Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2018

31-05-2017 | Original Article

Preparation of timing structure involves two independent sub-processes

Auteurs: Dana Maslovat, Romeo Chua, Stuart T. Klapp, Ian M. Franks

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The current study examined the processes involved in the preparation of sequencing and timing initiation for multi-component responses. In two experiments, participants performed a reaction time (RT) task involving a three key-press sequence with either a simple (isochronous) or complex (non-isochronous) timing structure. Conditions involved a precue that provided information about all features of the movement (simple RT), no features of the movement (choice RT), sequencing only, or timing structure only. When sequencing was precued, RT decreased significantly as compared to choice RT, indicative of advance preparation of sequencing. When timing was precued, RT decreased significantly compared to choice RT when the timing structure was simple, suggesting that some aspect of timing preparation can occur prior to the go stimulus. However, even when the timing structure was known in advance, RT was still affected by timing complexity, confirming that some aspect of timing preparation cannot occur until after the onset of the stimulus and thus occurs during the RT interval. To explain these findings, we propose a two-component model of preparation for the timing initiation structure in which timing selection occurs in advance but timing implementation must occur following the go signal. These results support and extend previous findings regarding the independence of the processes associated with response sequencing and timing initiation.
Literatuur
go back to reference Bortoletto, M., Cook, A., & Cunnington, R. (2011). Motor timing and the preparation for sequential actions. Brain and Cognition, 75(2), 196–204.CrossRefPubMed Bortoletto, M., Cook, A., & Cunnington, R. (2011). Motor timing and the preparation for sequential actions. Brain and Cognition, 75(2), 196–204.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bortoletto, M., & Cunnington, R. (2010). Motor timing and motor sequencing contribute differently to the preparation for voluntary movement. Neuroimage, 49(4), 3338–3348.CrossRefPubMed Bortoletto, M., & Cunnington, R. (2010). Motor timing and motor sequencing contribute differently to the preparation for voluntary movement. Neuroimage, 49(4), 3338–3348.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carlsen, A. N., Maslovat, D., & Franks, I. M. (2012). Preparation for voluntary movement in healthy and clinical populations: Evidence from startle. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(1), 21–33.CrossRefPubMed Carlsen, A. N., Maslovat, D., & Franks, I. M. (2012). Preparation for voluntary movement in healthy and clinical populations: Evidence from startle. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(1), 21–33.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Church, R. M., Guilhardi, P., Keen, R., MacInnis, M., & Kirkpatrick, K. (2003). Simultaneous temporal processing. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and Mind (pp. 3–19). Gottingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber. Church, R. M., Guilhardi, P., Keen, R., MacInnis, M., & Kirkpatrick, K. (2003). Simultaneous temporal processing. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and Mind (pp. 3–19). Gottingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.
go back to reference Donders, F. C. (1868/1969). Over de snelheid van psychische processen [On the speed of mental processes]. In W. G. Koster (Ed.), Attention and performance II. Acta psychologica (Vol. 30, pp. 412–431). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company. Donders, F. C. (1868/1969). Over de snelheid van psychische processen [On the speed of mental processes]. In W. G. Koster (Ed.), Attention and performance II. Acta psychologica (Vol. 30, pp. 412–431). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company.
go back to reference Goodman, D., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1980). Are movements prepared in parts? Not under compatible (naturalized) conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(4), 475–495.CrossRef Goodman, D., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1980). Are movements prepared in parts? Not under compatible (naturalized) conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(4), 475–495.CrossRef
go back to reference Haith, A. M., Pakpoor, J., & Krakauer, J. W. (2016). Independence of movement preparation and movement initiation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(10), 3007–3015.CrossRefPubMed Haith, A. M., Pakpoor, J., & Krakauer, J. W. (2016). Independence of movement preparation and movement initiation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(10), 3007–3015.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Henry, F. M., & Rogers, D. E. (1960). Increased response latency for complicated movements and a “memory drum” theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly, 31, 448–458. Henry, F. M., & Rogers, D. E. (1960). Increased response latency for complicated movements and a “memory drum” theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly, 31, 448–458.
go back to reference Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2), 136–152.CrossRefPubMed Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2), 136–152.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 387–403.CrossRef Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 387–403.CrossRef
go back to reference Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: The role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1015–1027. Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: The role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1015–1027.
go back to reference Klapp, S. T. (2003). Reaction time analysis of two types of motor preparation for speech articulation: action as a sequence of chunks. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(2), 135–150.CrossRefPubMed Klapp, S. T. (2003). Reaction time analysis of two types of motor preparation for speech articulation: action as a sequence of chunks. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(2), 135–150.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klapp, S. T. (2010). Comments on the classic Henry and Rogers (1960) paper on its 50th anniversary: Resolving the issue of simple versus choice reaction time. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81(1), 108–112.CrossRefPubMed Klapp, S. T. (2010). Comments on the classic Henry and Rogers (1960) paper on its 50th anniversary: Resolving the issue of simple versus choice reaction time. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81(1), 108–112.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klapp, S. T., & Jagacinski, R. J. (2011). Gestalt principles in the control of motor action. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 443–462.CrossRefPubMed Klapp, S. T., & Jagacinski, R. J. (2011). Gestalt principles in the control of motor action. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 443–462.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kleinman, M. R., Sohn, H., & Lee, D. (2016). A two-stage model of concurrent interval timing in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 116(3), 1068–1081.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kleinman, M. R., Sohn, H., & Lee, D. (2016). A two-stage model of concurrent interval timing in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 116(3), 1068–1081.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–136). New York: Wiley. Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–136). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Leonard, J. A. (1953). Advance information in sensorimotor skills. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 5, 41–49.CrossRef Leonard, J. A. (1953). Advance information in sensorimotor skills. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 5, 41–49.CrossRef
go back to reference Leuthold, H., & Jentzsch, I. (2011). Are temporal response features prepared in fixed order? Inferences from movement-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 48(5), 633–644.CrossRefPubMed Leuthold, H., & Jentzsch, I. (2011). Are temporal response features prepared in fixed order? Inferences from movement-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 48(5), 633–644.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (1996). Partial advance information and response preparation: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(3), 307–323.CrossRef Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (1996). Partial advance information and response preparation: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(3), 307–323.CrossRef
go back to reference Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2004). Event-related potentials as indices of time processing: A review. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 89–104.CrossRef Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2004). Event-related potentials as indices of time processing: A review. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 89–104.CrossRef
go back to reference Magnuson, C. E., Wright, D., & Verwey, W. (2004). Changes in the incidental context impacts search but not loading of the motor buffer. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 57(5), 935–951.CrossRef Magnuson, C. E., Wright, D., & Verwey, W. (2004). Changes in the incidental context impacts search but not loading of the motor buffer. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 57(5), 935–951.CrossRef
go back to reference Maslovat, D., Chua, R., Klapp, S. T., & Franks, I. M. (2016). Independent Planning of Timing and Sequencing for Complex Movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1158–1172.PubMed Maslovat, D., Chua, R., Klapp, S. T., & Franks, I. M. (2016). Independent Planning of Timing and Sequencing for Complex Movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1158–1172.PubMed
go back to reference Maslovat, D., Klapp, S. T., Jagacinski, R. J., & Franks, I. M. (2014). Control of response timing occurs during the simple reaction time interval but on-line for choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 2005–2021.PubMed Maslovat, D., Klapp, S. T., Jagacinski, R. J., & Franks, I. M. (2014). Control of response timing occurs during the simple reaction time interval but on-line for choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 2005–2021.PubMed
go back to reference Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. BioEssays, 22(1), 94–103.CrossRefPubMed Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. BioEssays, 22(1), 94–103.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(4), 444–474.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(4), 444–474.CrossRef
go back to reference Shea, C. H., & Wulf, G. (2005). Schema theory: A critical appraisal and reevaluation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(2), 85–101.CrossRefPubMed Shea, C. H., & Wulf, G. (2005). Schema theory: A critical appraisal and reevaluation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(2), 85–101.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shin, J. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Spatial and temporal sequence learning in patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(8), 1232–1243.CrossRefPubMed Shin, J. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Spatial and temporal sequence learning in patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(8), 1232–1243.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Ullen, F., & Bengtsson, S. L. (2003). Independent processing of the temporal and ordinal structure of movement sequences. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(6), 3725–3735.CrossRefPubMed Ullen, F., & Bengtsson, S. L. (2003). Independent processing of the temporal and ordinal structure of movement sequences. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(6), 3725–3735.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Verwey, W. B., Shea, C. H., & Wright, D. L. (2015). A cognitive framework for explaining serial processing and sequence execution strategies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 54–77.CrossRef Verwey, W. B., Shea, C. H., & Wright, D. L. (2015). A cognitive framework for explaining serial processing and sequence execution strategies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 54–77.CrossRef
go back to reference Wright, D. L., Black, C. B., Immink, M. A., Brueckner, S., & Magnuson, C. (2004). Long-term motor programming improvements occur via concatenation of movement sequences during random but not during blocked practice. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36(1), 39–50.CrossRefPubMed Wright, D. L., Black, C. B., Immink, M. A., Brueckner, S., & Magnuson, C. (2004). Long-term motor programming improvements occur via concatenation of movement sequences during random but not during blocked practice. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36(1), 39–50.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wright, D. L., Black, C., Park, J. H., & Shea, C. H. (2001). Planning and executing simple movements: contributions of relative-time and overall-duration specification. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(3), 273–285.CrossRefPubMed Wright, D. L., Black, C., Park, J. H., & Shea, C. H. (2001). Planning and executing simple movements: contributions of relative-time and overall-duration specification. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(3), 273–285.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Preparation of timing structure involves two independent sub-processes
Auteurs
Dana Maslovat
Romeo Chua
Stuart T. Klapp
Ian M. Franks
Publicatiedatum
31-05-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0877-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2018

Psychological Research 5/2018 Naar de uitgave