206480.CrossRef Burns, B. D., & Corpus, B. (2004). Randomness and inductions from streaks: “Gambler’s fallacy” versus “Hot hand”. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 179–184. doi:10.​3758/​BF03206480.CrossRef
go back to reference Clark, R. E., Manns, J. R., & Squire, L. R. (2001). Trace and delay eyeblink conditioning: contrasting phenomena of declarative and non-declarative memory. Psychological Science, 12, 304–308.CrossRefPubMed Clark, R. E., Manns, J. R., & Squire, L. R. (2001). Trace and delay eyeblink conditioning: contrasting phenomena of declarative and non-declarative memory. Psychological Science, 12, 304–308.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning and Behavior, 37, 1–20.CrossRefPubMed De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning and Behavior, 37, 1–20.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Houwer, J. (2014). Why a propositional single-process model of associative learning deserves to be defended. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual processes in social psychology (pp. 530–541). New York: Guilford. De Houwer, J. (2014). Why a propositional single-process model of associative learning deserves to be defended. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual processes in social psychology (pp. 530–541). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Destrebecqz, A., Perruchet, P., Cleeremans, A., Laureys, S., Maquet, P., & Peigneux, P. (2010). The influence of temporal factors on automatic priming and conscious expectancy in a simple reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 291–309.CrossRef Destrebecqz, A., Perruchet, P., Cleeremans, A., Laureys, S., Maquet, P., & Peigneux, P. (2010). The influence of temporal factors on automatic priming and conscious expectancy in a simple reaction time task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 291–309.CrossRef
go back to reference Howell, D. C. (2010). Statistical methods for psychology (8th ed.). Belmont: Cengage Wadsworth. Howell, D. C. (2010). Statistical methods for psychology (8th ed.). Belmont: Cengage Wadsworth.
go back to reference Livesey, E., & Costa, D. (2014). Automaticity and conscious control in single and choice response time versions of the Perruchet effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 646–664.CrossRefPubMed Livesey, E., & Costa, D. (2014). Automaticity and conscious control in single and choice response time versions of the Perruchet effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 646–664.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology, 8, 656–672.CrossRefPubMed Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology, 8, 656–672.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McAndrew, A., Jones, F. W., McLaren, R. P., & McLaren, I. P. L. (2012). Dissociating expectancy of shock and changes in skin conductance: an investigation of the perruchet effect using an electrodermal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 38, 203–208.PubMed McAndrew, A., Jones, F. W., McLaren, R. P., & McLaren, I. P. L. (2012). Dissociating expectancy of shock and changes in skin conductance: an investigation of the perruchet effect using an electrodermal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 38, 203–208.PubMed
go back to reference Miller, R. R., Barnet, R. C., & Grahame, N. J. (1995). Assessment of the Rescorla–Wagner model. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 363–386.CrossRefPubMed Miller, R. R., Barnet, R. C., & Grahame, N. J. (1995). Assessment of the Rescorla–Wagner model. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 363–386.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mitchell, C. J., Wardle, S. G., Lovibond, P. F., Weidemann, G., & Chang, B. P. I. (2010). Do reaction times in the Perruchet effect reflect variations in the strength of an associative link? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 567–572. doi:10.1037/a0018433.PubMed Mitchell, C. J., Wardle, S. G., Lovibond, P. F., Weidemann, G., & Chang, B. P. I. (2010). Do reaction times in the Perruchet effect reflect variations in the strength of an associative link? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 567–572. doi:10.​1037/​a0018433.PubMed
go back to reference Ornitz, E. M., & Guthrie, D. (1989). Long-term habituation and sensitization of the acoustic startle response in the normal adult human. Psychophysiology, 26, 166–173.CrossRefPubMed Ornitz, E. M., & Guthrie, D. (1989). Long-term habituation and sensitization of the acoustic startle response in the normal adult human. Psychophysiology, 26, 166–173.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes (trans: Gantt, W.H.). London: Allen and Unwin. Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes (trans: Gantt, W.H.). London: Allen and Unwin.
go back to reference Perruchet, P. (1985). A pitfall for the expectancy theory of human eyelid conditioning. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Sciences, 20, 163–170. Perruchet, P. (1985). A pitfall for the expectancy theory of human eyelid conditioning. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Sciences, 20, 163–170.
go back to reference Perruchet, P. (2015). Dissociating conscious expectancies from automatic link formation in associative learning: a review on the so-called Perruchet effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 41, 105–127. Perruchet, P. (2015). Dissociating conscious expectancies from automatic link formation in associative learning: a review on the so-called Perruchet effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 41, 105–127.
go back to reference Perruchet, P., Cleeremans, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (2006). Dissociating the effects of automatic activation and explicit expectancy on reaction times in a simple associative learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 955–965. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.955.PubMed Perruchet, P., Cleeremans, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (2006). Dissociating the effects of automatic activation and explicit expectancy on reaction times in a simple associative learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 955–965. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​32.​5.​955.PubMed
go back to reference Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
go back to reference Weidemann, G., Broderick, J., Lovibond, P. F., & Mitchell, C. J. (2012). Both trace and delay conditioned eyeblink responding can be dissociated from outcome expectancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 38, 1–10. doi:10.1037/a0024411.PubMed Weidemann, G., Broderick, J., Lovibond, P. F., & Mitchell, C. J. (2012). Both trace and delay conditioned eyeblink responding can be dissociated from outcome expectancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 38, 1–10. doi:10.​1037/​a0024411.PubMed
go back to reference Weidemann, G., Tangen, J. M., Lovibond, P. F., & Mitchell, C. J. (2009). Is Perruchet’s dissociation between eyeblink conditioned responding and outcome expectancy evidence for two learning systems? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 169–172. doi:10.1037/a0013294.PubMed Weidemann, G., Tangen, J. M., Lovibond, P. F., & Mitchell, C. J. (2009). Is Perruchet’s dissociation between eyeblink conditioned responding and outcome expectancy evidence for two learning systems? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 169–172. doi:10.​1037/​a0013294.PubMed
go back to reference Wilcox, R. R. (1987). New designs in analysis of variance. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 29–60.CrossRef Wilcox, R. R. (1987). New designs in analysis of variance. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 29–60.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Dissociating conscious expectancies from automatic-link formation in an electrodermal conditioning paradigm
Auteurs
Pierre Perruchet
Laurent Grégoire
Kevin Aerts
Bénédicte Poulin-Charronnat
Publicatiedatum
13-06-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0676-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2016

Psychological Research 4/2016 Naar de uitgave