Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2018

15-06-2017 | Original Article

Action effect features, but not anatomical features, determine the Backward Crosstalk Effect: evidence from crossed-hands experiments

Auteurs: Sandra Renas, Moritz Durst, Markus Janczyk

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The Backward Crosstalk Effect (BCE) indicates that response features of Task 2 in a dual-task paradigm influence even Task 1 performance. However, it can be assumed that responses are represented with multiple features in the cognitive system. In this regard, Ideomotor Theory suggests action effects as one central response feature in human action control and an earlier study by Janczyk, Pfister, Hommel, and Kunde (Cognition 132: 30–43, 2014) already provided some evidence that action effects are a crucial determinant of the BCE. The present study aimed to further investigate which response features are critical for the BCE. Therefore, a crossed-hands manipulation was implemented in two experiments to examine whether the spatial position of the actual response and thus its associated action effects or the body-side of the effectors’ anatomical connection determine the BCE. Analyses revealed that even when participants press a left response key with the right hand and vice versa, the usual BCE occurs. These results indicate that the BCE depends on spatial features of the action effects rather than on anatomical features and thus provide additional support for the assumption that action effects have an important influence on the BCE and on action control in general.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Recent studies have reported a sequential modulation of the BCE with large BCEs following compatible trials and much smaller or even reversed BCEs following incompatible trials (Janczyk, 2016a; Scherbaum, Gottschalk, Dshemuchadse, & Fischer, 2015). Although it was not the main purpose of the present study, it nonetheless offered a chance to test whether the sequential modulation generalizes to vocal responses and the crossed-hands setting (which it does). The respective analyses are reported in the Appendix.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Dignath, D., & Janczyk, M. (2016). Anticipation of delayed action-effects: Learning when an effect occurs, without knowing what this effect will be. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-016-0797-7. Dignath, D., & Janczyk, M. (2016). Anticipation of delayed action-effects: Learning when an effect occurs, without knowing what this effect will be. Psychological Research. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-016-0797-7.
go back to reference Eenshuistra, R. M., Weidema, M. A., & Hommel, B. (2004). Development of the acquisition and control of action–effect associations. Acta Psychologica, 115, 185–209.CrossRefPubMed Eenshuistra, R. M., Weidema, M. A., & Hommel, B. (2004). Development of the acquisition and control of action–effect associations. Acta Psychologica, 115, 185–209.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ellenbogen, R., & Meiran, N. (2008). Working Memory involvement in dual-task performance: evidence from the backward compatibility effect. Memory & Cognition, 36, 968–978.CrossRef Ellenbogen, R., & Meiran, N. (2008). Working Memory involvement in dual-task performance: evidence from the backward compatibility effect. Memory & Cognition, 36, 968–978.CrossRef
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.PubMed Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.PubMed
go back to reference Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73. Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMed Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Eglau, B. (2002). Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Psychological Research, 66, 260–273.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., & Eglau, B. (2002). Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Psychological Research, 66, 260–273.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference James, W. (1890/1981). The principles of psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University. James, W. (1890/1981). The principles of psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University.
go back to reference Janczyk, M. (2016a). Sequential modulation of backward crosstalk and task-shielding in dual-tasking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 631–647.PubMed Janczyk, M. (2016a). Sequential modulation of backward crosstalk and task-shielding in dual-tasking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 631–647.PubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M. (2016b). Die Rolle von Handlungszielen bei der Entstehung von Doppelaufgabenkosten. Psychologische Rundschau, 67, 237–249.CrossRef Janczyk, M. (2016b). Die Rolle von Handlungszielen bei der Entstehung von Doppelaufgabenkosten. Psychologische Rundschau, 67, 237–249.CrossRef
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Büschelberger, J., & Herbort, O. (2017). Larger between-task crosstalk in children than in adults: behavioral results from the backward-crosstalk paradigm and a diffusion model analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 155, 95–112.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Büschelberger, J., & Herbort, O. (2017). Larger between-task crosstalk in children than in adults: behavioral results from the backward-crosstalk paradigm and a diffusion model analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 155, 95–112.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Durst, M., & Ulrich, R. (2017). Action selection by temporally distal goal states. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 467–473.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Durst, M., & Ulrich, R. (2017). Action selection by temporally distal goal states. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 467–473.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Heinemann, A., & Pfister, R. (2012). Instant attraction: immediate action-effect bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 446.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Janczyk, M., Heinemann, A., & Pfister, R. (2012). Instant attraction: immediate action-effect bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 446.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2014). The role of effect grouping in free-choice response selection. Acta Psychologica, 150, 49–54.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2014). The role of effect grouping in free-choice response selection. Acta Psychologica, 150, 49–54.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132, 30–43.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132, 30–43.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Wallmeier, G., & Kunde, W. (2014). Exceptions from the PRP effect? A comparison of prepared and unconditioned reflexes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 776–786.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Wallmeier, G., & Kunde, W. (2014). Exceptions from the PRP effect? A comparison of prepared and unconditioned reflexes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 776–786.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Renas, S., & Durst, M. (2017). Identifying the locus of compatibility-based backward crosstalk: Evidence from an extended PRP paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi:10.1037/xhp0000445. Janczyk, M., Renas, S., & Durst, M. (2017). Identifying the locus of compatibility-based backward crosstalk: Evidence from an extended PRP paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi:10.​1037/​xhp0000445.
go back to reference Koch, I., & Kunde, W. (2002). Verbal response-effect compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1297–1303.CrossRef Koch, I., & Kunde, W. (2002). Verbal response-effect compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1297–1303.CrossRef
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.PubMed Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.PubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., & Wühr, P. (2004). Actions blind to conceptually overlapping stimuli. Psychological Research, 68, 199–207.PubMed Kunde, W., & Wühr, P. (2004). Actions blind to conceptually overlapping stimuli. Psychological Research, 68, 199–207.PubMed
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus–response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238.CrossRef Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus–response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.CrossRefPubMed Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.PubMed Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.PubMed
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., Umiltà, U., & Ladavas, E. (1984). Compatibility due to coding of the relative position of the effectors. Acta Psychologica, 57, 133–143.CrossRefPubMed Nicoletti, R., Umiltà, U., & Ladavas, E. (1984). Compatibility due to coding of the relative position of the effectors. Acta Psychologica, 57, 133–143.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45.CrossRef Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Pfister, R., Heinemann, A., Kiesel, A., Thomaschke, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Do endogenous and exogenous action control compete for perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 279–284.PubMed Pfister, R., Heinemann, A., Kiesel, A., Thomaschke, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Do endogenous and exogenous action control compete for perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 279–284.PubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Harleß’ Apparatus of Will: 150 years later. Psychological Research, 76, 561–565.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Harleß’ Apparatus of Will: 150 years later. Psychological Research, 76, 561–565.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9, 74–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9, 74–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2013). Dissecting the response in response-effect compatibility. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 647–655.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2013). Dissecting the response in response-effect compatibility. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 647–655.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rieger, M. (2007). Letters as visual action-effects in skilled typing. Acta Psychologica, 126(2), 138–153.CrossRefPubMed Rieger, M. (2007). Letters as visual action-effects in skilled typing. Acta Psychologica, 126(2), 138–153.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Scherbaum, S., Gottschalk, C., Dshemuchadse, M., & Fischer, R. (2015). Action dynamics in multitasking: the impact of additional task factors on the execution of the prioritized motor movement. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 934.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Scherbaum, S., Gottschalk, C., Dshemuchadse, M., & Fischer, R. (2015). Action dynamics in multitasking: the impact of additional task factors on the execution of the prioritized motor movement. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 934.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Schubert, T., Fischer, R., & Stelzel, C. (2008). Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 376–397.PubMed Schubert, T., Fischer, R., & Stelzel, C. (2008). Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 376–397.PubMed
go back to reference Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 943–974.CrossRefPubMed Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 943–974.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68, 176–188.CrossRefPubMed Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68, 176–188.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.CrossRef Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.CrossRef
go back to reference Thomson, S. J., Danis, L. K., & Watter, S. (2015). PRP training shows Task1 response selection is the locus of the backward response compatibility effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 212–218.CrossRef Thomson, S. J., Danis, L. K., & Watter, S. (2015). PRP training shows Task1 response selection is the locus of the backward response compatibility effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 212–218.CrossRef
go back to reference Thomson, S. J., & Watter, S. (2013). Information continuity across the response selection bottleneck: early parallel Task 2 response activation contributes to overt Task 2 performance. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 75, 934–953.CrossRef Thomson, S. J., & Watter, S. (2013). Information continuity across the response selection bottleneck: early parallel Task 2 response activation contributes to overt Task 2 performance. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 75, 934–953.CrossRef
go back to reference Wallace, R. J. (1971). S–R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.CrossRefPubMed Wallace, R. J. (1971). S–R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Watter, S., & Logan, G. D. (2006). Parallel response selection in dual-task situations. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 254–277.CrossRef Watter, S., & Logan, G. D. (2006). Parallel response selection in dual-task situations. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 254–277.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Action effect features, but not anatomical features, determine the Backward Crosstalk Effect: evidence from crossed-hands experiments
Auteurs
Sandra Renas
Moritz Durst
Markus Janczyk
Publicatiedatum
15-06-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0873-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2018

Psychological Research 5/2018 Naar de uitgave