Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 5/2011

01-10-2011 | Original Article

Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria

Auteurs: Alissa J. Ellis, Christopher G. Beevers, Tony T. Wells

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Uitgave 5/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Cognitive models of depression posit that biased emotional processing contributes to the maintenance of depression. Specifically, depression has been associated with biased attention and memory for emotional information; however, few studies have examined relations between these processes. In the current study, stably dysphoric (n = 23) and non-dysphoric (n = 40) participants’ line of visual gaze was assessed while viewing a 2 × 2 array of emotionally valenced words. Incidental recognition of study stimuli was then assessed. Non-dysphoric individuals demonstrated an attentional bias for positive words, while dysphoric individuals lacked this bias. Further, fixation duration and time spent viewing positive stimuli mediated the association between dysphoria status and incidental recognition of positive words. Results suggest that a “protective bias” to focus on positive stimuli, typically observed among non-dysphoric individuals, is absent in dysphoria.
Voetnoten
1
For secondary analyses, we also calculated the percent correct within each emotion category (i.e., the number of correct yes and no responses given for dysphoric, aversive, positive, and neutral stimuli / total responses × 100). A 4 (valence: dysphoric, aversive, positive, neutral) × 2 (dysphoria group: dysphoric, non-dysphoric) repeated measures ANOVA examined whether dysphoria groups differed in percentage of words from each stimuli category correctly recognized. The valence main effect, F(3, 156) = 17.32, P < .001, partial η2 = .25, was significant. LSD pairwise comparisons indicated that percent of correct responses was lowest for dysphoric words compared to aversive, neutral and positive words (Ps < .01). The interaction between word valence and dysphoria group was non-significant, F(3, 156) = .91, P = .44, partial η2 = .02. Further, the main effect of dysphoria group was also non-significant, F(1, 52) = .01, P = .94, partial η2 = .00. Thus, percentage of correctly identified word stimuli did not differ for dysphoria groups (see Table 1).
 
2
An indirect effect is the product of the direct effects that comprise the indirect effect pathway. Although exact significance tests are available for indirect effects involving up to three variables, Cohen and Cohen (1983) suggested that a reasonable proxy for exact significance tests for indirect effects involving four or more variables is whether each of the constituent direct effects are significant. If each direct effect is significant, as in our case, Cohen and Cohen (1983) suggested that the indirect effect is also likely significant. However, these effects tend to be small, given that the indirect effect size is a product of multiple direct effects.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: New American Library. Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: New American Library.
go back to reference Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in family practice: A rapid technique. Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 81–85.PubMed Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in family practice: A rapid technique. Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 81–85.PubMed
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory: Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory: Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
go back to reference Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., & Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: A metaanalysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285–305.PubMedCrossRef Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., & Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: A metaanalysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285–305.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caseras, X., Garner, M., Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (2007). Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: An eye movement study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 491–497.PubMedCrossRef Caseras, X., Garner, M., Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (2007). Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: An eye movement study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 491–497.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chun, M. M., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2007). Interactions between attention and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 177–184.PubMedCrossRef Chun, M. M., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2007). Interactions between attention and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 177–184.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Dozois, D., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 83–89.CrossRef Dozois, D., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 83–89.CrossRef
go back to reference Dunbar, G. C., & Lishman, W. A. (1984). Depression, recognition memory and hedonic tone: A signal detection analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 376–382.PubMedCrossRef Dunbar, G. C., & Lishman, W. A. (1984). Depression, recognition memory and hedonic tone: A signal detection analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 376–382.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eizenman, M., Yu, L., Grupp, L., Eizenman, E., Ellenbogen, M., Germar, M., et al. (2003). A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 118, 117–128.PubMedCrossRef Eizenman, M., Yu, L., Grupp, L., Eizenman, E., Ellenbogen, M., Germar, M., et al. (2003). A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 118, 117–128.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gotlib, I. H., Kasch, K. L., Traill, S., Joormann, J., Arnow, B. A., & Johnson, S. L. (2004a). Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(3), 386–398.PubMedCrossRef Gotlib, I. H., Kasch, K. L., Traill, S., Joormann, J., Arnow, B. A., & Johnson, S. L. (2004a). Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(3), 386–398.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., & Joorman, J. (2004b). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(1), 127–135.CrossRef Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., & Joorman, J. (2004b). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(1), 127–135.CrossRef
go back to reference Gotlib, I. H., McLachlan, A. L., & Katz, A. N. (1988). Biases in visual attention in depressed and nondepressed individuals. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 185–200.CrossRef Gotlib, I. H., McLachlan, A. L., & Katz, A. N. (1988). Biases in visual attention in depressed and nondepressed individuals. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 185–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Gould, J. (1982). A psychometric investigation of the standard and long form Beck Depression Inventory. Psychological Reports, 51, 1167–1170.PubMed Gould, J. (1982). A psychometric investigation of the standard and long form Beck Depression Inventory. Psychological Reports, 51, 1167–1170.PubMed
go back to reference Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Va′zquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247–278.PubMedCrossRef Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Va′zquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247–278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hertel, P. T. (2000). The cognitive-initiative account of depression-related impairments in memory. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 39, pp. 47–71). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Hertel, P. T. (2000). The cognitive-initiative account of depression-related impairments in memory. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 39, pp. 47–71). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Jermann, F., Van der Linden, M., Laurencon, M., & Schmitt, B. (2009). Recollective experience during recognition of emotional words in clinical depression. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31(1), 27–35.CrossRef Jermann, F., Van der Linden, M., Laurencon, M., & Schmitt, B. (2009). Recollective experience during recognition of emotional words in clinical depression. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31(1), 27–35.CrossRef
go back to reference Joorman, J., Teachman, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Sadder and less accurate? False memory for negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(2), 412–417.CrossRef Joorman, J., Teachman, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Sadder and less accurate? False memory for negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(2), 412–417.CrossRef
go back to reference Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 125–148.CrossRef Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 125–148.CrossRef
go back to reference Kellough, J. L., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., & Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: An eye tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(11), 1238–1243.PubMedCrossRef Kellough, J. L., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., & Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: An eye tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(11), 1238–1243.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (p. 354). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (p. 354). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: Maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455.PubMedCrossRef Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: Maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2009). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 219–225. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.004.CrossRef Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2009). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 219–225. doi:10.​1016/​j.​brat.​2009.​11.​004.CrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.PubMedCrossRef MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 107–123.PubMedCrossRef MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 107–123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mathews, G. R., & Antes, J. R. (1992). Visual attention and depression: Cognitive biases in the eye fixations of the dysphoric and the nondepressed. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 359–371.CrossRef Mathews, G. R., & Antes, J. R. (1992). Visual attention and depression: Cognitive biases in the eye fixations of the dysphoric and the nondepressed. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 359–371.CrossRef
go back to reference Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRef Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRef
go back to reference McCabe, S. B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1995). Selective attention and clinical depression: Performance on a deployment-of-attention task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 241–245.PubMedCrossRef McCabe, S. B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1995). Selective attention and clinical depression: Performance on a deployment-of-attention task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 241–245.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 29, 29–45.CrossRef Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 29, 29–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311.PubMedCrossRef Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mogg, K., Mathews, A., May, J., Grove, M., Eysenck, M., & Weinman, H. (1991). Assessment of cognitive bias in anxiety and depression using a colour perception task. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 221–238.CrossRef Mogg, K., Mathews, A., May, J., Grove, M., Eysenck, M., & Weinman, H. (1991). Assessment of cognitive bias in anxiety and depression using a colour perception task. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 221–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Sears, C. R., Thomas, C., LeHuquet, J., & Johnson, J. (2010). Attentional biases in dysphoria: an eye tracking study of the allocation and disengagement of attention. Cognition and Emotion (in press). Sears, C. R., Thomas, C., LeHuquet, J., & Johnson, J. (2010). Attentional biases in dysphoria: an eye tracking study of the allocation and disengagement of attention. Cognition and Emotion (in press).
go back to reference Siegle, G. J., Granholm, E., Ingram, R. E., & Matt, G. E. (2001). Pupillary and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 49(7), 624–636.PubMedCrossRef Siegle, G. J., Granholm, E., Ingram, R. E., & Matt, G. E. (2001). Pupillary and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 49(7), 624–636.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274.CrossRef Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274.CrossRef
go back to reference Wells, T. T., & Beevers, C. G. (2010). Biased attention and dysphoria: Manipulating selective attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms. Cognition and Emotion (in press). Wells, T. T., & Beevers, C. G. (2010). Biased attention and dysphoria: Manipulating selective attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms. Cognition and Emotion (in press).
go back to reference Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and the emotional disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and the emotional disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Metagegevens
Titel
Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria
Auteurs
Alissa J. Ellis
Christopher G. Beevers
Tony T. Wells
Publicatiedatum
01-10-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Uitgave 5/2011
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-010-9305-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2011

Cognitive Therapy and Research 5/2011 Naar de uitgave