Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 8/2021

03-01-2021 | Review

The effect of handedness on mental rotation of hands: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Auteurs: H. G. Jones, F. A. Braithwaite, L. M. Edwards, R. S. Causby, M. Conson, T. R. Stanton

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Body-specific mental rotation is thought to rely upon internal representations of motor actions. Handedness is a source of distinctly different motor experience that shapes the development of such internal representations. Yet, the influence of handedness upon hand mental rotation has never been systematically evaluated. Five databases were searched for studies evaluating hand left/right judgement tasks in adults. Two independent reviewers performed screening, data extraction, and critical appraisal. Eighty-seven datasets were included, with 72 datasets pooled; all had unclear/high risk of bias. Meta-analyses showed that right-handers were faster, but not more accurate, than left-handers at hand mental rotation. A unique effect of handedness was found on performance facilitation for images corresponding to the dominant hand. Meta-analyses showed that right-handers were quicker at identifying images of right hands than left hands—a dominance advantage not evident in left-handers. Differing hand representations (more lateralised hand dominance in right-handers) likely underpin these findings. Given potential differences between hand preference and motor performance, future research exploring their distinct contributions to mental rotation is warranted.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Altman, D., Egger, M., Pocock, S., Gøtzsche, P., & Vandenbroucke, J. (2007). Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 335(7624), 806–808.CrossRef Altman, D., Egger, M., Pocock, S., Gøtzsche, P., & Vandenbroucke, J. (2007). Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 335(7624), 806–808.CrossRef
go back to reference Amunts, K., Jäncke, L., Mohlberg, H., Steinmetz, H., & Zilles, K. (2000). Interhemispheric asymmetry of the human motor cortex related to handedness and gender. Neuropsychologia, 38(3), 304–312.PubMedCrossRef Amunts, K., Jäncke, L., Mohlberg, H., Steinmetz, H., & Zilles, K. (2000). Interhemispheric asymmetry of the human motor cortex related to handedness and gender. Neuropsychologia, 38(3), 304–312.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ashton, R., McFarland, K., Walsh, F., & White, K. (1978). Imagery ability and the identification of hands: A chronometric analysis. Acta psychologica, 42(4), 253–262.PubMedCrossRef Ashton, R., McFarland, K., Walsh, F., & White, K. (1978). Imagery ability and the identification of hands: A chronometric analysis. Acta psychologica, 42(4), 253–262.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Betts, G. H. (1909). The distribution and functions of mental imagery, vol. 26. Columbia: Teachers College, Columbia University. Betts, G. H. (1909). The distribution and functions of mental imagery, vol. 26. Columbia: Teachers College, Columbia University.
go back to reference Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Frey, S., & Evans, A. (1995). Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92(24), 11180–11184.CrossRef Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Frey, S., & Evans, A. (1995). Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92(24), 11180–11184.CrossRef
go back to reference Boonstra, A. M., de Vries, S. J., Veenstra, E., Tepper, M., Feenstra, W., & Otten, E. (2012). Using the hand laterality judgement task to assess motor imagery: A study of practice effects in repeated measurements. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 35(3), 278–280.PubMedCrossRef Boonstra, A. M., de Vries, S. J., Veenstra, E., Tepper, M., Feenstra, W., & Otten, E. (2012). Using the hand laterality judgement task to assess motor imagery: A study of practice effects in repeated measurements. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 35(3), 278–280.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brady, N., Maguinness, C., & Choisdealbha, Á. N. (2011). My hand or yours? Markedly different sensitivity to egocentric and allocentric views in the hand laterality task. PLoS ONE, 6(8), e23316.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Brady, N., Maguinness, C., & Choisdealbha, Á. N. (2011). My hand or yours? Markedly different sensitivity to egocentric and allocentric views in the hand laterality task. PLoS ONE, 6(8), e23316.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Braun, N., Kranczioch, C., Liepert, J., Dettmers, C., Zich, C., Büsching, I., et al. (2017). Motor imagery impairment in postacute stroke patients. Neural Plasticity, 2017(2017), 1–13.CrossRef Braun, N., Kranczioch, C., Liepert, J., Dettmers, C., Zich, C., Büsching, I., et al. (2017). Motor imagery impairment in postacute stroke patients. Neural Plasticity, 2017(2017), 1–13.CrossRef
go back to reference Briggs, G. G., & Nebes, R. D. (1975). Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex, 11(3), 230–238.PubMedCrossRef Briggs, G. G., & Nebes, R. D. (1975). Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex, 11(3), 230–238.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351–367.CrossRef Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1987). The measurement of handedness. Brain and Cognition, 6(2), 175–183.PubMedCrossRef Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1987). The measurement of handedness. Brain and Cognition, 6(2), 175–183.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Choisdealbha, Á. N., Brady, N., & Maguinness, C. (2011). Differing roles for the dominant and non-dominant hands in the hand laterality task. Experimental Brain Research, 211(1), 73–85.CrossRef Choisdealbha, Á. N., Brady, N., & Maguinness, C. (2011). Differing roles for the dominant and non-dominant hands in the hand laterality task. Experimental Brain Research, 211(1), 73–85.CrossRef
go back to reference Cocksworth, R. L., & Punt, T. D. (2013). When the left hand does not know what the left hand is doing: Response mode affects mental rotation of hands. Experimental Brain Research, 228(1), 87–95.PubMedCrossRef Cocksworth, R. L., & Punt, T. D. (2013). When the left hand does not know what the left hand is doing: Response mode affects mental rotation of hands. Experimental Brain Research, 228(1), 87–95.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conson, M., Cecere, R., Baiano, C., et al. (2020a). Implicit motor imagery and the lateral occipitotemporal cortex: Hints for tailoring non-invasive brain stimulation. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17, E5851.PubMedCrossRef Conson, M., Cecere, R., Baiano, C., et al. (2020a). Implicit motor imagery and the lateral occipitotemporal cortex: Hints for tailoring non-invasive brain stimulation. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17, E5851.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conson, M., Mazzarella, E., Donnarumma, C., & Trojano, L. (2012). Judging hand laterality from my or your point of view: Interactions between motor imagery and visual perspective. Neuroscience Letters, 530(1), 35–40.PubMedCrossRef Conson, M., Mazzarella, E., Donnarumma, C., & Trojano, L. (2012). Judging hand laterality from my or your point of view: Interactions between motor imagery and visual perspective. Neuroscience Letters, 530(1), 35–40.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conson, M., Mazzarella, E., & Trojano, L. (2011). Self-touch affects motor imagery: A study on posture interference effect. Experimental Brain Research, 215(2), 115–122.PubMedCrossRef Conson, M., Mazzarella, E., & Trojano, L. (2011). Self-touch affects motor imagery: A study on posture interference effect. Experimental Brain Research, 215(2), 115–122.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conson, M., Pistoia, F., Sarà, M., Grossi, D., & Trojano, L. (2010). Recognition and mental manipulation of body parts dissociate in locked-in syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), 189–193.PubMedCrossRef Conson, M., Pistoia, F., Sarà, M., Grossi, D., & Trojano, L. (2010). Recognition and mental manipulation of body parts dissociate in locked-in syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), 189–193.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conson, M., Sarà, M., Pistoia, F., & Trojano, L. (2009). Action observation improves motor imagery: Specific interactions between simulative processes. Experimental Brain Research, 199(1), 71–81.PubMedCrossRef Conson, M., Sarà, M., Pistoia, F., & Trojano, L. (2009). Action observation improves motor imagery: Specific interactions between simulative processes. Experimental Brain Research, 199(1), 71–81.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conson, M., Volpicella, F., De Bellis, F., Orefice, A., & Trojano, L. (2017). “Like the palm of my hands”: Motor imagery enhances implicit and explicit visual recognition of one’s own hands. Acta Psychologica, 180, 98–104.PubMedCrossRef Conson, M., Volpicella, F., De Bellis, F., Orefice, A., & Trojano, L. (2017). “Like the palm of my hands”: Motor imagery enhances implicit and explicit visual recognition of one’s own hands. Acta Psychologica, 180, 98–104.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1975). Mental transformation in the identification of left and right hands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(1), 48–56. Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1975). Mental transformation in the identification of left and right hands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(1), 48–56.
go back to reference Dalecki, M., Hoffmann, U., & Bock, O. (2012). Mental rotation of letters, body parts and complex scenes: Separate or common mechanisms? Human Movement Science, 31(5), 1151–1160.PubMedCrossRef Dalecki, M., Hoffmann, U., & Bock, O. (2012). Mental rotation of letters, body parts and complex scenes: Separate or common mechanisms? Human Movement Science, 31(5), 1151–1160.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dassonville, P., Zhu, X. H., Uurbil, K., Kim, S. G., & Ashe, J. (1997). Functional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the degree of handedness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94(25), 14015–14018.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Dassonville, P., Zhu, X. H., Uurbil, K., Kim, S. G., & Ashe, J. (1997). Functional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the degree of handedness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94(25), 14015–14018.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Date, S., Kurumadani, H., Watanabe, T., & Sunagawa, T. (2015). Transcranial direct current stimulation can enhance ability in motor imagery tasks. NeuroReport, 26(11), 613–617.PubMedCrossRef Date, S., Kurumadani, H., Watanabe, T., & Sunagawa, T. (2015). Transcranial direct current stimulation can enhance ability in motor imagery tasks. NeuroReport, 26(11), 613–617.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Bellis, F., Trojano, L., Errico, D., Grossi, D., & Conson, M. (2017). Whose hand is this? Differential responses of right and left extrastriate body areas to visual images of self and others’ hands. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(4), 826–837.CrossRef De Bellis, F., Trojano, L., Errico, D., Grossi, D., & Conson, M. (2017). Whose hand is this? Differential responses of right and left extrastriate body areas to visual images of self and others’ hands. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(4), 826–837.CrossRef
go back to reference De Lange, F. P., Hagoort, P., & Toni, I. (2005). Neural topography and content of movement representations. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 17(1), 97–112.PubMedCrossRef De Lange, F. P., Hagoort, P., & Toni, I. (2005). Neural topography and content of movement representations. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 17(1), 97–112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference de Lange, F. P., Helmich, R. C., & Toni, I. (2006). Posture influences motor imagery: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 33(2), 609–617.PubMedCrossRef de Lange, F. P., Helmich, R. C., & Toni, I. (2006). Posture influences motor imagery: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 33(2), 609–617.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Simone, L., Tomasino, B., Marusic, N., Eleopra, R., & Rumiati, R. I. (2013). The effects of healthy aging on mental imagery as revealed by egocentric and allocentric mental spatial transformations. Acta Psychologica, 143(1), 146–156.PubMedCrossRef De Simone, L., Tomasino, B., Marusic, N., Eleopra, R., & Rumiati, R. I. (2013). The effects of healthy aging on mental imagery as revealed by egocentric and allocentric mental spatial transformations. Acta Psychologica, 143(1), 146–156.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Edwards, L. M., Causby, R. S., Stewart, H., & Stanton, T. R. (2019). Differential influence of habitual third-person vision of a body part on mental rotation of images of hands and feet. Experimental Brain Research, 237(5), 1325–1337.PubMedCrossRef Edwards, L. M., Causby, R. S., Stewart, H., & Stanton, T. R. (2019). Differential influence of habitual third-person vision of a body part on mental rotation of images of hands and feet. Experimental Brain Research, 237(5), 1325–1337.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ferri, F., Frassinetti, F., Ardizzi, M., Costantini, M., & Gallese, V. (2012). A sensorimotor network for the bodily self. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 24(7), 1584–1595.PubMedCrossRef Ferri, F., Frassinetti, F., Ardizzi, M., Costantini, M., & Gallese, V. (2012). A sensorimotor network for the bodily self. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 24(7), 1584–1595.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiori, F., Sedda, A., Ferrè, E. R., Toraldo, A., Querzola, M., Pasotti, F., et al. (2013). Exploring motor and visual imagery in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Experimental Brain Research, 226(4), 537–547.PubMedCrossRef Fiori, F., Sedda, A., Ferrè, E. R., Toraldo, A., Querzola, M., Pasotti, F., et al. (2013). Exploring motor and visual imagery in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Experimental Brain Research, 226(4), 537–547.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiorio, M., Tinazzi, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Selective impairment of hand mental rotation in patients with focal hand dystonia. Brain, 129(1), 47–54.PubMedCrossRef Fiorio, M., Tinazzi, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Selective impairment of hand mental rotation in patients with focal hand dystonia. Brain, 129(1), 47–54.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiorio, M., Tinazzi, M., Ionta, S., Fiaschi, A., Moretto, G., Edwards, M. J., et al. (2007). Mental rotation of body parts and non-corporeal objects in patients with idiopathic cervical dystonia. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2346–2354.PubMedCrossRef Fiorio, M., Tinazzi, M., Ionta, S., Fiaschi, A., Moretto, G., Edwards, M. J., et al. (2007). Mental rotation of body parts and non-corporeal objects in patients with idiopathic cervical dystonia. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2346–2354.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fuelscher, I., Williams, J., & Hyde, C. (2015). Developmental improvements in reaching correction efficiency are associated with an increased ability to represent action mentally. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 74–91.PubMedCrossRef Fuelscher, I., Williams, J., & Hyde, C. (2015). Developmental improvements in reaching correction efficiency are associated with an increased ability to represent action mentally. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 74–91.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Funk, M., & Brugger, P. (2008). Mental rotation of congenitally absent hands. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(1), 81–89.PubMedCrossRef Funk, M., & Brugger, P. (2008). Mental rotation of congenitally absent hands. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(1), 81–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Funk, M., Brugger, P., & Wilkening, F. (2005). Motor processes in children’s imagery: The case of mental rotation of hands. Developmental Science, 8(5), 402–408.PubMedCrossRef Funk, M., Brugger, P., & Wilkening, F. (2005). Motor processes in children’s imagery: The case of mental rotation of hands. Developmental Science, 8(5), 402–408.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ganis, G., Keenan, J. P., Kosslyn, S. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of primary motor cortex affects mental rotation. Cerebral Cortex, 10(2), 175–180.PubMedCrossRef Ganis, G., Keenan, J. P., Kosslyn, S. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of primary motor cortex affects mental rotation. Cerebral Cortex, 10(2), 175–180.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gentilucci, M., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (1998a). Implicit visual analysis in handedness recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 7(3), 478–493.PubMedCrossRef Gentilucci, M., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (1998a). Implicit visual analysis in handedness recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 7(3), 478–493.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gentilucci, M., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (1998b). Right-handers and left-handers have different representations of their own hand. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(3), 185–192.PubMedCrossRef Gentilucci, M., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (1998b). Right-handers and left-handers have different representations of their own hand. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(3), 185–192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of neurology, 42(5), 428–459.PubMedCrossRef Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of neurology, 42(5), 428–459.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grabowska, A., Gut, M., Binder, M., Forsberg, L., Rymarczyk, K., & Urbanik, A. (2012). Switching handedness: fMRI study of hand motor control in right-handers, left-handers and converted left-handers. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 72(4), 439–451.PubMed Grabowska, A., Gut, M., Binder, M., Forsberg, L., Rymarczyk, K., & Urbanik, A. (2012). Switching handedness: fMRI study of hand motor control in right-handers, left-handers and converted left-handers. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 72(4), 439–451.PubMed
go back to reference Grigoriadis, I., & Punt, D. (2016). Can the hand laterality recognition task be performed without using motor imagery? Manual Therapy, 100(25), e144–e145.CrossRef Grigoriadis, I., & Punt, D. (2016). Can the hand laterality recognition task be performed without using motor imagery? Manual Therapy, 100(25), e144–e145.CrossRef
go back to reference Guo, X., Lyu, Y., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Flor, H., & Tong, S. (2015 Aug). Handedness change after dominant side amputation: Evaluation from a hand laterality judgment task. In Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2015, 8002–8005. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2015.7320249.CrossRef Guo, X., Lyu, Y., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Flor, H., & Tong, S. (2015 Aug). Handedness change after dominant side amputation: Evaluation from a hand laterality judgment task. In Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2015, 8002–8005. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1109/​EMBC.​2015.​7320249.CrossRef
go back to reference Habacha, H., Molinaro, C., Tabben, M., & Lejeune-Poutrain, L. (2014). Implementation of specific motor expertise during a mental rotation task of hands. Experimental Brain Research, 232(11), 3465–3473.PubMedCrossRef Habacha, H., Molinaro, C., Tabben, M., & Lejeune-Poutrain, L. (2014). Implementation of specific motor expertise during a mental rotation task of hands. Experimental Brain Research, 232(11), 3465–3473.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hamada, H., Matsuzawa, D., Sutoh, C., Hirano, Y., Chakraborty, S., Ito, H., et al. (2018). Comparison of brain activity between motor imagery and mental rotation of the hand tasks: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 12(6), 1596–1606.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hamada, H., Matsuzawa, D., Sutoh, C., Hirano, Y., Chakraborty, S., Ito, H., et al. (2018). Comparison of brain activity between motor imagery and mental rotation of the hand tasks: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 12(6), 1596–1606.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Heremans, E., D’hooge, A.-M., De Bondt, S., Helsen, W., & Feys, P. (2012). The relation between cognitive and motor dysfunction and motor imagery ability in patients with multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis Journal, 18(9), 1303–1309.PubMedCrossRef Heremans, E., D’hooge, A.-M., De Bondt, S., Helsen, W., & Feys, P. (2012). The relation between cognitive and motor dysfunction and motor imagery ability in patients with multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis Journal, 18(9), 1303–1309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Herron, J. (2012). Neuropsychology of left-handedness. New York: Academic Press. Herron, J. (2012). Neuropsychology of left-handedness. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Higgins, J. P., & Green, S. (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, vol 4. New York: Wiley. Higgins, J. P., & Green, S. (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, vol 4. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Hirnstein, M., Ocklenburg, S., Schneider, D., & Hausmann, M. (2009). Sex differences in left–right confusion depend on hemispheric asymmetry. Cortex, 45(7), 891–899.PubMedCrossRef Hirnstein, M., Ocklenburg, S., Schneider, D., & Hausmann, M. (2009). Sex differences in left–right confusion depend on hemispheric asymmetry. Cortex, 45(7), 891–899.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hudson, M. L., McCormick, K., Zalucki, N., & Moseley, G. L. (2006). Expectation of pain replicates the effect of pain in a hand laterality recognition task: Bias in information processing toward the painful side? European Journal of Pain, 10(3), 219–219.PubMedCrossRef Hudson, M. L., McCormick, K., Zalucki, N., & Moseley, G. L. (2006). Expectation of pain replicates the effect of pain in a hand laterality recognition task: Bias in information processing toward the painful side? European Journal of Pain, 10(3), 219–219.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ionta, S., & Blanke, O. (2009). Differential influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet in left and right handers. Experimental Brain Research, 195(2), 207–217.PubMedCrossRef Ionta, S., & Blanke, O. (2009). Differential influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet in left and right handers. Experimental Brain Research, 195(2), 207–217.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ionta, S., Fourkas, A. D., Fiorio, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007). The influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet. Experimental Brain Research, 183(1), 1–7.PubMedCrossRef Ionta, S., Fourkas, A. D., Fiorio, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007). The influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet. Experimental Brain Research, 183(1), 1–7.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ionta, S., Villiger, M., Jutzeler, C. R., Freund, P., Curt, A., & Gassert, R. (2016). Spinal cord injury affects the interplay between visual and sensorimotor representations of the body. Scientific Reports, 6, 20144.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ionta, S., Villiger, M., Jutzeler, C. R., Freund, P., Curt, A., & Gassert, R. (2016). Spinal cord injury affects the interplay between visual and sensorimotor representations of the body. Scientific Reports, 6, 20144.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, B. W., McKenzie, K. J., & Hamm, J. P. (2002). Cerebral asymmetry for mental rotation: Effects of response hand, handedness and gender. NeuroReport, 13(15), 1929–1932.PubMedCrossRef Johnson, B. W., McKenzie, K. J., & Hamm, J. P. (2002). Cerebral asymmetry for mental rotation: Effects of response hand, handedness and gender. NeuroReport, 13(15), 1929–1932.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jones, B., & Anuza, T. (1982). Effects of sex, handedness, stimulus and visual field on “mental rotation.” Cortex, 18(4), 501–514.PubMedCrossRef Jones, B., & Anuza, T. (1982). Effects of sex, handedness, stimulus and visual field on “mental rotation.” Cortex, 18(4), 501–514.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Katschnig, P., Edwards, M. J., Schwingenschuh, P., Aguirregomozcorta, M., Kägi, G., Rothwell, J. C., et al. (2010). Mental rotation of body parts and sensory temporal discrimination in fixed dystonia. Movement Disorders, 25(8), 1061–1067.PubMedCrossRef Katschnig, P., Edwards, M. J., Schwingenschuh, P., Aguirregomozcorta, M., Kägi, G., Rothwell, J. C., et al. (2010). Mental rotation of body parts and sensory temporal discrimination in fixed dystonia. Movement Disorders, 25(8), 1061–1067.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kemlin, C., Moulton, E., Samson, Y., & Rosso, C. (2016). Do motor imagery performances depend on the side of the lesion at the acute stage of stroke? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 321–331.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kemlin, C., Moulton, E., Samson, Y., & Rosso, C. (2016). Do motor imagery performances depend on the side of the lesion at the acute stage of stroke? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 321–331.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kemlin-Méchin, C., Moulton, E., & Rosso, C. (2015). Is motor imagery really a window for studying stroke recovery at subacute stage? Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 58, e3.CrossRef Kemlin-Méchin, C., Moulton, E., & Rosso, C. (2015). Is motor imagery really a window for studying stroke recovery at subacute stage? Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 58, e3.CrossRef
go back to reference Kosslyn, S. M., DiGirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L., & Alpert, N. M. (1998). Mental rotation of objects versus hands: Neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology, 35(2), 151–161.PubMedCrossRef Kosslyn, S. M., DiGirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L., & Alpert, N. M. (1998). Mental rotation of objects versus hands: Neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology, 35(2), 151–161.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lebon, F., Lotze, M., Stinear, C. M., & Byblow, W. D. (2012). Task-dependent interaction between parietal and contralateral primary motor cortex during explicit versus implicit motor imagery. PLoS ONE, 7(5), e37850.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lebon, F., Lotze, M., Stinear, C. M., & Byblow, W. D. (2012). Task-dependent interaction between parietal and contralateral primary motor cortex during explicit versus implicit motor imagery. PLoS ONE, 7(5), e37850.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Lyu, Y., Guo, X., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Flor, H., & Tong, S. (2016). Phantom limb perception interferes with motor imagery after unilateral upper-limb amputation. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 1–10.CrossRef Lyu, Y., Guo, X., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Flor, H., & Tong, S. (2016). Phantom limb perception interferes with motor imagery after unilateral upper-limb amputation. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 1–10.CrossRef
go back to reference Lyu, Y., Guo, X., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Flor, H., & Tong, S. (2017). An event-related potential study on the time course of mental rotation in upper-limb amputees. Clinical Neurophysiology, 128(5), 744–750.PubMedCrossRef Lyu, Y., Guo, X., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Flor, H., & Tong, S. (2017). An event-related potential study on the time course of mental rotation in upper-limb amputees. Clinical Neurophysiology, 128(5), 744–750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Magni, N. E., McNair, P. J., & Rice, D. A. (2018). Sensorimotor performance and function in people with osteoarthritis of the hand: A case–control comparison. Seminars in Arthritis and Rheumatism, 47(5), 676–682.PubMedCrossRef Magni, N. E., McNair, P. J., & Rice, D. A. (2018). Sensorimotor performance and function in people with osteoarthritis of the hand: A case–control comparison. Seminars in Arthritis and Rheumatism, 47(5), 676–682.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Maimon-Mor, R. O., Schone, H. R., Moran, R., Brugger, P., & Makin, T. R. (2020). Motor control drives visual bodily judgements. Cognition, 196, 104120.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Maimon-Mor, R. O., Schone, H. R., Moran, R., Brugger, P., & Makin, T. R. (2020). Motor control drives visual bodily judgements. Cognition, 196, 104120.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Mazhari, S., Parvaresh, N., Pourrahimi, A. M., Rajabizadeh, G., & Abasabadi, K. (2017). Mental imagery in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 11(2), e7425. Mazhari, S., Parvaresh, N., Pourrahimi, A. M., Rajabizadeh, G., & Abasabadi, K. (2017). Mental imagery in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 11(2), e7425.
go back to reference Mazhari, S., Tabrizi, Y. M., & Nejad, A. G. (2015). Neural evidence for compromised mental imagery in individuals with chronic schizophrenia. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 27(2), 127–132.PubMedCrossRef Mazhari, S., Tabrizi, Y. M., & Nejad, A. G. (2015). Neural evidence for compromised mental imagery in individuals with chronic schizophrenia. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 27(2), 127–132.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meng, S., Oi, M., Saito, G., & Saito, H. (2017). The neural correlates of biomechanical constraints in hand laterality judgment task performed from other person’s perspective: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS ONE, 12(9), e0183818.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Meng, S., Oi, M., Saito, G., & Saito, H. (2017). The neural correlates of biomechanical constraints in hand laterality judgment task performed from other person’s perspective: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS ONE, 12(9), e0183818.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Meng, S., Oi, M., Sekiyama, K., & Saito, H. (2016). The neural mechanism of biomechanical constraints in the hand laterality judgment task: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroscience Letters, 627, 211–215.PubMedCrossRef Meng, S., Oi, M., Sekiyama, K., & Saito, H. (2016). The neural mechanism of biomechanical constraints in the hand laterality judgment task: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroscience Letters, 627, 211–215.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moseley, G. L. (2004). Why do people with complex regional pain syndrome take longer to recognize their affected hand? Neurology, 62(12), 2182–2186.PubMedCrossRef Moseley, G. L. (2004). Why do people with complex regional pain syndrome take longer to recognize their affected hand? Neurology, 62(12), 2182–2186.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moseley, G. L. (2006). Graded motor imagery for pathologic pain: A randomized controlled trial. Neurology, 67(12), 2129–2134.PubMedCrossRef Moseley, G. L. (2006). Graded motor imagery for pathologic pain: A randomized controlled trial. Neurology, 67(12), 2129–2134.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nico, D., Daprati, E., Rigal, F., Parsons, L., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Left and right hand recognition in upper limb amputees. Brain, 127(1), 120–132.PubMedCrossRef Nico, D., Daprati, E., Rigal, F., Parsons, L., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Left and right hand recognition in upper limb amputees. Brain, 127(1), 120–132.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nicolini, C., Harasym, D., Turco, C. V., & Nelson, A. J. (2019). Human motor cortical organization is influenced by handedness. Cortex, 115, 172–183.PubMedCrossRef Nicolini, C., Harasym, D., Turco, C. V., & Nelson, A. J. (2019). Human motor cortical organization is influenced by handedness. Cortex, 115, 172–183.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Osuagwu, B. A., & Vuckovic, A. (2014). Similarities between explicit and implicit motor imagery in mental rotation of hands: An EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 65, 197–210.PubMedCrossRef Osuagwu, B. A., & Vuckovic, A. (2014). Similarities between explicit and implicit motor imagery in mental rotation of hands: An EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 65, 197–210.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Osuagwu, B. A., Zych, M., & Vuckovic, A. (2017). Is implicit motor imagery a reliable strategy for a brain–computer interface? IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, 25(12), 2239–2248.PubMedCrossRef Osuagwu, B. A., Zych, M., & Vuckovic, A. (2017). Is implicit motor imagery a reliable strategy for a brain–computer interface? IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, 25(12), 2239–2248.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19(2), 178–241.PubMedCrossRef Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19(2), 178–241.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Parsons, L. M. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 709–730.PubMed Parsons, L. M. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 709–730.PubMed
go back to reference Parsons, L. M. (2001). Integrating cognitive psychology, neurology and neuroimaging. Acta Psychologica, 107(1–3), 155–181.PubMedCrossRef Parsons, L. M. (2001). Integrating cognitive psychology, neurology and neuroimaging. Acta Psychologica, 107(1–3), 155–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Parsons, L. M., & Fox, P. T. (1998). The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(1), 583–616. Parsons, L. M., & Fox, P. T. (1998). The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(1), 583–616.
go back to reference Parsons, L. M., Fox, P. T., Downs, J. H., Glass, T., Hirsch, T. B., Martin, C. C., et al. (1995). Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature, 375(6526), 54–58.PubMedCrossRef Parsons, L. M., Fox, P. T., Downs, J. H., Glass, T., Hirsch, T. B., Martin, C. C., et al. (1995). Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature, 375(6526), 54–58.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pelgrims, B., Andres, M., & Olivier, E. (2009). Double dissociation between motor and visual imagery in the posterior parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19(10), 2298–2307.PubMedCrossRef Pelgrims, B., Andres, M., & Olivier, E. (2009). Double dissociation between motor and visual imagery in the posterior parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19(10), 2298–2307.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pelletier, R., Higgins, J., & Bourbonnais, D. (2018). Laterality recognition of images, motor performance, and aspects related to pain in participants with and without wrist/hand disorders: An observational cross-sectional study. Musculoskeletal Science and Practice, 35, 18–24.PubMedCrossRef Pelletier, R., Higgins, J., & Bourbonnais, D. (2018). Laterality recognition of images, motor performance, and aspects related to pain in participants with and without wrist/hand disorders: An observational cross-sectional study. Musculoskeletal Science and Practice, 35, 18–24.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perruchoud, D., Fiorio, M., Cesari, P., & Ionta, S. (2018). Beyond variability: Subjective timing and the neurophysiology of motor cognition. Brain Stimulation, 11(1), 175–180.PubMedCrossRef Perruchoud, D., Fiorio, M., Cesari, P., & Ionta, S. (2018). Beyond variability: Subjective timing and the neurophysiology of motor cognition. Brain Stimulation, 11(1), 175–180.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perruchoud, D., Murray, M. M., Lefebvre, J., & Ionta, S. (2014). Focal dystonia and the sensory-motor integrative loop for enacting (SMILE). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 458.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Perruchoud, D., Murray, M. M., Lefebvre, J., & Ionta, S. (2014). Focal dystonia and the sensory-motor integrative loop for enacting (SMILE). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 458.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Pietsch, S., & Jansen, P. (2019). The relation between mental rotation and handedness is a consequence of how handedness is measured. Brain and Cognition, 130, 28–36.PubMedCrossRef Pietsch, S., & Jansen, P. (2019). The relation between mental rotation and handedness is a consequence of how handedness is measured. Brain and Cognition, 130, 28–36.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pool, E. M., Rehme, A. K., Fink, G. R., Eickhoff, S. B., & Grefkes, C. (2014). Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor system. Neuroimage, 99, 451–460.PubMedCrossRef Pool, E. M., Rehme, A. K., Fink, G. R., Eickhoff, S. B., & Grefkes, C. (2014). Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor system. Neuroimage, 99, 451–460.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Porac, C., & Coren, S. (1981). Lateral preferences and human behavior. Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Porac, C., & Coren, S. (1981). Lateral preferences and human behavior. Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Reinersmann, A., Haarmeyer, G. S., Blankenburg, M., Frettlöh, J., Krumova, E. K., Ocklenburg, S., et al. (2010). Left is where the L is right. Significantly delayed reaction time in limb laterality recognition in both CRPS and phantom limb pain patients. Neuroscience Letters, 486(3), 240–245.PubMedCrossRef Reinersmann, A., Haarmeyer, G. S., Blankenburg, M., Frettlöh, J., Krumova, E. K., Ocklenburg, S., et al. (2010). Left is where the L is right. Significantly delayed reaction time in limb laterality recognition in both CRPS and phantom limb pain patients. Neuroscience Letters, 486(3), 240–245.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Review Manager (RevMan) [Computer program] (2014). Version 5.3. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration. Review Manager (RevMan) [Computer program] (2014). Version 5.3. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration.
go back to reference Scandola, M., Dodoni, L., Lazzeri, G., Arcangeli, C. A., Avesani, R., Moro, V., et al. (2019). Neurocognitive benefits of physiotherapy for spinal cord injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 36, 2028–2035.PubMedCrossRef Scandola, M., Dodoni, L., Lazzeri, G., Arcangeli, C. A., Avesani, R., Moro, V., et al. (2019). Neurocognitive benefits of physiotherapy for spinal cord injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 36, 2028–2035.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schmid, A. B., & Coppieters, M. W. (2012). Left/right judgment of body parts is selectively impaired in patients with unilateral carpal tunnel syndrome. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 28(7), 615–622.PubMedCrossRef Schmid, A. B., & Coppieters, M. W. (2012). Left/right judgment of body parts is selectively impaired in patients with unilateral carpal tunnel syndrome. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 28(7), 615–622.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schwoebel, J., Friedman, R., Duda, N., & Coslett, H. B. (2001). Pain and the body schema: Evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of movement. Brain, 124(10), 2098–2104.PubMedCrossRef Schwoebel, J., Friedman, R., Duda, N., & Coslett, H. B. (2001). Pain and the body schema: Evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of movement. Brain, 124(10), 2098–2104.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sekiyama, K. (1982). Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands. Perception & Psychophysics, 32(2), 89–95.CrossRef Sekiyama, K. (1982). Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands. Perception & Psychophysics, 32(2), 89–95.CrossRef
go back to reference Sekiyama, K., Kinoshita, T., & Soshi, T. (2014). Strong biomechanical constraints on young children’s mental imagery of hands. Royal Society Open Science, 1(4), 140118.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sekiyama, K., Kinoshita, T., & Soshi, T. (2014). Strong biomechanical constraints on young children’s mental imagery of hands. Royal Society Open Science, 1(4), 140118.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.PubMedCrossRef Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shimoda, N., Takeda, K., Imai, I., Kaneko, J., & Kato, H. (2008). Cerebral laterality differences in handedness: A mental rotation study with NIRS. Neuroscience Letters, 430(1), 43–47.PubMedCrossRef Shimoda, N., Takeda, K., Imai, I., Kaneko, J., & Kato, H. (2008). Cerebral laterality differences in handedness: A mental rotation study with NIRS. Neuroscience Letters, 430(1), 43–47.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Somers, M., Shields, L. S., Boks, M. P., Kahn, R. S., & Sommer, I. E. (2015). Cognitive benefits of right-handedness: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51(1), 48–63.CrossRef Somers, M., Shields, L. S., Boks, M. P., Kahn, R. S., & Sommer, I. E. (2015). Cognitive benefits of right-handedness: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51(1), 48–63.CrossRef
go back to reference Stanton, T., Leake, H., Wallwork, S., & Moseley, G. (2015). Disruptions to cortical proprioceptive representation in people with chronic neck pain. Physiotherapy, 101, e1433–e1434.CrossRef Stanton, T., Leake, H., Wallwork, S., & Moseley, G. (2015). Disruptions to cortical proprioceptive representation in people with chronic neck pain. Physiotherapy, 101, e1433–e1434.CrossRef
go back to reference Stanton, T. R., Lin, C.-W.C., Smeets, R. J., Taylor, D., Law, R., & Lorimer Moseley, G. (2012). Spatially defined disruption of motor imagery performance in people with osteoarthritis. Rheumatology, 51(8), 1455–1464.PubMedCrossRef Stanton, T. R., Lin, C.-W.C., Smeets, R. J., Taylor, D., Law, R., & Lorimer Moseley, G. (2012). Spatially defined disruption of motor imagery performance in people with osteoarthritis. Rheumatology, 51(8), 1455–1464.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tabrizi, Y. M., Mazhari, S., Nazari, M. A., Zangiabadi, N., & Sheibani, V. (2014). Abnormalities of motor imagery and relationship with depressive symptoms in mildly disabling relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 38(2), 111–118.PubMedCrossRef Tabrizi, Y. M., Mazhari, S., Nazari, M. A., Zangiabadi, N., & Sheibani, V. (2014). Abnormalities of motor imagery and relationship with depressive symptoms in mildly disabling relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 38(2), 111–118.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tabrizi, Y. M., Mazhari, S., Nazari, M. A., Zangiabadi, N., Sheibani, V., & Azarang, S. (2013). Compromised motor imagery ability in individuals with multiple sclerosis and mild physical disability: An ERP study. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 115(9), 1738–1744.PubMedCrossRef Tabrizi, Y. M., Mazhari, S., Nazari, M. A., Zangiabadi, N., Sheibani, V., & Azarang, S. (2013). Compromised motor imagery ability in individuals with multiple sclerosis and mild physical disability: An ERP study. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 115(9), 1738–1744.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Takeda, K., Shimoda, N., Sato, Y., Ogano, M., & Kato, H. (2010). Reaction time differences between left-and right-handers during mental rotation of hand pictures. Laterality, 15(4), 415–425.PubMedCrossRef Takeda, K., Shimoda, N., Sato, Y., Ogano, M., & Kato, H. (2010). Reaction time differences between left-and right-handers during mental rotation of hand pictures. Laterality, 15(4), 415–425.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tao, W., Liu, Q., Huang, X., Tao, X., Yan, J., Teeter, C. J., et al. (2009). Effect of degree and direction of rotation in egocentric mental rotation of hand: An event-related potential study. NeuroReport, 20(2), 180–185.PubMedCrossRef Tao, W., Liu, Q., Huang, X., Tao, X., Yan, J., Teeter, C. J., et al. (2009). Effect of degree and direction of rotation in egocentric mental rotation of hand: An event-related potential study. NeuroReport, 20(2), 180–185.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ter Horst, A. C., Van Lier, R., & Steenbergen, B. (2010). Mental rotation task of hands: Differential influence number of rotational axes. Experimental Brain Research, 203(2), 347–354.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ter Horst, A. C., Van Lier, R., & Steenbergen, B. (2010). Mental rotation task of hands: Differential influence number of rotational axes. Experimental Brain Research, 203(2), 347–354.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Thayer, Z. C., & Johnson, B. W. (2006). Cerebral processes during visuo-motor imagery of hands. Psychophysiology, 43(4), 401–412.PubMedCrossRef Thayer, Z. C., & Johnson, B. W. (2006). Cerebral processes during visuo-motor imagery of hands. Psychophysiology, 43(4), 401–412.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thomas, M., Dalecki, M., & Abeln, V. (2013). EEG coherence during mental rotation of letters, hands and scenes. International journal of psychophysiology, 89(1), 128–135.PubMedCrossRef Thomas, M., Dalecki, M., & Abeln, V. (2013). EEG coherence during mental rotation of letters, hands and scenes. International journal of psychophysiology, 89(1), 128–135.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vannuscorps, G., Pillon, A., & Andres, M. (2012). Effect of biomechanical constraints in the hand laterality judgment task: Where does it come from? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 299–308.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Vannuscorps, G., Pillon, A., & Andres, M. (2012). Effect of biomechanical constraints in the hand laterality judgment task: Where does it come from? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 299–308.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Vogel, J. J., Bowers, C. A., & Vogel, D. S. (2003). Cerebral lateralization of spatial abilities: A meta-analysis. Brain and Cognition, 52(2), 197–204.PubMedCrossRef Vogel, J. J., Bowers, C. A., & Vogel, D. S. (2003). Cerebral lateralization of spatial abilities: A meta-analysis. Brain and Cognition, 52(2), 197–204.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Volkmann, J., Schnitzler, A., Witte, O., & Freund, H.-J. (1998). Handedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(4), 2149–2154.PubMedCrossRef Volkmann, J., Schnitzler, A., Witte, O., & Freund, H.-J. (1998). Handedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(4), 2149–2154.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Voyer, D., Jansen, P., & Kaltner, S. (2017). Mental rotation with egocentric and object-based transformations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(11), 2319–2330.PubMedCrossRef Voyer, D., Jansen, P., & Kaltner, S. (2017). Mental rotation with egocentric and object-based transformations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(11), 2319–2330.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wallwork, S. B., Butler, D. S., Fulton, I., Stewart, H., Darmawan, I., & Moseley, G. L. (2013). Left/right neck rotation judgments are affected by age, gender, handedness and image rotation. Manual Therapy, 18, 225–230.PubMedCrossRef Wallwork, S. B., Butler, D. S., Fulton, I., Stewart, H., Darmawan, I., & Moseley, G. L. (2013). Left/right neck rotation judgments are affected by age, gender, handedness and image rotation. Manual Therapy, 18, 225–230.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wallwork, S. B., Butler, D. S., Wilson, D. J., & Moseley, G. L. (2015). Are people who do yoga any better at a motor imagery task than those who do not? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 49(2), 123–127.PubMedCrossRef Wallwork, S. B., Butler, D. S., Wilson, D. J., & Moseley, G. L. (2015). Are people who do yoga any better at a motor imagery task than those who do not? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 49(2), 123–127.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference White, K., & Ashton, R. (1976). Handedness assessment inventory. Neuropsychologia, 14(2), 261–264.PubMedCrossRef White, K., & Ashton, R. (1976). Handedness assessment inventory. Neuropsychologia, 14(2), 261–264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Hand preference influences neural correlates of action observation. Brain Research, 1269, 90–104.PubMedCrossRef Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Hand preference influences neural correlates of action observation. Brain Research, 1269, 90–104.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Willems, R. M., Peelen, M. V., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Cerebral lateralization of face-selective and body-selective visual areas depends on handedness. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1719–1725.PubMedCrossRef Willems, R. M., Peelen, M. V., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Cerebral lateralization of face-selective and body-selective visual areas depends on handedness. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1719–1725.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Willems, R. M., Toni, I., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. (2009). Body-specific motor imagery of hand actions: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3(1), 39.PubMedPubMedCentral Willems, R. M., Toni, I., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. (2009). Body-specific motor imagery of hand actions: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3(1), 39.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Zapparoli, L., Invernizzi, P., Gandola, M., Berlingeri, M., De Santis, A., Zerbi, A., et al. (2014). Like the back of the (right) hand? A new fMRI look on the hand laterality task. Experimental Brain Research, 232(12), 3873–3895.PubMedCrossRef Zapparoli, L., Invernizzi, P., Gandola, M., Berlingeri, M., De Santis, A., Zerbi, A., et al. (2014). Like the back of the (right) hand? A new fMRI look on the hand laterality task. Experimental Brain Research, 232(12), 3873–3895.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zapparoli, L., Saetta, G., De Santis, C., Gandola, M., Zerbi, A., Banfi, G., et al. (2016). When I am (almost) 64: The effect of normal ageing on implicit motor imagery in young elderlies. Behavioural Brain Research, 303, 137–151.PubMedCrossRef Zapparoli, L., Saetta, G., De Santis, C., Gandola, M., Zerbi, A., Banfi, G., et al. (2016). When I am (almost) 64: The effect of normal ageing on implicit motor imagery in young elderlies. Behavioural Brain Research, 303, 137–151.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The effect of handedness on mental rotation of hands: a systematic review and meta-analysis
Auteurs
H. G. Jones
F. A. Braithwaite
L. M. Edwards
R. S. Causby
M. Conson
T. R. Stanton
Publicatiedatum
03-01-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01444-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2021

Psychological Research 8/2021 Naar de uitgave