Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Netherlands Heart Journal 1/2024

Open Access 09-11-2023 | Original Article

Brief lifestyle advice in cardiac care: an experimental study on message source and framing

Auteurs: Renée V. H. IJzerman, Rosalie van der Vaart, Linda D. Breeman, Inge van den Broek, Mike Keesman, Roderik A. Kraaijenhagen, Thomas Reijnders, Margo Weerts, Andrea W. M. Evers, Wilma J. M. Scholte op Reimer, Veronica R. Janssen, the BENEFIT consortium

Gepubliceerd in: Netherlands Heart Journal | Uitgave 1/2024

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Objective

Communicating risk information and offering lifestyle advice are important goals in cardiac rehabilitation. However, the most effective way and the most effective source to communicate this information are not yet known. Therefore, we examined the effect of source (cardiologist, physiotherapist) and framing (gain, loss) of brief lifestyle advice on patients’ intention-to-change-lifestyle.

Methods

In an online experimental study, 636 cardiac patients (40% female, 67 (10) yrs.) were randomly assigned to one of four textual vignettes. Effect of source and framing on intention-to-change-lifestyle (assessed using a 5-point Likert scale) was analysed using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

Results

Patients expressed positive intention-to-change-lifestyle after receiving advice from the cardiologist (M = 4.1) and physiotherapist (M = 3.9). However, patients showed significantly higher intention-to-change-lifestyle after receiving advice from the cardiologist (0.58 [0.54–0.61]) when compared with the physiotherapist (0.52 [0.48–0.56]), (F[1,609] = 7.06, P = 0.01). Gain-framed and loss-framed advice appeared equally effective. However, communicating risks (loss) was remembered by only 9% of patients, whereas 89% remembered benefits (gain).

Conclusions

Our study shows the value of cardiologists and physiotherapists communicating brief lifestyle advice, as cardiac patients expressed positive intention for lifestyle change after receiving advice, irrespective of framing. Lifestyle advice should include benefits due to better recall.
Extras
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12471-023-01827-7) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Data sharing

The data underlying this article will be shared by the corresponding author at reasonable request.
What’s new?
  • To understand mechanisms influencing effectiveness of brief lifestyle advice, we examined the effect of message source (cardiologist, physiotherapist) and message framing (gain, loss) of brief lifestyle advice on intention-to-change-lifestyle in cardiac patients.
  • This study demonstrates the importance of communicating brief lifestyle advice, as patients showed positive intention-to-change-lifestyle after receiving advice from the cardiologist and physiotherapist. However, patients showed significantly higher intention after receiving advice communicated by the cardiologist.
  • Higher adherence to brief lifestyle advice may arise if healthcare providers focus on communicating benefits in addition to risks, as positively framed information was remembered better.

Introduction

Engaging in a healthy lifestyle is crucial for secondary prevention of cardiovascular disease (CVD) [1, 2]. The European Guidelines on Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease in Clinical Practice recommend addressing risk information before referring patients to lifestyle interventions [1]. Using brief lifestyle advice, healthcare providers in cardiac rehabilitation can address modifiable cardiovascular risk factors such as smoking, physical activity, diet, and alcohol consumption in 2–3 min during a single consultation [3]. However, up to 50% of patients consciously choose not to follow recommendations [1]. Therefore, understanding the mechanisms influencing the effectiveness of brief lifestyle advice is essential to increase its impact [4, 5].
Message source [6, 7] and message framing [8, 9] have proved to affect acceptance of brief lifestyle advice. Messages delivered by highly credible sources (e.g., medical professionals) are perceived as more persuasive than those from less credible sources (e.g., non-experts) [6]. In the Netherlands, CVD patients often receive brief lifestyle advice from their cardiologist during quarterly consultations and from their physiotherapist during cardiac rehabilitation (CR). These healthcare professionals are both perceived as highly credible but differ in terms of frequency and duration of patient contact. However, possible differences in the effectiveness of brief lifestyle advice communicated by different credible sources are still unknown. Moreover, people have proved to respond differently to lifestyle advice communicated in terms of risks (loss-frame) or benefits (gain-frame) [10]. For example, “Quitting smoking has a positive effect on energy, appearance, and sleep quality.” (gain-frame) and “Continuing to smoke increases the risk of cardiovascular disease.” (loss-frame). It is unclear, however, which message frame is most effective when conveyed by various credible sources in cardiac rehabilitation. Therefore, this study examined whether CVD patients differ in intention to change their lifestyle after receiving either gain-framed or loss-framed brief lifestyle advice communicated by either a cardiologist or physiotherapist.

Methods

Study design

This study was an online vignette experiment on patients’ intention-to-change-lifestyle, using a 2-by‑2 between-subjects design. Following recommendations on conducting vignette studies [11], four written vignettes were developed to represent variations in source (cardiologist, physiotherapist) and framing (gain, loss) of brief lifestyle advice. This study protocol was approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee of Leiden University (2020-06-05‑A.W.M. Evers-V1-2474).

Patient population and recruitment

The study population consisted of members of the patient panel of Harteraad, the Dutch patient association for CVD, a voluntary database of approximately 2,600 CVD patients. Harteraad sent their panel members a survey invitation on 29 June 2020, with a two-week response period. The invitation described the study’s purpose, duration, participation procedure, data protection, and the survey link (2020 Qualtrics, Provo, UT). Informed consent was obtained on the first page of the survey. Inclusion criteria were Dutch-speaking adults (≥ 18 years old). With a priori power analysis (two-sided, α = 0.05, power = 0.80) [12], 128 participants were required to detect an effect of source and framing on intention-to-change-lifestyle with a small effect size (f = 0.25). To account for 10% of dropouts, we aimed for at least 141 participants in total.

Survey

The survey first assessed patients’ characteristics (age, sex, education level, last doctor’s visit, current lifestyle score, motivation and self-efficacy concerning lifestyle change). Afterwards, patients were instructed to imagine having a consultation with their healthcare provider [11]. Each patient was then randomly assigned one of the four vignettes (see Fig. 1) which described brief lifestyle advice communicated by a cardiologist or physiotherapist (message source), using a gain-framed or loss-framed perspective on the health-related future (message framing). Vignettes (see App. A) had similar word counts, contained supporting images, and allowed reading the message aloud [11]. Afterwards, the vignettes’ applicability and meaningfulness were assessed, together with the primary outcome measure intention-to-change-lifestyle [13, 14]. Finally, used as manipulation check, patients were asked if they recalled which messenger delivered the brief lifestyle advice and what message was emphasised. After completing the survey, a debriefing explained the study’s purpose.

Outcome measures

Main outcome

Similar to Taylor and colleagues (2005) [15], primary outcome measure intention-to-change-lifestyle was assessed using two items, “I want to change my lifestyle or continue to maintain my healthy lifestyle.” and “I intend to change my lifestyle or continue to maintain my healthy lifestyle.”, answered via a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from Completely disagree (1) to Completely agree (5) [16].

Other measures

Based on previous research [17], motivation, self-efficacy, and current lifestyle score were assessed with the questions “At this moment, how motivated do you feel to change your lifestyle or maintain your healthy lifestyle?”, “At this moment, how confident are you that you will be able to change your lifestyle or maintain your healthy lifestyle?”, and “At this moment, how would you rate your current lifestyle?”. Variables were rated on a 0–10 visual analogue scale (VAS) [18], representing a continuum between Very unmotivated/No confidence at all/Very unhealthy (0), and Very motivated/A lot of confidence/Very healthy (10). Perceived applicability and perceived meaningfulness of the vignette were assessed with the question, “Given your personal situation, how would you rate the conversation with the healthcare provider as just described?”. Variables were rated on a 0–10 VAS, representing a continuum between Not applicable at all/Not meaningful at all (0) and Very applicable/Very meaningful (10). Recall of message source was measured by asking “Which messenger were you speaking to?” with the answer options: Nurse, Cardiologist, Physiotherapist, or Mental health counsellor. Recall of message framing was measured by asking “What message did the healthcare provider emphasise?” with the answer options: a) “Positive effects of a healthy lifestyle” or b) “Negative consequences of an unhealthy lifestyle”.

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics V.25. As the primary outcome intention-to-change-lifestyle was skewed, inverse transformation of intention was used. Two-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) examined the effect of source and framing on intention while controlling for sex, education level, motivation, and self-efficacy [1922]. Bonferroni post hoc analyses were used for multiple comparisons. A p-value of < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Descriptive statistics were used for patient characteristics. Internal reliability was checked by calculating the scale score and Cronbach’s alpha for the intention scale, consisting of both intention items (α = 0.897). Pearson’s correlations were used to assess the strength and direction of the relationship between the control variables education level, motivation, and self-efficacy, and the dependent variable intention-to-change-lifestyle. An independent t‑test was used to assess the relationship between the control variable sex and dependent variable intention-to-change-lifestyle. Chi-squared tests were used to assess differences in recall between message source and types of message framing. Independent t‑tests were used to investigate differences in perceived applicability and meaningfulness of the vignettes for both sources and types of framing. Data are reported as n (%), mean (standard deviation), or mean (95% confidence interval).

Results

Patient characteristics

Tab. 1 describes characteristics of the 636 patients who completed the survey (estimated response rate: 24.5%). Of these patients, 383 were male (60%); mean age was 67 (10) years. Patients’ education levels were categorised as low (23%), medium (29%), high (47%). Intention-to-change-lifestyle was rated 3.9 (1.0) (range 1–5).
Table 1
Socio-demographic and health-related characteristics
Variable
Statistics
Age, years
 67 (10)
Female sex
253 (40)
Education level
 
Low (no education, and primary and preparatory vocational secondary education)
144 (23)
Medium (vocational, senior general secondary, and university preparation education)
185 (29)
High (higher vocational education and research university)
298 (47)
Other
  9 (1)
Last doctor’s visit
 
Past month
146 (23)
Past 2 to 6 months
238 (37)
More than 6 months ago
252 (40)
Current lifestyle scorea
  6.94 (1.75)
Motivation concerning lifestyle changea
  6.89 (2.32)
Self-efficacy concerning lifestyle changea
  6.88 (2.20)
Intention-to-change-lifestyleb
  3.92 (1.00)
Data are n (%) or mean (standard deviation); mentioned when otherwise
aQuestion answered via a 0–10 visual analogue scale (low [0] to high [10])
bStatement answered via a 5-point Likert scale (Completely disagree [1] to Completely agree [5])

Recall and vignette perceptions

After 35.5 (303) minutes on average, when the experiment ended, patients’ recall of source and framing was examined to determine the effectiveness of both manipulations. Significant associations were found between recall and source and between recall and framing, χ2(1) = 399.890, P = 0.05 and χ2(1) = 399.680, P < 0.001 respectively. Regarding source, 74% of the patients recalled the cardiologist, and 67% recalled the physiotherapist. Regarding framing, 89% of the patients recalled the gain frame, and 9% recalled the loss frame.
Concerning vignette perceptions (0–10 VAS), patients rated applicability 4.88 (2.69), and meaningfulness 5.22 (2.70). A significant difference in perceived meaningfulness between the cardiologist’s vignettes, 5.44 (2.63), and the physiotherapist’s, 4.99 (2.76), was found, t = 2.07, P = 0.04. No differences were found in other patient conditions.

Effect of source and framing on intention-to-change-lifestyle

Tab. 2 presents the effect of message source and framing on intention-to-change-lifestyle. A two-way ANCOVA showed a significant main effect of source on intention, F(1,609) = 7.06, P = 0.01, while controlling for sex, education level, motivation, and self-efficacy. Patients expressed significantly higher intention after receiving advice from the cardiologist, 0.58 (0.54–0.61), than from the physiotherapist, 0.52 (0.48–0.56), P = 0.01. The main effect of framing on intention was non-significant, F(1,609) = 0.01, P = 0.92, as was the interaction effect between source and framing on intention, F(1,609) = 0.42, P = 0.52. Repeating the analysis without covariates showed similar results.
Table 2
Effect of message source and message framing on intention-to-change-lifestyle
   
Effect of the manipulations
 
Adjusted
value
Unadjusted
value
Adjusted
F (df = 609)a
P
Unadjusted
F (df = 632)
P
Message framing
  
0.01
0.92
0.08
0.78
Gain
0.55 (0.51–0.59)
0.61 (0.58–0.64)
    
Loss
0.45 (0.51–0.59)
0.60 (0.57–0.63)
    
Message source
  
7.06
0.01
5.65
0.02
Cardiologist
0.58 (0.54–0.61)
0.63 (0.60–0.66)
    
Physiotherapist
0.52 (0.48–0.56)
0.58 (0.55–0.61)
    
Message framing x message source
  
0.42
0.52
0.04
0.84
Gain x cardiologist
0.57 (0.52–0.62)
0.63 (0.59–0.67)
    
Gain x physiotherapist
0.53 (0.48–0.58)
0.58 (0.54–0.63)
    
Loss x cardiologist
0.58 (0.53–0.63)
0.63 (0.58–0.67)
    
Loss x physiotherapist
0.52 (0.47–0.56)
0.57 (0.53–0.61)
    
Values are estimated mean (95% confidence interval)
aAdjusted for sex, education level, motivation, and self-efficacy
df degrees of freedom

Discussion

This study demonstrates the value of communicating brief lifestyle advice to cardiac patients, as they expressed positive intentions to change their lifestyle after receiving advice from either the cardiologist or the physiotherapist. Generally, however, a higher intention to change was expressed when the advice was communicated by the cardiologist; this difference in message source was found to be significant. Gain-framed or loss-framed advice appeared equally effective for patients’ intention. However, gain-framed information was recalled better than loss-framed information.
Concerning message source, the fact that patients expressed a significantly higher intention after receiving advice from the cardiologist than the physiotherapist may be due to slightly different perceived credibility towards the healthcare providers. Possibly, patients make a subtle distinction between a cardiologist and physiotherapist when evaluating their expertise, trustworthiness, or caring [7]. Specifically, patients appear more likely to adhere to treatment advice when communicated by credible physicians [23] and feel more satisfied with the healthcare received when they perceive the source as competent or caring [24]. This may explain why patients rated the meaningfulness of the cardiologist’s vignette higher, even though it was identical to the physiotherapist’s vignette, and recalled the advice from the cardiologist better than the advice from the physiotherapist.
Concerning message framing, gain-framed and loss-framed brief lifestyle advice appeared equally effective on intention. This contrasts with prior research that showed a more positive influence of gain-framed health messages on patient evaluation of treatment advice and their intention to change preventive lifestyle behaviour [8, 9]. However, these studies mainly focussed on specific lifestyle behaviour (e.g., smoking cessation) rather than generic lifestyle behaviour as we did. Moreover, in line with our results, meta-analyses have shown that communicating either gain-framed or loss-framed advice did not have significantly different advantages for patients’ intentions to change lifestyle [10]. Nevertheless, patients rated the vignette’s applicability and meaningfulness as average, but this may have been different if it had been discussed in the consultation room and the advice had been tailored to the patient under the healthcare provider’s guidance.
Subsequently, patients recalled gain-framed advice more often than loss-framed advice. This may be due to patients undergoing treatment for an extended period and probably already receiving similar lifestyle advice. Moreover, patients’ memory representation may have affected memories of the received advice; exact details such as framing of words quickly become inaccessible because of rapid memory decline during the first 15 to 30 min after exposure to the message. After this period, an essential meaning of the message is remembered [25]. Finally, the positivity effect, which is the tendency for older adults to remember positive information and disregard negative information, may also have played a role in this outcome [26].

Limitations and implications for practice

Concerning limitations, loss-framed lifestyle advice appeared challenging to recall. Possibly, the manipulation of message framing was not strong enough or insufficiently specified, or healthcare providers had already communicated the generic health-related information described in the vignette. As a result, message framing might not have been striking enough. Next, this study specifically explored the impact of source and framing on intention after providing generic brief lifestyle advice, using vignettes solely describing the advice. Future research could examine whether intention increases when vignettes convey the steps required to implement the desired lifestyle change. Furthermore, even though intention is an essential predictor of actual behaviour [27], there is a gap between intention and actual behaviour [28]. Additionally, absence of direct face-to-face consultations may have influenced our findings’ generalisability to real-world settings. While our vignettes were developed based on scientific recommendations [11] and aimed to simulate realistic patient-provider interactions in cardiac care, we emphasise the need for future research to validate our findings in actual clinical settings.
Current ESC guidelines on cardiovascular disease prevention recommend providing risk information to stimulate attendance to lifestyle interventions [1]. This study found that only 9% of patients remembered risk communication, whereas 89% remembered communication of benefits. To improve adherence to brief lifestyle advice, healthcare providers should emphasise the benefits of healthy living in addition to risks. Additionally, the mere endorsement of a healthcare provider may bolster patients’ intention-to-change-lifestyle. Given physicians’ limited consultation time, emphasising the value of a healthy lifestyle without extensive discussion of associated behaviours can be effective. Easy referral to other healthcare providers using motivational interviewing techniques [29, 30], for example, physiotherapists, generalist or specialist nurses, or healthcare providers working at lifestyle care counters, will further facilitate suitable treatment and sustainable lifestyle change.

Conclusion

Cardiac patients expressed the highest intention to change their lifestyle when brief lifestyle advice was communicated by a cardiologist. This was irrespective of message framing, but communicating benefits rather than risks appeared to be related to better recall. The results and associated practical implications can support healthcare providers in cardiac care in optimising the brief lifestyle advice they provide their patients to facilitate suitable treatment and sustainable lifestyle change.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Netherlands Cardiovascular Research Initiative: An Initiative with the support of the Dutch Heart Foundation (CVON2016-12 BENEFIT) and ZonMw (The Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development), Harteraad (the Dutch patient association for CVD) for their participation in spreading the survey among the members of their research panel, all members who participated in the study, and the members of the BENEFIT consortium.

Funding

This work was supported by the Netherlands Cardiovascular Research Initiative: An Initiative with the support of the Dutch Heart Foundation (CVON2016-12 BENEFIT), ZonMw (The Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development), and members of the BENEFIT consortium.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

R.V.H. IJzerman, R. van der Vaart, L.D. Breeman, I. van den Broek, M. Keesman, R.A. Kraaijenhagen, T. Reijnders, M. Weerts, A.W.M. Evers, W.J.M. Scholte op Reimer and V.R. Janssen declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical standards

Approval for this study was obtained from the Psychology Research Ethics Committee of Leiden University, the Netherlands (reference number: 2020-06-05‑A.W.M. Evers-V1-2474). All procedures performed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Patient consent: Informed consent was obtained from all participants before taking part in the online experimental study. Public and patient involvement: Cardiac patients and an employee of Harteraad, the Dutch patient association for CVD, were asked to provide feedback during the development of the survey’s content and design. All those involved also conducted one or more pilot tests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Onze productaanbevelingen

Netherlands Heart Journal

Het Netherlands Heart Journal wordt uitgegeven in samenwerking met de Nederlandse Vereniging voor Cardiologie en de Nederlandse Hartstichting. Het tijdschrift is Engelstalig en wordt gratis beschikbaa ...

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Visseren FLJ, Mach F, Smulders YM, et al. ESC Guidelines on cardiovascular disease prevention in clinical practice. Eur Heart J. 2021;42:3227–337.CrossRef Visseren FLJ, Mach F, Smulders YM, et al. ESC Guidelines on cardiovascular disease prevention in clinical practice. Eur Heart J. 2021;42:3227–337.CrossRef
2.
go back to reference Kotseva K, de Backer G, de Bacquer D, et al. Lifestyle and impact on cardiovascular risk factor control in coronary patients across 27 countries: results from the European Society of Cardiology ESC-EORP EUROASPIRE V registry. Eur J Prev Cardiol. 2019;26:824–35.CrossRef Kotseva K, de Backer G, de Bacquer D, et al. Lifestyle and impact on cardiovascular risk factor control in coronary patients across 27 countries: results from the European Society of Cardiology ESC-EORP EUROASPIRE V registry. Eur J Prev Cardiol. 2019;26:824–35.CrossRef
3.
go back to reference Bull FC, Jamrozik K. Advice on exercise from a family physician can help sedentary patients to become active. Am J Prev Med. 1998;15:85–94.CrossRef Bull FC, Jamrozik K. Advice on exercise from a family physician can help sedentary patients to become active. Am J Prev Med. 1998;15:85–94.CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Young JM, Ward JE. Implementing guidelines for smoking cessation advice in Australian general practice: opinions, current practices, readiness to change and perceived barriers. Fam Pract. 2001;18:14–20.CrossRefPubMed Young JM, Ward JE. Implementing guidelines for smoking cessation advice in Australian general practice: opinions, current practices, readiness to change and perceived barriers. Fam Pract. 2001;18:14–20.CrossRefPubMed
5.
go back to reference Williams K, Beeken RJ, Fisher A, Wardle J. Health professionals’ provision of lifestyle advice in the oncology context in the United Kingdom. Eur J Cancer Care. 2015;24:522–30.CrossRef Williams K, Beeken RJ, Fisher A, Wardle J. Health professionals’ provision of lifestyle advice in the oncology context in the United Kingdom. Eur J Cancer Care. 2015;24:522–30.CrossRef
6.
go back to reference Jones LW, Sinclair RC, Courneya KS. The effects of source credibility and message framing on exercise intentions, behaviors, and attitudes: an integration of the elaboration likelihood model and prospect theory. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33:179–96.CrossRef Jones LW, Sinclair RC, Courneya KS. The effects of source credibility and message framing on exercise intentions, behaviors, and attitudes: an integration of the elaboration likelihood model and prospect theory. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33:179–96.CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Hovland CI, Weiss W. The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opin Q. 1951;15:635-50. Hovland CI, Weiss W. The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opin Q. 1951;15:635-50.
8.
go back to reference Pope JP, Pelletier L, Guertin C. Starting off on the best foot: a review of message framing and message tailoring, and recommendations for the comprehensive messaging strategy for sustained behavior change. Health Commun. 2018;33:1068–77.CrossRef Pope JP, Pelletier L, Guertin C. Starting off on the best foot: a review of message framing and message tailoring, and recommendations for the comprehensive messaging strategy for sustained behavior change. Health Commun. 2018;33:1068–77.CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Glare P, Fridman I, Ashton-James CE. Choose your words wisely: the impact of message framing on patients’ responses to treatment advice. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2018;139:159–90.CrossRefPubMed Glare P, Fridman I, Ashton-James CE. Choose your words wisely: the impact of message framing on patients’ responses to treatment advice. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2018;139:159–90.CrossRefPubMed
10.
go back to reference O’Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Commun. 2009;59:296–316.CrossRef O’Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Commun. 2009;59:296–316.CrossRef
11.
go back to reference Evans SC, Roberts MC, Keeley JW, et al. Vignette methodologies for studying clinicians’ decision-making: validity, utility, and application in ICD-11 field studies. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2015;15:160–70.CrossRef Evans SC, Roberts MC, Keeley JW, et al. Vignette methodologies for studying clinicians’ decision-making: validity, utility, and application in ICD-11 field studies. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2015;15:160–70.CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91.CrossRef Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91.CrossRef
13.
go back to reference Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Predicting and changing behavior: the reasoned action approach. New York: Psychology Press; 2010. pp. 1–464. Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Predicting and changing behavior: the reasoned action approach. New York: Psychology Press; 2010. pp. 1–464.
14.
go back to reference Perugini M, Bagozzi RP. The role of desires and anticipated emotions in goal-directed behaviours: broadening and deepening the theory of planned behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol. 2001;40:79–98.CrossRef Perugini M, Bagozzi RP. The role of desires and anticipated emotions in goal-directed behaviours: broadening and deepening the theory of planned behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol. 2001;40:79–98.CrossRef
15.
go back to reference Taylor SD, Bagozzi RP, Gaither CA. Decision making and effort in the self-regulation of hypertension: testing two competing theories. Br J Health Psychol. 2005;10(pt 4):505–30.CrossRef Taylor SD, Bagozzi RP, Gaither CA. Decision making and effort in the self-regulation of hypertension: testing two competing theories. Br J Health Psychol. 2005;10(pt 4):505–30.CrossRef
16.
go back to reference Krosnick JA, Presser S. Question and questionnaire design. In: Marsden PV, Wright JD, editors. Handbook of survey research. Bingley: Emerald Publishing; 2010. pp. 263–314. Krosnick JA, Presser S. Question and questionnaire design. In: Marsden PV, Wright JD, editors. Handbook of survey research. Bingley: Emerald Publishing; 2010. pp. 263–314.
17.
go back to reference Bandura A. Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In: Pajares F, Urdan T, editors. Self-Efficacy beliefs of adolescents. Greenwich: Age Information Publishing; 2006. pp. 307–37. Bandura A. Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In: Pajares F, Urdan T, editors. Self-Efficacy beliefs of adolescents. Greenwich: Age Information Publishing; 2006. pp. 307–37.
18.
go back to reference Kuhlmann T, Reips UD, Wienert J, Lippke S. Using visual analogue scales in eHealth: non-response effects in a lifestyle intervention. J Med Internet Res. 2016;18:e126.CrossRef Kuhlmann T, Reips UD, Wienert J, Lippke S. Using visual analogue scales in eHealth: non-response effects in a lifestyle intervention. J Med Internet Res. 2016;18:e126.CrossRef
19.
go back to reference Bandura A. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman; 1997. pp. 1–604. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman; 1997. pp. 1–604.
20.
go back to reference Cutler DM, Huang W, Lleras-Muney A. When does education matter? The protective effect of education for cohorts graduating in bad times. Soc Sci Med. 2015;127:63–73.CrossRefPubMed Cutler DM, Huang W, Lleras-Muney A. When does education matter? The protective effect of education for cohorts graduating in bad times. Soc Sci Med. 2015;127:63–73.CrossRefPubMed
21.
go back to reference Ryan RM, Deci EL. A self-determination theory approach to psychotherapy: the motivational basis for effective change. Can Psychol. 2008;49:186–93.CrossRef Ryan RM, Deci EL. A self-determination theory approach to psychotherapy: the motivational basis for effective change. Can Psychol. 2008;49:186–93.CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Loprinzi PD, Branscum A, Hanks J, Smit E. Healthy lifestyle characteristics and their joint association with cardiovascular disease biomarkers in US adults. Mayo Clin Proc. 2016;91:432–42.CrossRef Loprinzi PD, Branscum A, Hanks J, Smit E. Healthy lifestyle characteristics and their joint association with cardiovascular disease biomarkers in US adults. Mayo Clin Proc. 2016;91:432–42.CrossRef
23.
go back to reference Wrench JS, Booth-Butterfield M. Increasing patient satisfaction and compliance: an examination of physician humor orientation, compliance-gaining strategies, and perceived credibility. Commun Q. 2003;51:482–503.CrossRef Wrench JS, Booth-Butterfield M. Increasing patient satisfaction and compliance: an examination of physician humor orientation, compliance-gaining strategies, and perceived credibility. Commun Q. 2003;51:482–503.CrossRef
24.
go back to reference Paulsel ML, McCroskey JC, Richmond VP. Perceptions of health care professionals’ credibility as a predictor of patients’ satisfaction with their medical care and physician. Commun Res Rep. 2006;23:69–76.CrossRef Paulsel ML, McCroskey JC, Richmond VP. Perceptions of health care professionals’ credibility as a predictor of patients’ satisfaction with their medical care and physician. Commun Res Rep. 2006;23:69–76.CrossRef
25.
go back to reference Blalock SJ, Reyna VF. Using fuzzy-trace theory to understand and improve health judgments, decisions, and behaviors: a literature review. Health Psychol. 2016;35:781–92.CrossRef Blalock SJ, Reyna VF. Using fuzzy-trace theory to understand and improve health judgments, decisions, and behaviors: a literature review. Health Psychol. 2016;35:781–92.CrossRef
26.
go back to reference Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:496–502.CrossRef Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:496–502.CrossRef
27.
go back to reference Sheeran P. Intention—behavior relations: a conceptual and empirical review. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2002;12:1–36.CrossRef Sheeran P. Intention—behavior relations: a conceptual and empirical review. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2002;12:1–36.CrossRef
28.
go back to reference Sheeran P, Webb TL. The intention–behavior gap. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2016;10:503–18.CrossRef Sheeran P, Webb TL. The intention–behavior gap. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2016;10:503–18.CrossRef
29.
go back to reference Rubak S, Sandbaek A, Lauritzen T, Christensen B. Motivational interviewing: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Gen Pract. 2005;55:305–12. Rubak S, Sandbaek A, Lauritzen T, Christensen B. Motivational interviewing: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Gen Pract. 2005;55:305–12.
30.
go back to reference Magill M, Hallgren KA. Mechanisms of behavior change in motivational interviewing: do we understand how MI works? Curr Opin Psychol. 2019;30:1–5.CrossRefPubMed Magill M, Hallgren KA. Mechanisms of behavior change in motivational interviewing: do we understand how MI works? Curr Opin Psychol. 2019;30:1–5.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Brief lifestyle advice in cardiac care: an experimental study on message source and framing
Auteurs
Renée V. H. IJzerman
Rosalie van der Vaart
Linda D. Breeman
Inge van den Broek
Mike Keesman
Roderik A. Kraaijenhagen
Thomas Reijnders
Margo Weerts
Andrea W. M. Evers
Wilma J. M. Scholte op Reimer
Veronica R. Janssen
the BENEFIT consortium
Publicatiedatum
09-11-2023
Uitgeverij
Bohn Stafleu van Loghum
Gepubliceerd in
Netherlands Heart Journal / Uitgave 1/2024
Print ISSN: 1568-5888
Elektronisch ISSN: 1876-6250
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12471-023-01827-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2024

Netherlands Heart Journal 1/2024 Naar de uitgave