Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development 5/2022

Open Access 29-04-2021 | Original Article

Borderline Personality Traits and Emotion Regulation Strategies in Adolescents: The Role of Implicit Theories

Auteurs: Jane McLachlan, Mani Mehdikhani, Beth Larham, Luna C. Muñoz Centifanti

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 5/2022

Abstract

Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is associated with emotion dysregulation. The emotion regulation strategies people adopt may depend on one’s belief about emotion as either fixed or changeable (termed “implicit theory”). We test this experimentally by modifying beliefs about emotions using virtual reality (VR). A sample of 29 adolescents (ages 14–17 years) were recruited from two adolescent inpatient units for an uncontrolled pilot trial of a new brief intervention. We measured BPD traits, beliefs about emotion, treatment preference, cognitive reappraisal and rumination, before the VR game and 14–31 days later. Adolescents with higher levels of BPD traits endorsed fixed beliefs of emotion and reported higher levels of rumination and lower levels of cognitive reappraisal. After a one-time message delivered via VR, adolescents evidenced an increase in belief that their emotions were changeable. These findings suggest beliefs about emotions may have an important role in interventions for adolescents with BPD traits.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10578-021-01169-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is characterised by difficult interpersonal relationships, emotional instability, and impulsivity, including non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI; 13]. Adolescence is often marked by one or more of these features, but for most people these traits do not cause significant levels of distress [4]. However, for some, these traits can be associated with a poor quality of life [5]. Many features of BPD manifest during adolescence or young adulthood [6]. The majority of young adults with mental health difficulties first received any mental health diagnosis in adolescence [7, 8], which suggests that recognising and understanding BPD traits in adolescence may inform the development of early interventions appropriate to this population.
People’s beliefs about emotions may be a factor that plays a role in how they respond to negative affect. Implicit theories are beliefs that a person has about the inherent malleability of certain domains, such as emotions [9]. Individuals fall along a continuum of how they view controllability of emotions [10]. At one end, a changeable belief about emotion views this as dynamic and controllable through effort. At the other extreme, a fixed belief views emotion as unchangeable and immutable once they occur. Whether people lean towards a fixed or changeable theory of their own emotion has significant implications for emotion regulation tendencies [11].
People who endorse a fixed belief may think that changes cannot be made through exerting additional effort and thus are more likely to use emotion regulation strategies that are deemed unhelpful (e.g. rumination; [12]). Conversely, those who view emotions as malleable tended to use strategies that are considered adaptive in many settings (e.g. cognitive reappraisal; 11).
To our knowledge, prior research is non-existent on implicit theories of emotion within an adolescent clinical population. This is surprising given that adolescence is a crucial stage of development for early intervention; it is an opportunity to reduce chronicity, improve outcomes and prevent stigmatising labels in later life [13, 14].
Rumination is one of the most widely researched cognitive emotion regulation strategies and has been linked to BPD across a range of studies [1518]. Rumination has also been associated with many of the destructive behaviours in BPD, such as NSSI [19], suicide attempts [20] and substance misuse [21]. Such ‘dysregulated behaviours’ [20] are difficult to control and can lead to impairment in a person’s daily and interpersonal functioning.
People with a diagnosis of BPD have been found to engage in cognitive reappraisal less frequently, although this strategy may reduce their emotional lability [22, 23]. People who use cognitive reappraisal interpret events in ways that can reduce the negative impact of the emotional response [24, 25] and this has been shown to be a protective factor from psychopathology [26]. There is limited research on the longitudinal development of cognitive emotion regulation strategies, but initial findings indicate a strong linear increase in their use across the lifespan [27]. Adolescence therefore may be a crucial period for intervention to reduce use and impact of rumination and promote helpful strategies such as cognitive reappraisal that support positive wellbeing. What leads adolescents to show preference for one strategy over another remains unclear.
Finding ways to involve young people in treatment is key for early intervention. Historically, the retention of people with BPD in psychological treatments has been low, and the presence of BPD in clinical samples often predicts dropout [28]. Studies often do not report on how people engage with treatment. Results from treatment trials indicate that Dialectical Behaviour Therapy (DBT) and psychodynamic approaches are shown to be most effective for BPD, but effect sizes are low [29]. For people to initiate attempts to regulate their emotions, they must first believe that emotions can in principle be controlled and most importantly, that they can personally control them [30].
Virtual reality (VR) can be an innovative tool to enhance how we assess and treat mental health difficulties. VR involves the simulation of real-world experiences using computer graphics in which the user is immersed into and interacts with the virtual environment. It has been used in the treatment of specific phobias [31], social anxiety [32] and post-traumatic stress [33]. Emerging technologies such as VR and Augmented Reality (AR) have the potential to enhance how clinicians assess, diagnose and treat mental health conditions, including personality disorder. Potential benefits of VR (in research and in treatment) are ‘making the impossible, possible’ (to create environments which would be hard or impossible to recreate outside of the realm of imagination), ‘making the possible, safer’ (one can explore in complete safety dangerous or threat scenarios, for example in the context of exposure therapy) and by ‘making the possible safe, more practical and effective’ [34].
To our knowledge, no study to date has explored the use of VR for BPD within adolescent inpatient mental health services. Single-session interventions that promote a changeable belief of personality have shown to be effective in reducing risk factors for youth internalising disorders [35]. Thus, we used a single-session VR game as a platform for delivering a psychoeducational message on the changeability of emotions in inpatient settings.
In the present study, our aims were fourfold: [1] adolescents who endorsed more fixed beliefs of emotions would be more likely to report engaging in unhelpful cognitive emotion regulation strategies, such as rumination as compared to those who reported a more malleable belief of emotions. We also predicted that they would be less likely to report engaging in more helpful antecedent cognitive emotion regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal. [2] We predicted that those with higher BPD traits would report more fixed beliefs than changeable beliefs, given the associated cognitive and behavioural difficulties that those adolescents experience. [3] Adolescents would be more likely to report feeling that their emotions were more malleable rather than fixed after a period of 2–4 weeks following game play in VR where they were told a story which asserted the changeability of emotion. [4] There is a possibility that adolescents who hold a more fixed belief of emotion would be less motivated to engage in psychological therapies, since they might not believe that changing emotions is within their control. This possibility was explored.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

Participants were invited to take part in an uncontrolled pilot trial of a new brief intervention. Participants were drawn from all admissions to two National Health Service (NHS) inpatient child and adolescent mental health units in the North West of England. Exclusion criteria were those patients with a diagnosis of a learning disability or a current episode of psychosis. Sample characteristics are presented in Table 1. All study procedures were approved by the University of Liverpool and the NHS. We obtained written informed consent/assent from all the participants. Consent from a parent or guardian was obtained for those under the age of 16 years.
Table 1
Sample characteristics
Variable
Time 1 n (%)
Time 2 n (%)
Participants
30
19
Sex
  
Male, n
9
6
Female, n
20
12
Transgender male, n
1
1
Age, mean (SD)
15.9 (1.2)
15.7 (1.3)
Ethnicity
  
White, n (%)
25 (83%)
15 (78%)
Mixed race, n (%)
2 (7%)
2 (11%)
Asian, n (%)
1 (3%)
0 (0%)
Undisclosed, n (%)
2 (7%)
2 (11%)
Current treatment
  
Medication, n (%)
5 (13%)
3 (16%)
Psychological therapy, n (%)
6 (20%)
3 (16%)
Combination, n (%)
17 (57%)
10 (52%)
Monitoring only, n (%)
3 (10%)
3 (16%)
Primary psychiatric diagnosis
  
Autism Spectrum Disorder (%)
4 (13%)
3 (16%)
Adjustment Disorder (%)
1 (3%)
0 (0%)
ADHD (%)
2 (7%)
1 (5%)
Depression (%)
2 (7%)
2 (11%)
Eating disorder (%)
1 (3%)
1 (5%)
Previous psychotic episode (%)
3 (10%)
2 (11%)
PTSD (%)
2 (7%)
2 (11%)
OCD (%)
1 (3%)
0 (0%)
No psychiatric diagnosis (%)
14 (47%)
8 (42%)
SD standard deviation
Participants completed four self-report measures and then completed the VR task. After a minimum of 2 weeks (M = 19.42 days; SD = 7.14; Range = 14 – 31 days), all participants were invited to repeat the set of self-report measures. An option was provided to repeat the VR game if they chose, but this was at the end of all procedures as an incentive to complete the follow-up assessment. All participants received a £3 voucher as gratitude for their time at both time points.

Measures

VR task

The VR game ran on a Blade Pro-17.3″ (full HD) laptop, with Core i7-7700HQ Processor with Hyper-Threading 2.8 GHz, a graphic card NVIDIA GeForce GTX 1060 (6 GB GDDR5 VRAM) and Windows 10 Home operating system. The immersive virtual environment was displayed on an Oculus Rift VR HMD DK2 system. The Oculus head mounted display provides an immersive 3D virtual environment in a wide field of view. The InMind version 1 VR game was designed by Nival [36] and was played on Steam. Participants were verbally introduced to a character that struggled with intense emotions but had recently discovered research findings that supported the notion that emotions are changeable (Appendix S1). The concept of doing things differently to help reduce intensity of negative experiences was reinforced via the VR game where they actively participated and took charge of changing emotions within the brain. Participants were asked to focus on red neurons and were told to “fire” on them to transform them to green, which signalled a reduction in the intensity of emotion. This was synonymous with reducing the intensity of an emotional experience by doing something differently. Participants completed a self-report measure of flow following their gaming experience. The participants’ flow experiences were measured using the Flow Short Scale [37]. Participants rated 10-items on a Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much), which assessed fluency in action and being absorbed by action.

Borderline Personality Traits

Borderline personality traits were assessed using the Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children (BPFS-C; [38]). Participants rated 24-items using a Likert scale from 1 (not at all true) to 5 (always true). Items 1, 5, 23 and 24 were reverse scored.

Implicit Theories of Emotions

Beliefs about emotions were measured using the Implicit Theories of Emotion Scale [10, 11]. Participants rated the degree to which they agree or disagree to 8 statements about the changeability of their own emotions or the emotions of others on a 5-point Likert scale. Changeability belief items were reverse scored and the mean was obtained. Higher scores indicated a fixed belief and lower scores indicated a changeable belief of emotion.

Emotion Regulation Strategies

Cognitive emotion regulation strategies were measured using the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ; [39]). Participants rated 36-items on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). The items consisted of 9 subscales including rumination and positive reappraisal.

Hypothetical Treatment Choice

Single item adapted from [40]: “If you struggle, or were to struggle with emotional difficulties (e.g. uncontrollable outbursts of anger, intense sadness) and had a choice between some form of psychological intervention, medication, a combination of medication and psychological intervention or not treatment other than standard monitoring to help you with these difficulties, which would you choose?” Further information was given about what each treatment option would entail, and opportunity was provided for participants to ask the researcher.

Results

Data Analyses

Results from the G*Power calculations [41] for paired sample t-tests, assuming a p value = 0.05, a large effect size of 0.5, with a statistical power of 0.8, recommend a sample size of 27. One participant was excluded from analysis because they did not complete the VR task. This reduced the sample size at Time 1 to 29, and to 18 at Time 2.
Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 2. The z-scores for skewness and kurtosis indicated that only positive reappraisal at Time 2 had significant skewness and kurtosis, based on a z-score of larger than 1.96 for a sample size of less than 50 [42]. One outlier identified was readjusted to one score above the next highest score. No other normality issues were identified. To test engagement in the VR task, we examined the average score of flow. The average score for total flow (M = 48.03, SD = 11.20, range 28–70) was consistent with previous studies using the measure in community samples who reported a mean of 48.88 (SD = 10.90; Bian et al., 2016).
Table 2
Descriptive statistics for primary study variables at Time 1 and Time 2
 
Time 2 (n = 18)
Cronbach’s α
Mean (SD)
Cronbach’s α
Mean (SD)
Rumination
0.884
3.534 (0.293)
0.695
3.597 (0.083)
Cognitive reappraisal
0.808
2.103 (0.241)
0.864
2.292 (0.254)
BPFS
0.794
3.478 (0.403)
0.798
3.382 (0.409)
Implicit beliefs—self
0.875
3.560 (00.325)
0.821
3.431 (0.404)
Implicit beliefs—General
0.798
3.190 (0.215)
0.645
3.014 (0.233)
Flow—fluency
0.811
4.874 (0.566)
0.655
4.630 (0.639)
Flow—absorption
0.270
4.698 (0.174)
 − 0.274
4.583 (0.962)
Flow- Total
0.777
4.803 (0.443)
0.706
4.611 (0.732)
We used Spearman correlations to examine associations among measures at Time 1 and Time 2. We subsequently performed paired-samples t-tests to examine change from Time 1 to Time 2 to test the effect of the VR task which aimed to change beliefs about emotions.

Does Implicit Theory of Emotions Relate to Emotion Regulation Strategies and BPD?

To test if people who held a more fixed rather than changeable belief of emotion were more likely to use higher levels of rumination and less cognitive reappraisal, we conducted Spearman’s Rho correlations (Table 3). The association between general implicit beliefs of emotion and cognitive reappraisal or rumination were weak and failed to reach significance.
Table 3
Spearman correlations between variables of interest at Time 1 and Time 2
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
1.BPD Traits
0.696*
0.164
0.580*
 − 0.054
0.189
 − 0.172
0.339
 − 0.356
2.Implicit beliefs—General
0.233
0.621*
0.042
 − 0.101
0.007
0.264
 − 0.196
0.033
3.Implicit beliefs—self
0.435*
0.453*
0.837*
 − 0.593*
0.364
 − 0.177
0.156
 − 0.338
4.Cognitive reappraisal
 − 0.477*
 − 0.262
 − 0.724*
0.810*
 − 0.596*
0.106
0.071
 − 0.152
5.Rumination
0.354
 − 0.029
0.381*
 − 0.384*
0.715*
 − 0.071
0.113
0.315
6.Age (Time 1)
0.098
0.214
 − 0.221
0.084
 − 0.175
7.Gender (Time 1)
0.235
 − 0.149
0.011
 − 0.184
0.066
8.Psychiatric diagnosis (Time 1)
 − 0.172
 − 0.375*
 − 0.304
0.147
0.189
Correlation coefficients for Time 1 are below the diagonal and Time 2 are above the diagonal, with covariance measures reported on the diagonal and in bold. Age, gender, and psychiatric diagnosis were only measured at Time 1
*p < .05
Similarly, we tested if people who held a more fixed belief of their own emotion were more likely to use more rumination and less cognitive reappraisal. Consistent with predictions, a more fixed belief of one’s own emotions was associated with higher levels of rumination at Time 1 (rs = 0.38, p = 0.042); however, this did not hold at Time 2 (rs = 0.37, p = 0.133). Cognitive reappraisal showed a strong and negative association with implicit beliefs of one’s own emotions at Time 1 (rs = -0.72, p = 0.01) and a moderate positive association at Time 2 (rs = 0.59, p = 0.01). Therefore, if adolescents considered their own emotions to be more fixed than changeable at Time 2, they reported using cognitive reappraisal during a negative experience.
We next tested if people with higher BPD traits would have higher fixed beliefs of emotion. A more fixed belief of one’s own emotions was associated with higher BPD traits (Time 1: rs = 0.44, p = 0.018; Time 2: rs = 0.58, p = 0.012). This moderate association suggests that adolescents with higher BPD traits also have a more fixed belief about their own emotion but not emotions in general (see Table 3).
Beliefs about emotions were not significantly associated with gender, age or ethnicity. Therefore, these variables were not included in further analyses.
Psychiatric diagnosis (coded as present = 1, absent = 0) was significantly associated with belief about emotion in general at Time 1 (r = -0.375, p = 0.045), but not at Time 2 (r = 0.033, p = 0.898). However, we explored this further through further analyses. Partial correlations controlling for psychiatric diagnosis for all variables were computed at Time 1 and Time 2. The significant associations remained for almost all variables, except for beliefs about one’s own emotion and emotions in general at Time 1 (r = 0.341, p = 0.071), suggesting that psychiatric diagnosis was masking this covariance. After controlling for psychiatric diagnosis, rumination and beliefs about one’s own emotions now showed a significant positive correlation at Time 2 (r = 0.501, p = 0.034). Further, the other significant findings remained significant: BPD still showed significant associations with beliefs about emotions and rumination but now rumination showed a positive association with BPD traits at Time 1 and Time 2, although only Time 1 was significant (r = 0.382, p = 0.041). Rumination still remained significantly associated with cognitive strategies and beliefs, and cognitive strategies remained significantly associated with beliefs about emotions, as before. Therefore, beliefs about one’s own emotions was associated with cognitive regulation strategies, regardless of psychiatric diagnosis.

Do Implicit Theories of Emotion Change After Engaging in a Short VR Task?

The VR game was explicitly designed to intervene in people’s beliefs about the malleability of emotions. Based on a paired samples t-test, there was a significant increase in beliefs about the changeability of one’s own emotions from Time 1 to Time 2 in response to the VR game (t (17)  = 3.31, p = 0.004, d = 0.78). For completeness, we tested if adolescents would be more likely to adopt healthy cognitive emotion regulation strategies (i.e. cognitive reappraisal) rather than a more unhelpful strategy such as rumination after the VR game. To test this, we compared scores on the key variables before and after the intervention using paired samples t-tests. Results are shown in Table 4. Effects were small and did not reach significance for changes between the two time points for implicit beliefs about emotions in general, BPD traits, rumination and cognitive reappraisal. Thus, only the target of intervention showed improvement.
Table 4
Spearman correlations between variables of interest at Time 1 and Time 2 controlling for psychiatric diagnosis
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
1.BPD Traits
0.681*
0.201
0.503*
 − 0.119
0.311
 − 0.197
0.407
2.Implicit beliefs—General
0.136
0.581*
0.053
 − 0.139
 − 0.030
0.186
 − 0.186
3.Implicit beliefs—self
0.403*
0.341
0.805*
 − 0.678*
0.501*
 − 0.182
0.192
4.Cognitive reappraisal
 − 0.474*
 − 0.167
 − 0.723*
0.834*
 − 0.538*
0.184
0.041
5.Rumination
0.382*
0.057
0.463*
 − 0.413*
0.709*
 − 0.054
0.073
6.Age (Time 1)
0.035
0.229
 − 0.292
0.152
 − 0.145
7.Gender (Time 1)
0.240
 − 0.210
 − 0.004
 − 0.189
0.079
Correlation coefficients for Time 1 are below the diagonal and Time 2 are above the diagonal, with covariance measures reported on the diagonal and in bold. Age and gender were only measured at Time 1
*p < .05
Table 5
Descriptive statistics for key variables and analysis of change from Time 1 to Time 2
Variable
Mean (SD)
Paired sample t-test
Time 1
Time 2
t
df
p
d
Implicit Beliefs -self
3.560 (0.965)
3.431 (0.835)
3.305
17
0.004
0.779
Implicit Beliefs—General
3.190 (0.850)
3.014 (0.597)
1.578
17
0.133
0.372
BPD Traits
83.483 (11.627)
81.167 (11.873)
0.21
17
0.836
0.05
Rumination
14.138 (4.711)
14.389 (3.381)
0.357
17
0.726
0.084
Cognitive reappraisal
8.276 (2.987)
9.167 (4.033)
− 1.661
17
0.115
− 0.391

Does Implicit Theory of Emotion Affect Choice of Treatment for Emotional Difficulties?

Overall, a total of 3 participants (10.3%) indicated that they would prefer psychological therapies as a treatment option, 5 participants (17.2%) selected the medication only option, 17 participants (58.6%) opted for a combination of psychological therapies and medication, whilst 4 participants (13.8%) selected the ‘No treatment’ option at Time 1. At Time 2, only 1 participant (6%) opted for psychological therapies as a treatment option, compared to 3 (17%) who opted for medication or 12 (67%) who opted for a combination of psychological therapies and medication. A total of 2 (11%) participants indicated a preference for no treatment at time 2. Given the small numbers within each category and little change over time, we did not conduct further statistical testing.

Discussion

The present study showed the effectiveness of a short VR task which aimed to modify the beliefs about the changeability of emotions for inpatient adolescents with BPD traits. In the present study, we found BPD traits were significantly associated with implicit beliefs about one’s own emotions, such that they felt they were unable to take control of their own emotions. Also, adolescents with more fixed beliefs about own emotions reported more rumination and less cognitive reappraisal. Finally, these beliefs about one’s own emotions being changeable increased over time after a one-time message through an active VR experience during which adolescents took control of emotions. Although based on a small sample, these findings suggest that interventions based on modifying beliefs about the malleability of emotions could be beneficial when using a game in VR. Virtual reality may be an engaging tool for adolescents in inpatient mental health units, since the adolescents in the present study reported experiencing “flow”.
We found that BPD traits were associated with beliefs about own emotions but not emotions in general. Young people’s beliefs about their own emotions are more pertinent for BPD traits than beliefs about emotions in general. This is supported by previous research looking at wellbeing and psychological distress in a community sample [11]. Beliefs about the self in relation to others are parallel with research into self-stigma amongst women with BPD [43]. In this context, self-stigma refers to the notion that they alone are inadequate and perceived as such from others. Self-stigma is shown to be inversely related to self-efficacy, which determines the effort an individual will expend [43]. People with high self-efficacy in a certain domain display more effort and persistence [44], which may bear some overlap with the implicit beliefs literature. Future research could consider applying the implicit beliefs framework to self-stigma of mental health difficulties, particularly BPD (Table 5).
Consistent with predictions, the present study showed that adolescents with stronger beliefs in a fixed theory of emotions were more likely to ruminate and less likely to use cognitive reappraisal to regulate their emotions. These results support previous findings amongst community samples [11, 45]. This is akin to the learned helplessness theory [46], which describes the passive behaviour a person may engage in if enduring a repeatedly painful experience that they are unable to escape or avoid. These results support the idea that a person’s perceptions of possibility to change one’s own attributes may influence the person’s use of strategies to change. If a person believes that they have no control or ability to influence the intensity of emotions as they occur, they are more likely to passively dwell on their negative mood.
Our findings have implications for treatment. Focusing on emotion regulation strategies raises the distinct possibility that promoting a changeable mindset may lead to reductions in symptoms. The main purpose of this study was to test if a brief changeable mindset intervention, delivered through a VR platform, increased the belief in the malleability of emotions. We also explored if the intervention reduced the use of rumination and increased the use of cognitive reappraisal in adolescents in a mental health inpatient unit, given that implicit beliefs about emotions were related to emotion regulation strategies. The results revealed that there was an increase in adolescents endorsing a changeable mindset of emotions whilst there was no significant change in the use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies between the two points of testing. These results are consistent with recent research in which students increasingly favoured a growth mindset of emotion following two 45-min intervention sessions [47]. The current study expanded on these findings by producing a change using a very brief and innovative mode of intervention. This has practical clinical implications for the use of such a tool in a range of settings, such as schools or mental health services, to explicitly deliver this message efficiently.
To our knowledge, the current study was the first to explore the effects of an intervention for implicit beliefs of emotion with adolescents in a clinical population. The lack of significant change in cognitive emotion regulation strategies may be attributable to the severity of mental health difficulties and thus how embedded these strategies might be within this population. The intervention in the current study explicitly addressed the possibility of change in emotions, which impacted their beliefs in this domain. It did not however, directly challenge their beliefs about the changeability of thinking styles, such as rumination, which may have led to little change in this domain. Further research may hope to explore interventions addressing various domains and the differential impact on a range of outcomes. Future research may determine if repeated exposure to the brief intervention produces any changes in cognitive strategies, treatment preferences and motivation to engage in treatment for a clinical population. This could be explored over a longer time period. With the data we collected on treatment preferences, we were unable to explore effects over time.
The results from this study should be interpreted in light of the following limitations. Firstly, a control condition was not included in which participants either received a message promoting a fixed mindset of emotions or treatment as usual. Naturally, this impedes any conclusions about specific mechanisms by which the intervention exerted its effects, over and above the effect of time. Furthermore, the sample population was receiving some form of treatment as part of their inpatient stay, we do not know if the intervention worked or if people changed their implicit beliefs because of treatment in the facility. Research has indicated that both psychological and psychiatric interventions may drive changes at a behavioural, functional, and neurological level [4850] for psychotic and non-psychotic disorders. Many of the participants were receiving psychological and psychiatric interventions during their stay on the units. These treatments could have contributed to the change in the young people’s beliefs about the changeability of emotions between the two timepoints. Thus, without a control group we are unable to determine whether the VR intervention was the only mechanism of effect.
Psychiatric diagnosis was controlled for in the initial analysis via partial correlations, but not for changes between timepoints due to insufficient power. Thus, we do not know if those with a current psychiatric diagnosis have changed less (or even more) over time than those without a diagnosis. We were also unable to see if different diagnoses might be more or less amenable to this brief intervention. We urge future research to examine this possibility.
Further, girls were overrepresented in the present study so our findings may be more representative of clinical samples of girls. Indeed, girls are more likely to have BPD traits [52]. Because the zero-order correlations involving gender were nonsignificant, gender was not controlled for in further analyses. However, we had a higher proportion of girls, which may have driven the associations that we found, so the associations between BPD traits with regulation strategies and beliefs about emotions might have been more strongly positive than if there were greater numbers of boys.
We did not measure educational attainment and intellectual ability in the present study. The presence of internalising or externalising problems, such as those tied to BPD, has been associated with educational attainment [51], so intellectual functioning could affect the ability to take on board ideas about the controllability of emotions. This would be especially important, since our task involved listening and processing a story about a person dealing with emotions. This is why we had excluded known patients with IQ less than 75. We also did this to ensure participants could complete the questionnaires.
Finally, the small sample size means that some of the statistical analysis may be underpowered. Nonetheless, current work provides a foundation for future investigations into implicit beliefs of emotions in mental health; specifically, beliefs about the changeability of one’s own emotions may play an important role in presentation and treatment of BPD difficulties. Future research ought to track the progress of participants longitudinally, to determine if the onset of symptoms precedes the implicit beliefs.
Awareness of these limitations should not detract from the importance of the practical implications of the study. Notably for this study, the initial associations observed between a fixed belief of emotions, BPD traits and cognitive emotion regulation strategies indicates that these beliefs may have an important role to play in the aetiology and subsequent treatment of these difficulties. This research has enabled a timely integration of phenomena from social and educational psychology in a clinical psychology setting.
The present study's findings support the appropriate use of VR with this population. Our findings underscore the potential benefits of brief positive psychoeducational messages using this technology. Furthermore, the intervention used in the current study could help with the acquisition of skills to manage emotions in a non-clinical population. Of practical use to conducting research, in our experience, employing the use of VR facilitated the recruitment of adolescents. Although motivation to participate was not a focus of the present study, our anecdotal data supports the possibility of using VR to engage hard-to-reach groups in psychoeducational interventions.

Summary

People’s beliefs about emotion as either fixed or changeable have been found to be associated with anxiety, depression and adjustment to major life transitions. However, very little work has been undertaken to investigate the influence of a person’s belief about emotions on emotion regulation strategies in a clinical population. The present study provides novel insights into the changeability of one's beliefs about emotion, which informs the emotion regulation strategy adopted. These beliefs may be targeted with interventions delivered via VR.
The results of this study suggest that after a one-time message via VR promoting the changeability of emotions, adolescents evidenced an increase in the belief that emotions were indeed changeable. A fixed belief of one’s own emotions was associated with higher Borderline Personality traits, higher levels of rumination and lower levels of cognitive reappraisal. These findings not only have implications for the treatment and management of emotion regulation difficulties but demonstrate the utility of VR with this clinical population. Promoting a changeable mindset raises the distinct possibility of reduction in symptoms. The use of VR highlights the benefits of brief psychoeducational messages delivered using digital technology to young people.

Acknowledgements

This research was completed as part of a doctoral research project.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the Supplementary Information.
Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Hazlett EA, Zhang J, New AS, Zelmanova Y, Goldstein KE, Haznedar MM (2013) Potentiated amygdala response to repeated emotional pictures in borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry 72(6):448–456CrossRef Hazlett EA, Zhang J, New AS, Zelmanova Y, Goldstein KE, Haznedar MM (2013) Potentiated amygdala response to repeated emotional pictures in borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry 72(6):448–456CrossRef
2.
go back to reference Newhill CE, Eack SM, Mulvey EP (2009) Violent behavior in borderline personality. J Pers Disord 23(6):541–554PubMedCrossRef Newhill CE, Eack SM, Mulvey EP (2009) Violent behavior in borderline personality. J Pers Disord 23(6):541–554PubMedCrossRef
3.
go back to reference Scott LN, Stepp SD, Pilkonis PA (2014) Prospective associations between features of borderline personality disorder, emotion dysregulation and aggression. Person Disord 5(3):278–288CrossRef Scott LN, Stepp SD, Pilkonis PA (2014) Prospective associations between features of borderline personality disorder, emotion dysregulation and aggression. Person Disord 5(3):278–288CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Larrivée M-P (2013) Borderline personality disorder in adolescents: the he-who-must-not-be-named of psychiatry. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 15(2):171–179PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Larrivée M-P (2013) Borderline personality disorder in adolescents: the he-who-must-not-be-named of psychiatry. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 15(2):171–179PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
5.
go back to reference Feenstra DJ, Hutsebaut J, Laurenssen EMP, Verheul R, Busschbach JJV, Soeteman DI (2012) The burden of disease among adolescents with personality pathology: quality of life and costs. J Pers Disord 26(4):593–604PubMedCrossRef Feenstra DJ, Hutsebaut J, Laurenssen EMP, Verheul R, Busschbach JJV, Soeteman DI (2012) The burden of disease among adolescents with personality pathology: quality of life and costs. J Pers Disord 26(4):593–604PubMedCrossRef
7.
go back to reference Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A (2009) Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66(7):764–772PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A (2009) Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66(7):764–772PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
8.
go back to reference Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Milne B, J., & Poulton, R. (2000) Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder: developmental follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60:709–717CrossRef Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Milne B, J., & Poulton, R. (2000) Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder: developmental follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60:709–717CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Hong Y, Chiu C, Dweck CS, Lin DM-S, Wan W (1999) Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: a meaning system approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 77(3):588–599CrossRef Hong Y, Chiu C, Dweck CS, Lin DM-S, Wan W (1999) Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: a meaning system approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 77(3):588–599CrossRef
11.
go back to reference De Castella K, Goldin P, Jazaieri H, Ziv M, Dweck CS, Gross JJ (2013) Beliefs about emotion: links to emotion regulation, well-being, and psychological distress. Basic Appl Soc Psychol 35(6):497–505CrossRef De Castella K, Goldin P, Jazaieri H, Ziv M, Dweck CS, Gross JJ (2013) Beliefs about emotion: links to emotion regulation, well-being, and psychological distress. Basic Appl Soc Psychol 35(6):497–505CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Kneeland ET, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Dovidio JF, Gruber J (2016) Emotion malleability beliefs influence the spontaneous regulation of social anxiety. Cognit Ther Res 40(4):496–509CrossRef Kneeland ET, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Dovidio JF, Gruber J (2016) Emotion malleability beliefs influence the spontaneous regulation of social anxiety. Cognit Ther Res 40(4):496–509CrossRef
13.
go back to reference Heary C, Hennessy E, Swords L, Corrigan P (2017) Stigma towards mental health problems during childhood and adolescence: theory, research and intervention approaches. J Child Fam Stud 26(11):2949–2959CrossRef Heary C, Hennessy E, Swords L, Corrigan P (2017) Stigma towards mental health problems during childhood and adolescence: theory, research and intervention approaches. J Child Fam Stud 26(11):2949–2959CrossRef
14.
go back to reference McGorry P (2013) Early clinical phenotypes and risk for serious mental disorders. Can J Psychiatry 58(1):19–21PubMedCrossRef McGorry P (2013) Early clinical phenotypes and risk for serious mental disorders. Can J Psychiatry 58(1):19–21PubMedCrossRef
15.
go back to reference Abela JRZ, Payne AVL, Moussaly N (2003) Cognitive vulnerability to depression in individuals with borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord 17(4):319–329PubMedCrossRef Abela JRZ, Payne AVL, Moussaly N (2003) Cognitive vulnerability to depression in individuals with borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord 17(4):319–329PubMedCrossRef
16.
go back to reference Baer RA, Sauer SE (2011) Relationships between depressive rumination, anger rumination, and borderline personality features. Personal Disord 2(2):142–150PubMedCrossRef Baer RA, Sauer SE (2011) Relationships between depressive rumination, anger rumination, and borderline personality features. Personal Disord 2(2):142–150PubMedCrossRef
17.
go back to reference Selby EA, Anestis MD, Bender TW, Joiner TE Jr (2009) An exploration of the emotional cascade model in borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 118(2):375–387PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Selby EA, Anestis MD, Bender TW, Joiner TE Jr (2009) An exploration of the emotional cascade model in borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 118(2):375–387PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
18.
go back to reference Smith JM, Grandin LD, Alloy LB, Abramson ÆLY (2006) Cognitive vulnerability to depression and axis II personality dysfunction. Cognit Ther Res 30:609–621CrossRef Smith JM, Grandin LD, Alloy LB, Abramson ÆLY (2006) Cognitive vulnerability to depression and axis II personality dysfunction. Cognit Ther Res 30:609–621CrossRef
19.
go back to reference Armey MF, Crowther JH (2008) A comparison of linear versus non-linear models of aversive among young adults. J Consult Clin Psychol 76(1):9–14PubMedCrossRef Armey MF, Crowther JH (2008) A comparison of linear versus non-linear models of aversive among young adults. J Consult Clin Psychol 76(1):9–14PubMedCrossRef
20.
go back to reference Selby EA, Anestis MD, Joiner TE (2008) Understanding the relationship between emotional and behavioral dysregulation: emotional cascades. Behav Res Ther 46(5):593–611PubMedCrossRef Selby EA, Anestis MD, Joiner TE (2008) Understanding the relationship between emotional and behavioral dysregulation: emotional cascades. Behav Res Ther 46(5):593–611PubMedCrossRef
21.
go back to reference Nolen-Hoeksema S, Stice E, Wade E, Bohon C (2007) Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol 116(1):198–207PubMedCrossRef Nolen-Hoeksema S, Stice E, Wade E, Bohon C (2007) Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol 116(1):198–207PubMedCrossRef
22.
go back to reference Lang S, Kotchoubey B, Frick C, Spitzer C, Jörgen H, Barnow S (2012) Neuroimage cognitive reappraisal in trauma-exposed women with borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage 59(2):1727–1734PubMedCrossRef Lang S, Kotchoubey B, Frick C, Spitzer C, Jörgen H, Barnow S (2012) Neuroimage cognitive reappraisal in trauma-exposed women with borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage 59(2):1727–1734PubMedCrossRef
23.
go back to reference Schulze L, Domes G, Krüger A, Berger C, Fleischer M, Prehn K, Herpertz SC (2010) Neuronal correlates of cognitive reappraisal in borderline patients with affective instability. Biol Psychiatr 69(6):564–573CrossRef Schulze L, Domes G, Krüger A, Berger C, Fleischer M, Prehn K, Herpertz SC (2010) Neuronal correlates of cognitive reappraisal in borderline patients with affective instability. Biol Psychiatr 69(6):564–573CrossRef
24.
go back to reference Giuliani NR, Gross JJ (2009) Reappraisal. In: Sander D, Scherer KR (eds) Oxford companion to the affective sciences. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 329–330 Giuliani NR, Gross JJ (2009) Reappraisal. In: Sander D, Scherer KR (eds) Oxford companion to the affective sciences. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 329–330
25.
go back to reference Gross JJ, Thompson RA (2007) Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations. In: Gross JJ (ed) Handbook of emotion regulation. Guilford Press, New York, pp 3–24 Gross JJ, Thompson RA (2007) Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations. In: Gross JJ (ed) Handbook of emotion regulation. Guilford Press, New York, pp 3–24
26.
go back to reference Troy AS, Wilhelm FH, Shallcross AJ, Mauss IB (2010) Seeing the silver lining: cognitive reappraisal ability moderates the relationship between stress and depressive symptoms. Emotion 10(6):783–795PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Troy AS, Wilhelm FH, Shallcross AJ, Mauss IB (2010) Seeing the silver lining: cognitive reappraisal ability moderates the relationship between stress and depressive symptoms. Emotion 10(6):783–795PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
go back to reference Mcrae K, Gross JJ, Weber J, Robertson ER, Sokol-hessner P, Ray RD, Ochsner KN (2012) The development of emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 7(1):11–22PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mcrae K, Gross JJ, Weber J, Robertson ER, Sokol-hessner P, Ray RD, Ochsner KN (2012) The development of emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 7(1):11–22PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
29.
go back to reference Cristea IA, Gentili C, Cotet CD, Palomba D, Barbui C, Cuijpers P (2017) Efficacy of psychotherapies for borderline personality disorder. JAMA Psychiat 74(4):319CrossRef Cristea IA, Gentili C, Cotet CD, Palomba D, Barbui C, Cuijpers P (2017) Efficacy of psychotherapies for borderline personality disorder. JAMA Psychiat 74(4):319CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Tamir M, Mauss IB (2011) Social cognitive factors in emotion regulation: implications for well-being. In: Nyklicek I, Vingerhoets A, Zeelenberg M (eds) Emotion regulation and wellbeing. Springer, New York, pp 31–47CrossRef Tamir M, Mauss IB (2011) Social cognitive factors in emotion regulation: implications for well-being. In: Nyklicek I, Vingerhoets A, Zeelenberg M (eds) Emotion regulation and wellbeing. Springer, New York, pp 31–47CrossRef
31.
go back to reference Botella C, Osma J, Quero S, Baños RM (2004) Treatment of flying phobia using virtual reality: data from a 1-year follow-up using a multiple baseline design. Clin Psychol Psychother 11:311–323CrossRef Botella C, Osma J, Quero S, Baños RM (2004) Treatment of flying phobia using virtual reality: data from a 1-year follow-up using a multiple baseline design. Clin Psychol Psychother 11:311–323CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Anderson PL, Price M, Edwards SM, Obasaju MA, Schmertz SK, Zimand E, Calamaras MR (2013) Virtual reality exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 81(5):751–760PubMedCrossRef Anderson PL, Price M, Edwards SM, Obasaju MA, Schmertz SK, Zimand E, Calamaras MR (2013) Virtual reality exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 81(5):751–760PubMedCrossRef
33.
go back to reference Fuggetta G, Rizzo S, Pobric G, Lavidor M, Walsh V (2009) Functional representation of living and non-living domains across the cerebral hemispheres: a combined event-related potential/transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci 21(2):403–414PubMedCrossRef Fuggetta G, Rizzo S, Pobric G, Lavidor M, Walsh V (2009) Functional representation of living and non-living domains across the cerebral hemispheres: a combined event-related potential/transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci 21(2):403–414PubMedCrossRef
34.
go back to reference Negen J (2018) Using VR in psychology. VR Workshop, University of Liverpool, Liverpool Negen J (2018) Using VR in psychology. VR Workshop, University of Liverpool, Liverpool
35.
go back to reference Schleider JL, Weisz JR (2017) Reducing risk for anxiety and depression in adolescents: effects of a single-session intervention teaching that personality can change. Behav Res Ther 87:170–181CrossRef Schleider JL, Weisz JR (2017) Reducing risk for anxiety and depression in adolescents: effects of a single-session intervention teaching that personality can change. Behav Res Ther 87:170–181CrossRef
37.
go back to reference Rheinberg F, Vollmeyer R, Engeser S (2003) Die Erfassung des Flow-Erlebens [The assessment of flow experience]. In: Rheinberg F (ed) Diagnostik von Selbstkonzept, Lernmotivation und Selbstregulation [Diagnosis of motivation and self-concept]. Hogrefe, Gottingen, pp 261–279 Rheinberg F, Vollmeyer R, Engeser S (2003) Die Erfassung des Flow-Erlebens [The assessment of flow experience]. In: Rheinberg F (ed) Diagnostik von Selbstkonzept, Lernmotivation und Selbstregulation [Diagnosis of motivation and self-concept]. Hogrefe, Gottingen, pp 261–279
38.
go back to reference Crick NR, Murray-Close D, Woods K (2005) Borderline personality features in childhood: a short-term longitudinal study. Dev Psychopathol 17(4):1051–1070PubMedCrossRef Crick NR, Murray-Close D, Woods K (2005) Borderline personality features in childhood: a short-term longitudinal study. Dev Psychopathol 17(4):1051–1070PubMedCrossRef
39.
go back to reference Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P (2002) Manual for the use of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire a questionnaire measuring cognitive coping strategies Nadia Garnefski Vivian Kraaij Philip Spinhoven Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P (2002) Manual for the use of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire a questionnaire measuring cognitive coping strategies Nadia Garnefski Vivian Kraaij Philip Spinhoven
40.
go back to reference Schroder HS, Dawood S, Yalch MM, Donnellan MB, Moser JS (2015) The role of implicit theories in mental health symptoms, emotion regulation, and hypothetical treatment choices in college students. Cognit Ther Res 39(2):120–139PubMedCrossRef Schroder HS, Dawood S, Yalch MM, Donnellan MB, Moser JS (2015) The role of implicit theories in mental health symptoms, emotion regulation, and hypothetical treatment choices in college students. Cognit Ther Res 39(2):120–139PubMedCrossRef
41.
go back to reference Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A (2007) G * Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the. Behav Res Methods 39(2):175–191PubMedCrossRef Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A (2007) G * Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the. Behav Res Methods 39(2):175–191PubMedCrossRef
42.
go back to reference Kim H (2013) Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor Dent Endod 38(1):52–54PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kim H (2013) Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor Dent Endod 38(1):52–54PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
43.
go back to reference Rusch N, Holzer A, Hermann C, Schramm E, Jacob GA, Bohus M, Corrigan PW (2006) Self-stigma in women with borderline personality disorder and women with social phobia. J Nerv Ment Dis 194(10):766–773PubMedCrossRef Rusch N, Holzer A, Hermann C, Schramm E, Jacob GA, Bohus M, Corrigan PW (2006) Self-stigma in women with borderline personality disorder and women with social phobia. J Nerv Ment Dis 194(10):766–773PubMedCrossRef
44.
go back to reference Bandura A (1997) Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and Company, New York Bandura A (1997) Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and Company, New York
45.
go back to reference Kneeland ET, Dovidio JF, Joormann J, Clark MS (2016) Emotion malleability beliefs, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: integrating affective and clinical science. Clin Psychol Rev 45:81–88PubMedCrossRef Kneeland ET, Dovidio JF, Joormann J, Clark MS (2016) Emotion malleability beliefs, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: integrating affective and clinical science. Clin Psychol Rev 45:81–88PubMedCrossRef
47.
go back to reference Smith EN, Romero C, Donovan B, Herter R, Paunesku D, Cohen GL, Gross JJ (2018) Emotion theories and adolescent well-being: results of an online intervention. Emotion 18(6):781–788PubMedCrossRef Smith EN, Romero C, Donovan B, Herter R, Paunesku D, Cohen GL, Gross JJ (2018) Emotion theories and adolescent well-being: results of an online intervention. Emotion 18(6):781–788PubMedCrossRef
48.
go back to reference Mancke F, Schmitt R, Winter D, Niedfeld I, Herpertz SC, Schmahl C (2018) Assessing the marks of change: how psychotherapy alters the brain structure in women with borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 43(3):171PubMedCrossRef Mancke F, Schmitt R, Winter D, Niedfeld I, Herpertz SC, Schmahl C (2018) Assessing the marks of change: how psychotherapy alters the brain structure in women with borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 43(3):171PubMedCrossRef
49.
go back to reference Marceau EM, Meuldijk D, Townsend ML, Solowij N, Grenyer BF (2018) Biomarker correlates of psychotherapy outcomes in borderline personality disorder: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 94:166–178PubMedCrossRef Marceau EM, Meuldijk D, Townsend ML, Solowij N, Grenyer BF (2018) Biomarker correlates of psychotherapy outcomes in borderline personality disorder: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 94:166–178PubMedCrossRef
50.
go back to reference Sani G, Simonetti A, Janiri D, Banaj N, Ambrosi E, De Rossi P, Spalletta G (2018) Association between duration of lithium, exposure and hippocampus/amygdala volumes in type I bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 232:341–348PubMedCrossRef Sani G, Simonetti A, Janiri D, Banaj N, Ambrosi E, De Rossi P, Spalletta G (2018) Association between duration of lithium, exposure and hippocampus/amygdala volumes in type I bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 232:341–348PubMedCrossRef
51.
go back to reference McLeod JD, Kaiser K (2004) Childhood emotional and behavioral problems and educational attainment. Am Sociol Rev 69(5):636–658CrossRef McLeod JD, Kaiser K (2004) Childhood emotional and behavioral problems and educational attainment. Am Sociol Rev 69(5):636–658CrossRef
52.
go back to reference Bradley RZ, Conklin C, Westen D (2005) The borderline personality diagnosis in adolescents: gender differences and subtypes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(9):1006–1019PubMedCrossRef Bradley RZ, Conklin C, Westen D (2005) The borderline personality diagnosis in adolescents: gender differences and subtypes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(9):1006–1019PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Borderline Personality Traits and Emotion Regulation Strategies in Adolescents: The Role of Implicit Theories
Auteurs
Jane McLachlan
Mani Mehdikhani
Beth Larham
Luna C. Muñoz Centifanti
Publicatiedatum
29-04-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 5/2022
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-021-01169-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2022

Child Psychiatry & Human Development 5/2022 Naar de uitgave