Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2016

20-05-2015 | Original Article

Interference of lateralized distractors on arithmetic problem solving: a functional role for attention shifts in mental calculation

Auteurs: Nicolas Masson, Mauro Pesenti

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Solving arithmetic problems has been shown to induce shifts of spatial attention in simple probe-detection tasks, subtractions orienting attention to the left side and additions to the right side of space. Whether these attentional shifts constitute epiphenomena or are critically linked to the calculation process is still unknown. In the present study, we investigate participants’ performance on addition and subtraction solving while they have to detect central or lateralized targets. The results show that lateralized distractors presented in the hemifield congruent to the operation to be solved interfere with arithmetical solving: participants are slower to solve subtractions or additions when distractors are located on the left or on the right, respectively. These results converge with previous data to show that attentional shifts underlie not only number processing but also mental arithmetic. They extend them as they reveal the reverse effect of the one previously reported by showing that inducing attention shifts interferes with the solving of arithmetic problems. They also demonstrate that spatial attentional shifts are part of the calculation procedure of solving mentally arithmetic problems. Their functional role is to access, from the first operand, the representation of the result in a direction congruent to the operation.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: a review of data and theory. Cognition, 44, 75–106.CrossRefPubMed Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: a review of data and theory. Cognition, 44, 75–106.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Campbell, J. I. D. (2005). Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. New York: Psychology Press. Campbell, J. I. D. (2005). Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. New York: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2989–3000.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2989–3000.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Charras, P., Brod, G., & Lupiáñez, J. (2012). Is 26+ 26 smaller than 24+ 28? Estimating the approximate magnitude of repeated versus different numbers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(1), 163–173.CrossRef Charras, P., Brod, G., & Lupiáñez, J. (2012). Is 26+ 26 smaller than 24+ 28? Estimating the approximate magnitude of repeated versus different numbers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(1), 163–173.CrossRef
go back to reference Charras, P., Molina, E., & Lupiáñez, J. (2014). Additions are biased by operands: evidence from repeated versus different operands. Psychological Research, 78(2), 248–265.CrossRefPubMed Charras, P., Molina, E., & Lupiáñez, J. (2014). Additions are biased by operands: evidence from repeated versus different operands. Psychological Research, 78(2), 248–265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 4, 1–42. Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 4, 1–42.
go back to reference Dormal, V., Schuller, A. M., Nihoul, J., Pesenti, M., & Andres, M. (2014). Causal role of spatial attention in arithmetic problem solving: evidence from left unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 60, 1–9.CrossRefPubMed Dormal, V., Schuller, A. M., Nihoul, J., Pesenti, M., & Andres, M. (2014). Causal role of spatial attention in arithmetic problem solving: evidence from left unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 60, 1–9.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes shifts of spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555–556.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes shifts of spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555–556.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition. From single digits to arithmetic. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461–1483.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition. From single digits to arithmetic. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461–1483.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hoeckner, S. H., Moeller, K., Zauner, H., Wood, G., Haider, C., Gabner, A., & Nuerk, H. C. (2008). Impairments of the mental line for two-digits numbers in neglect. Cortex, 44(4), 429–438.CrossRefPubMed Hoeckner, S. H., Moeller, K., Zauner, H., Wood, G., Haider, C., Gabner, A., & Nuerk, H. C. (2008). Impairments of the mental line for two-digits numbers in neglect. Cortex, 44(4), 429–438.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klein, E., Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., & Moeller, K. (2014). Operational momentum affects eye fixation behavior. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1614–1625.CrossRefPubMed Klein, E., Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., & Moeller, K. (2014). Operational momentum affects eye fixation behavior. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1614–1625.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Knops, A., Dehaene, S., Berteletti, I., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Can approximate mental calculation account for operational momentum in addition and subtraction? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1541–1556.CrossRef Knops, A., Dehaene, S., Berteletti, I., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Can approximate mental calculation account for operational momentum in addition and subtraction? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1541–1556.CrossRef
go back to reference Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., & Dehaene, S. (2009a). Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science, 324, 1583–1585.CrossRefPubMed Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., & Dehaene, S. (2009a). Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science, 324, 1583–1585.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Knops, A., Viarouge, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009b). Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: evidence from the operational momentum effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 803–821.CrossRef Knops, A., Viarouge, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009b). Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: evidence from the operational momentum effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 803–821.CrossRef
go back to reference Lindemann, O., & Tira, M. (2011). Operational momentum in numerosity production judgments of mutli-digit number problems. Journal of Psychology, 219(1), 50–57. Lindemann, O., & Tira, M. (2011). Operational momentum in numerosity production judgments of mutli-digit number problems. Journal of Psychology, 219(1), 50–57.
go back to reference Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2013). Counting is easier while experiencing a congruent motion. PLoS One, 8(5), e64500.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2013). Counting is easier while experiencing a congruent motion. PLoS One, 8(5), e64500.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Marghetis, T., Nunez, R., & Bergen, B. (2014). Hand movements during exact arithmetic reveal systematic, dynamic spatial processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1579–1596.CrossRefPubMed Marghetis, T., Nunez, R., & Bergen, B. (2014). Hand movements during exact arithmetic reveal systematic, dynamic spatial processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1579–1596.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2014). Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1514–1526.CrossRefPubMed Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2014). Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1514–1526.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Masson, N., Pesenti, M., & Dormal, V. (2013). Spatial bias in symbolic and non-symbolic numerical comparison in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1925–1932.CrossRefPubMed Masson, N., Pesenti, M., & Dormal, V. (2013). Spatial bias in symbolic and non-symbolic numerical comparison in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1925–1932.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(8), 1324–1333.CrossRefPubMed McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(8), 1324–1333.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 400–408.CrossRefPubMed McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 400–408.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pinhas, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic. Cognition, 109, 408–415.CrossRefPubMed Pinhas, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic. Cognition, 109, 408–415.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Heed the signs: operation signs have spatial associations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1527–1540.CrossRefPubMed Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Heed the signs: operation signs have spatial associations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1527–1540.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). Addition goes where the big numbers are: evidence for a reversed operational momentum effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0786-z. (in press). Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). Addition goes where the big numbers are: evidence for a reversed operational momentum effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,. doi:10.​3758/​s13423-014-0786-z. (in press).
go back to reference Ranzini, M., Lisi, M., Blini, E., Pitteri, M., Treccani, B., Priftis, K., & Zorzi, M. (2015) Larger, smaller, odd or even? Task-specific effects of optokinetic stimulation on the mental number space. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 459–470.CrossRef Ranzini, M., Lisi, M., Blini, E., Pitteri, M., Treccani, B., Priftis, K., & Zorzi, M. (2015) Larger, smaller, odd or even? Task-specific effects of optokinetic stimulation on the mental number space. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 459–470.CrossRef
go back to reference Rossetti, Y., Jacquin-Courtois, S., Rode, G., Ota, H., Michel, C., & Boisson, D. (2004). Does action make the link between number and space representation? Visual-manual adaptation improves number line bisection in unilateral neglect. Psychological Science, 15(6), 426–430.CrossRefPubMed Rossetti, Y., Jacquin-Courtois, S., Rode, G., Ota, H., Michel, C., & Boisson, D. (2004). Does action make the link between number and space representation? Visual-manual adaptation improves number line bisection in unilateral neglect. Psychological Science, 15(6), 426–430.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Salillas, E., Granà, A., Juncadella, M., Rico, I., & Semenza, C. (2009). Leftward motion restores number space in neglect. Cortex, 45(6), 730–737.CrossRefPubMed Salillas, E., Granà, A., Juncadella, M., Rico, I., & Semenza, C. (2009). Leftward motion restores number space in neglect. Cortex, 45(6), 730–737.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
go back to reference Stoianov, I., Kramer, P., Umiltà, C., & Zorzi, M. (2008). Visuospatial priming of the mental number line. Cognition, 106, 770–779.CrossRefPubMed Stoianov, I., Kramer, P., Umiltà, C., & Zorzi, M. (2008). Visuospatial priming of the mental number line. Cognition, 106, 770–779.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vuilleumier, P., Ortigue, S., & Brugger, P. (2004). The number space and neglect. Cortex, 40(2), 399–410.CrossRefPubMed Vuilleumier, P., Ortigue, S., & Brugger, P. (2004). The number space and neglect. Cortex, 40(2), 399–410.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Werner, K., & Raab, M. (2014). Moving your eyes to solution: effects of movement on the perception of problem-solving task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1571–1578.CrossRefPubMed Werner, K., & Raab, M. (2014). Moving your eyes to solution: effects of movement on the perception of problem-solving task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1571–1578.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wiemers, M., Bekkering, H., & Lindemann, O. (2014). Spatial interferences in mental arithmetic: evidence from the motion-arithmetic compatibility effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1557–1570.CrossRefPubMed Wiemers, M., Bekkering, H., & Lindemann, O. (2014). Spatial interferences in mental arithmetic: evidence from the motion-arithmetic compatibility effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1557–1570.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138–139.CrossRefPubMed Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138–139.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Interference of lateralized distractors on arithmetic problem solving: a functional role for attention shifts in mental calculation
Auteurs
Nicolas Masson
Mauro Pesenti
Publicatiedatum
20-05-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0668-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2016

Psychological Research 4/2016 Naar de uitgave