Arts and cultural engagement is a potential strategy for reducing or preventing reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors in adolescence. However, most research to date has focused on arts-based interventions and has not tested arts and cultural engagement in large population-based longitudinal studies. There are a range of potential mechanisms through which arts and cultural engagement may reduce reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors, from which attitudes towards these behaviors and self-control have been identified as key factors (Social Exclusion Unit,
2002). Yet, it is currently unclear whether ubiquitous arts and cultural engagement, which is not a targeted intervention, can lead to changes in attitudes and self-control. Research to date has not tested whether these mechanisms mediate the association between arts and cultural engagement and behavior. This study therefore aimed to investigate whether overall arts and cultural engagement in mid-adolescence influenced reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors in mid- to late adolescence, and also aimed to test two distinct potential mechanisms linking these behaviors: attitudes towards reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors and self-control. Across two large longitudinal studies in the US, there was evidence that more arts and cultural engagement was associated with reduced reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors concurrently and one to two years later, as well as some limited evidence that this association could be mediated by both attitudes and self-control.
Across both Add Health and NELS:88, more arts and cultural engagement was concurrently associated with fewer reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. Associations were sustained one to two years later, mainly through earlier reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors, except for a direct association between arts and cultural engagement and behavior one year later in Add Health. In terms of longer follow-ups, in NELS:88, this relationship was maintained four years later, but only indirectly through earlier behaviors. Similarly, in Add Health, this relationship was only maintained seven years later because of earlier behavior. This is not surprising given that prior occurrence of reportedly antisocial or criminalized behavior is a strong predictor of future behavior in adolescence (Perez et al.,
2018). Although this study hypothesized that arts and cultural engagement would reduce subsequent behavior, it is possible that this association is due to reverse causality. Adolescents with more reportedly antisocial or criminalized behavior may be less likely to engage in the arts, giving rise to the associations found here. The direct association between arts and cultural engagement and behavior one year later in Add Health indicates that this is not the case; engagement was associated with subsequent behavior even after adjusting for previous behavior. Overall, these findings provide preliminary evidence that arts and cultural engagement may reduce subsequent reportedly antisocial or criminalized behavior. This adds to the limited existing evidence that participation in performing and fine arts is associated with lower rates of skipping school (Eccles & Barber,
1999), dropping out of school (McNeal,
1995), and being arrested (Zill et al.,
1995) and contrasts with research finding no evidence for this association (Fauth et al.,
2007).
In this study, results were remarkably consistent across the two cohorts, despite differences in participant age, time scale, and measures of arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. Add Health followed individuals from early adolescence into young adulthood, whereas NELS:88 focused on the teen years. This indicates that the potential benefits of arts and cultural engagement are not limited to childhood or early adolescence but may occur across this whole developmental period. The differences in the measures included in each cohort should also be considered when interpreting these findings. As done previously (Martin et al.,
2013), this study aimed to measure overall engagement in arts and culture as an overarching construct, and thus maximized the number of items measuring engagement in each cohort. This meant that Add Health mainly included items related to participation in school arts clubs, although questions on family attendance at cultural events also loaded highly onto the latent factor. In contrast, NELS:88 included a broader measure of arts and cultural engagement, which had higher internal consistency and was most strongly determined by parent reports of attendance at arts and cultural classes, venues, and events. However, both latent variables still indicated overall levels of engagement in a range arts and cultural activities (e.g., participatory, receptive, within school, outside school) so, despite some differences, it is not surprising that both forms of arts and cultural engagement were associated with lower reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. Perhaps more different across cohorts were the measures of reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. NELS:88 focused on a narrower range of school-based reportedly antisocial behaviors, whereas Add Health measured a range of criminalized behaviors occurring mainly outside of school, all of which may also be considered antisocial. As there is currently a lack of population-level evidence on arts and cultural engagement and these behaviors, this study aimed to provide preliminary evidence on whether engagement could reduce or prevent a wide range of behaviors. The findings demonstrate that arts and cultural engagement has potential for reducing various types of behavior, all of which could be investigated in more detail in future research. In summary, the replication of findings across two cohorts that are not directly comparable suggests that the results are conceptually robust and relevant from mid-adolescence to young adulthood.
Potential Mechanisms
To extend previous findings, this study sought to identify mediating factors that could explain the relationship between arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. Two distinct potential mediators were tested: self-control and attitudes towards reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. In Add Health, more arts and cultural engagement was associated with higher self-control scores concurrently and one to two years later. This is in line with previous evidence that self-control is improved by participating in arts programs in the legal system (Bilby et al.,
2013), national orchestras (Alemán et al.,
2017), singing (Moon,
2017), dance programs (Milliken,
2002), and theater-based interventions (Farhadi & Tabatabaei Zavareh,
2020). Worse self-control was consistently associated with more reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors, also consistent with previous evidence (Wolfe & Hoffmann,
2016). Despite this, there was only weak evidence that self-control mediated the association between arts and cultural engagement and behavior at wave three, and this was inconsistent, as self-control did not mediate the association with behavior at wave two. This could be because the association between arts and cultural engagement and self-control is due to reverse causality, or because self-control is a relatively stable trait by adolescence (Britt & Gottfredson,
2011), which is not modified by arts and cultural engagement. In a previous randomized trial, a music program improved self-control in participants aged up to 14 years (Alemán et al.,
2017), providing evidence against both possibilities. Future research should therefore explore the developmental changes in associations between arts and cultural engagement, self-control, and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. Overall, even if self-control does not mediate the association with behaviors, an effect of arts and cultural engagement on self-control may still benefit a range of other outcomes, such as social functioning (Gottfredson & Hirschi,
1990), emotion regulation, wellbeing (Wenzel et al.,
2021), and other health behaviors (Boisvert et al.,
2013).
In NELS:88, more arts and cultural engagement was longitudinally associated with fewer positive perceptions of reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. This is consistent with previous evidence that young people’s attitudes may be improved by arts-based interventions in the legal system (Hughes,
2005) and community music sessions (Clennon,
2013). Fewer positive perceptions were also associated with fewer reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors concurrently, but the direction of this association was reversed longitudinally. Therefore, although there was evidence for attitudes as a mediator, this was in the opposite direction to that hypothesized: arts and cultural engagement led to fewer positive perceptions of reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors, which then led to more behaviors. However, this evidence was weak, and the coefficient was very small. This finding could be because of the questions included in NELS:88, which measured
general attitudes towards reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors but measured only
school-based behaviors. Regardless of this, attitudes are an important intermediate outcome in and of themselves (Clawson & Coolbaugh,
2001), and are associated with spending more time with peers with similar attitudes (Brendgen et al.,
2000), decreased wellbeing (Phillips & Pittman,
2007), and lower academic achievement (Chang & Le,
2005). It is thus promising that ubiquitous arts and cultural engagement may reduce positive perceptions of reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors.
Despite previous evidence for gender differences in the associations between extracurricular participation, reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors (Linville & Huebner,
2005), and school misconduct (Miller et al.,
2005), the findings in this study were mostly replicated across genders. More arts and cultural engagement was associated with fewer behaviors up to seven years later in males and females across both cohorts. The short-term associations, with behavior up to one year later, appeared slightly larger in females. This could indicate that arts and cultural engagement is more beneficial for females, which is supported by evidence that self-control only mediated this association in females, and not males. Given that this was an exploratory sensitivity analysis, future research should investigate this possibility further. In a second sensitivity analysis, the findings of this study were replicated when including only violent behaviors, such as using a weapon, fighting, and seriously injuring someone. Violence is more likely to be perceived as antisocial or a criminalized behavior across cultures, so may be a less subjective outcome. The replication of study findings (except for a lack of mediation by self-control) specifically for violent behaviors demonstrates the critical policy relevance of promoting arts and cultural engagement for the prevention of these behaviors in adolescence.
More research is needed to explore other potential moderators and mediators of the relationship between arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. When considering potential mediators, a review found that arts and cultural engagement is associated with increased empathy and prosocial behavior (Konrath & Kisida,
2021). Another systematic review found evidence that lower empathy is associated with more offending (Jolliffe & Farrington,
2004). Similarly, reductions in prosocial behavior throughout adolescence are associated with increases in aggression and “delinquency” (Padilla-Walker et al.,
2018). In addition, arts and cultural engagement may enhance emotion regulation (Fancourt & Ali,
2019) and self-esteem (Mak & Fancourt,
2019), and improvements in both emotion regulation (Rodriguez et al.,
2016) and self-esteem (Donnellan et al.,
2005) may reduce reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. These activities may also allow safe exploration of boundaries in expression, providing opportunities for learning from risk-taking. Furthermore, many arts and cultural activities are structured in nature, as they involve adult supervision, rule-guided engagement, skill development, sustained attention, and regular schedules (Mahoney,
2000). This may be beneficial, as there is evidence that structured extracurricular activities reduce reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors, in contrast to unstructured activities (Mahoney & Stattin,
2000). Future studies should seek to understand whether both structured and unstructured arts and cultural engagement reduce reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors while also accounting for the role of other structured activities in adolescence. Additionally, in this study, the broad definition of arts and cultural engagement included both participatory engagement in artistic and creative activities, as well as receptive engagement such as visiting museums and attending performances (Fancourt & Finn,
2019). Further research could investigate whether participatory and receptive engagement have differential effects on reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors in adolescence.
There is also some evidence that the positive impact of extracurricular activities on reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors relies on adolescents’ peers being engaged in similar activities (Mahoney,
2014). Adolescence is an important developmental period of social reorientation, in which individuals become more susceptible to peer influence and more sensitive to peer rejection (Andrews et al.,
2020). Although measures of peer reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors or peer arts and cultural engagement could not be included in this study, arts and cultural activities can provide a positive environment in which adolescents are likely to be involved with a peer group who may encourage healthy behaviors and lifestyle choices. Future research should investigate the role of adolescents’ peer networks in the relationship between arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors.
Implications and Challenges in “Delinquency” Research
The findings of this study indicate that arts and cultural engagement could have both short-term and enduring effects on adolescents’ lives, providing opportunities to realize positive developmental outcomes. This demonstrates the need for further research on the role of arts and cultural engagement in supporting health-promoting behaviors in adolescence. In addition to advancing related research, the next priority should be to ensure different forms of arts and cultural engagement can be made accessible, socially inclusive, and culturally appropriate for all young people. This is particularly important given that, when adjusted for inflation, funding for the arts in schools has decreased by 30% over the past twenty years in the US (Jung,
2018). Additionally, the US government has repeatedly proposed cutting all federal arts and cultural funding, and there are frequent debates about the extent to which the arts should be part of school curricula (e.g., McGlone,
2020). Given the time that children spend in school, as well as barriers to and increased social gradients in participation in arts and cultural activities outside of school (Mak & Fancourt,
2021), this study’s findings underscore the importance of curricular and extracurricular arts programs at schools and the need for policies that ensure funding for arts in education. These findings also support the use of the arts in rehabilitation programs for adolescents in the legal system, both for males and females, and for those with a history of non-violent and violent behavior. Additionally, work is currently underway in other countries such as the UK as well as in pilots in the US to bring arts to adolescents via social prescribing (SP) schemes. SP usually involves a health, social or educational professional referring an adolescent to a link worker, who develops a plan that connects the adolescent with psychosocial activities such as the arts with the aim of improving psychological or social wellbeing. Preliminary studies involving adolescents have shown benefits for mental health, the development of social networks, reductions in loneliness, and reductions in feelings of stigma (Bertotti et al.,
2020). Consequently, SP schemes could be explored further for adolescents considered at risk for developing reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors.
Language is a significant issue in this area of research. Throughout this study, the term “reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors” has been used as an alternative to “delinquent behaviors”. “Reportedly antisocial” highlights that pro- and antisociality are assigned rather than inherent types of behavior. “Criminalized” highlights that behavior does not have inherent legality or illegality but that certain behaviors and circumstances have been criminalized, including for particular groups (such as alcohol consumption by under-18s). This characterization may or may not accurately and adequately reflect an adolescent’s perception of their own behavior. Although the current literature regularly uses the term “delinquency”, this term is problematic, as it contributes to the association of criminality with behaviors that often result from adversity. The significance of early life adversity and maltreatment to health, development, and life course trajectories is well-established (Struck et al.,
2021). Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) include abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction, and over 60% of Americans have experienced at least one ACE (CDC,
2021). Young people with these experiences are often at greater risk of reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors and becoming involved in the legal system (Baglivio et al.,
2015). Terms like “delinquency” also lack consideration of other important factors such as neurobiology (Zijlmans et al.,
2021) and neurodiversity (Lollini,
2018). The noun “delinquent” labels and criminalizes individuals themselves. This kind of labelling could be particularly detrimental in school settings, where much development and identity formation take place.
Additionally, assuming that reductions of certain types of adolescent behaviors is an inherent public good is problematic. Adolescent behaviors that have been associated with criminality, pathologies, or antisociality may in some cases be adaptive responses to destructive or oppressive environments. For example, asserting control outside of a home that does not allow normal levels of control or self-determination may be adaptive, as exercising control is an essential developmental behavior (Bandura,
2006). In addition, reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors may elicit positive social responses from peers or be related to different biological antecedents or cultural meanings across groups (Chen,
2020). Attitudes towards reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors, including the extent to which adolescents consider that behaviors such as belonging to gangs or stealing are acceptable, may be determined by how safe they feel in their home or school environment and experiences with scarcity of food or other necessary goods. The role of these structural and social determinants may be obscured by the criminalization or pathologizing of affected adolescents. Assuming that behaviors are detrimental and prioritizing their reduction can convey apparent concern for adolescents’ health while avoiding systemic responsibility for that health by acting at community, organizational, and policy levels. The prevention or reduction of behaviors that have historically been referred to as “delinquent” should not be presumed to indicate a public health success, particularly without analyses of related social and structural determinants of health.
Furthermore, adolescents’ behaviors cannot be assumed to be accurately interpreted or reported by adults and those with relative power. For example, gender norms and stereotypes have caused girls in the juvenile legal system to be labeled as problematically aggressive due to behaviors that would be considered common among boys (Golden,
2017). Attributions and interpretations of delinquency, illegality, sociality, and even health are subjective, and may not be shared across age, gender, race/ethnicity, cultures, or historical moments. As a result, it is critical that data related to adolescent behaviors be interpreted with regard for the potential effects of biases. Despite previous studies’ failures to consistently navigate critical nuances, their data do provide information regarding adolescent behaviors and associations with health outcomes and may also generate insights into more equitable means of collecting and analyzing data in the future. By considering these issues, and not using the problematic language of “delinquency”, this article contributes to this dual effort. In the future, researchers must a) recognize the need for changes in terminology around “delinquency”; b) acknowledge the fact that behaviors are often labeled and interpreted by others; and c) undertake more research into the effects of social and structural determinants of health on adolescent behaviors and health outcomes.
Strengths and Limitations
This study has several strengths. Two large nationally representative longitudinal studies were used, and findings were replicated despite differences in the cohorts, indicating that the results are conceptually robust. Add Health and NELS:88 were chosen for their rich array of data on arts and cultural engagement and covariates, meaning that sociodemographic factors that are likely to confound the association between arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors could be included (Feldman Farb & Matjasko,
2012). In analyses, participants were clustered within schools, accounting for the fact that adolescents within schools are more similar to each other than to adolescents at other schools. Additionally, reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors were modelled across three waves in each cohort. This is important as these behaviors may change developmentally (Cook et al.,
2015).
This study also has some limitations. It was limited by the measures of arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors in Add Health and NELS:88, which were not consistent across waves. More sophisticated approaches such as growth curve models therefore could not be used. The internal consistency of these measures differed across cohorts and waves, which could be a result of developmental changes or indicate that heterogeneous constructs were grouped together. However, given that measures were represented by latent factors in structural equation models (SEMs), Cronbach’s alpha is likely to be an underestimate of the reliability of these measures (Sijtsma,
2009), as it is not derived from the parameters of the factor model (Raykov,
1997). This approach assumes that the observed information reflects unmeasurable constructs and accounts for measurement error in latent variables by simultaneously estimating measurement and structural models (Kline,
2015). Yet, using SEMs assumes linear relations between these factors, which may not be appropriate (e.g., Matjasko et al.,
2019). Future research should investigate the developmental trajectories of arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors in more detail. Additionally, the measure of reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors in NELS:88 included getting into trouble for behavior at school, which may have been influenced by teachers’ beliefs and behavior, but variation in teacher conduct could not be accounted for in analyses.
Furthermore, Add Health and NELS:88 are relatively old cohorts. While these cohorts included nationally representative samples of the target populations (1994-1995 grades 7-11 in Add Health, 1988 grade 8 in NELS:88), these samples are no longer representative of the current US population (US Census Bureau,
2021). It is likely that the associations observed between arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors are due to enduring psychological and social mechanisms, in which case the age of the data is not important. Nevertheless, it is possible that these associations have been altered by changes in children’s educational and developmental environments. Advances in external factors such as technology, educational styles, and behavioral management techniques may have modified the relationship between arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. The findings of this study should thus by replicated with more recent data, although replication is challenging due to the lack of current data on arts and cultural engagement in representative cohorts.
Although analyses were adjusted for a wide range of sociodemographic factors, it is possible that other characteristics influence both arts and cultural engagement (Mak & Fancourt,
2021) and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors (Shader,
2000), as there is a social gradient in both behaviors. It remains difficult to disentangle whether the association between arts and cultural engagement and reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors is due to self-selection or because engaging in arts and cultural activities reduces those behaviors. Additionally, a biased sample may have been included in both cohorts due to attrition, as participants in this study were more likely to be engaged in arts and cultural activities, younger, White, live in a rural area, and have higher socioeconomic position than the baseline Add Health and NELS:88 samples. Weighting adjusted the distribution of both samples accordingly. Future research should also examine whether these associations are moderated by age, race/ethnicity, and other factors that may increase adolescents’ participation in reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors (Feldman & Matjasko,
2005).
This study used an overly simple race/ethnicity variable (White, Black, Asian/Pacific Islander, Other) due to small numbers in non-White groups. This approach conflates experiences across diverse racial/ethnic groups, which might be particularly problematic as these groups may not have equal access to artistic and cultural resources (Bone et al.,
2021). Future research should thus use more diverse samples and collect more nuanced data on race/ethnicity, while considering the persistence of structural racism in US communities, schools, and legal systems (Williams,
2012). Additionally, many arts and cultural activities take place within groups that are not well represented in this sample, and these activities may not be included in the narrow definitions of arts and cultural engagement used in Add Health and NELS:88. Finally, the findings are limited in that they rely on survey constructs that, to the authors’ knowledge, were not informed by adolescents at the time of the studies, and thus may not adequately reflect their experiences with arts and culture and with reportedly antisocial or criminalized behaviors. To help address such issues in the future, adolescents themselves should be included and given leadership roles in research related to their lives and behaviors (see Checkoway,
2011).