Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2016

17-02-2015 | Original Article

Set-shifting as a component process of goal-directed problem-solving

Auteurs: Richard P. Cooper, Verity Marsh

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In two experiments, we compared secondary task interference on Tower of London performance resulting from three different secondary tasks. The secondary tasks were designed to tap three different executive functions, namely set-shifting, memory monitoring and updating, and response inhibition. Previous work using individual differences methodology suggests that, all other things being equal, the response inhibition or memory tasks should result in the greatest interference. However, this was not found to be the case. Rather, in both experiments the set-shifting task resulted in significantly more interference on Tower of London performance than either of the other secondary tasks. Subsequent analyses suggest that the degree of interference could not be attributed to differences in secondary task difficulty. Results are interpreted in the light of related work which suggests that solving problems with non-transparent goal/subgoal structure requires flexible shifting between subgoals—a process that is held to be impaired by concurrent performance of a set-shifting task.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Because of substantial skewness in some of the dependent measures, excess moves was square root transformed prior to analysis, while first move time and time per subsequent move were log transformed. The sphericity assumption was violated for the ANOVA for time per subsequent move, and so Greenhouse–Geisser corrections to degrees of freedom have been made for the analysis of variance of this measure.
 
2
Recall that in all cases the baseline condition was performed before the dual-task conditions, but that the order of dual-task conditions was counterbalanced over participants. Performance of the primary task on the dual-task conditions therefore potentially benefits from prior practice on the single-task condition but suffers from the requirement to concurrently perform a secondary task. It is consequently not possible to interpret pairwise comparisons between the baseline condition and each dual-task condition.
 
3
The difference between the dependent variable values in the baseline condition and the experimental conditions is presumably due to a combination of the baseline condition being easier because it does not involve dual-tasking, but harder because it is unfamiliar. Recall that the baseline condition was completed first by all participants but order of the three dual-task conditions was counterbalanced, so the dependent variables across the dual-task conditions are directly comparable.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Albert, D., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Age differences in strategic planning as indexed by the Tower of London. Child Development, 82, 1501–1517.CrossRefPubMed Albert, D., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Age differences in strategic planning as indexed by the Tower of London. Child Development, 82, 1501–1517.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Allport, A. D., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: The MIT Press. Allport, A. D., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Asato, M. R., Sweeney, J. A., & Luna, B. (2006). Cognitive processes in the development of TOL performance. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2259–2269.CrossRefPubMed Asato, M. R., Sweeney, J. A., & Luna, B. (2006). Cognitive processes in the development of TOL performance. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2259–2269.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 5–28.CrossRef Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 5–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Baughman, F. D., & Cooper, R. P. (2007). Inhibition and young children’s performance on the Tower of London task. Cognitive Systems Research, 8, 216–226.CrossRef Baughman, F. D., & Cooper, R. P. (2007). Inhibition and young children’s performance on the Tower of London task. Cognitive Systems Research, 8, 216–226.CrossRef
go back to reference Berg, W. K., & Byrd, D. L. (2002). The Tower of London spatial problem-solving task: enhancing clinical and research implementation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(5), 586–604.CrossRef Berg, W. K., & Byrd, D. L. (2002). The Tower of London spatial problem-solving task: enhancing clinical and research implementation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(5), 586–604.CrossRef
go back to reference Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). A comparison of performance on the Towers of London and Hanoi in young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 743–754.CrossRefPubMed Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). A comparison of performance on the Towers of London and Hanoi in young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 743–754.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cheetham, J. M., Rahm, B., Kaller, C. P., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2012). Visuospatial over verbal demands in predicting Tower of London planning tasks. British Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 98–116.CrossRefPubMed Cheetham, J. M., Rahm, B., Kaller, C. P., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2012). Visuospatial over verbal demands in predicting Tower of London planning tasks. British Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 98–116.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Christoff, K., Pranhakaran, V., Dorfman, J., Shao, Z., Kroger, J. K., Holyoak, K. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relation integration during reasoning. Neuroimage, 14, 1136–1149.CrossRefPubMed Christoff, K., Pranhakaran, V., Dorfman, J., Shao, Z., Kroger, J. K., Holyoak, K. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relation integration during reasoning. Neuroimage, 14, 1136–1149.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cockburn, J. (1995). Performance on the Tower of London test after severe head injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1(06), 537–544.CrossRefPubMed Cockburn, J. (1995). Performance on the Tower of London test after severe head injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1(06), 537–544.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cooper, R. P., Wutke, K., & Davelaar, E. (2012). Differential contributions of set-shifting and monitoring to dual-task interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(3), 587–612.CrossRef Cooper, R. P., Wutke, K., & Davelaar, E. (2012). Differential contributions of set-shifting and monitoring to dual-task interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(3), 587–612.CrossRef
go back to reference Coxon, J. P., Stinear, C. M., & Byblow, W. D. (2009). Stop and go: the neural basis of selective movement prevention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(6), 1193–1203.CrossRefPubMed Coxon, J. P., Stinear, C. M., & Byblow, W. D. (2009). Stop and go: the neural basis of selective movement prevention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(6), 1193–1203.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. (1997). A hierarchical neural network for planning behaviour. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 94, 13293–13298.CrossRef Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. (1997). A hierarchical neural network for planning behaviour. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 94, 13293–13298.CrossRef
go back to reference Donders, J., & Larsen, T. (2012). Clinical utility of the Tower of London—Drexel University, second edition (TOLDX) after adolescent traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37, 333–342.CrossRefPubMed Donders, J., & Larsen, T. (2012). Clinical utility of the Tower of London—Drexel University, second edition (TOLDX) after adolescent traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37, 333–342.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gilhooly, K. J., Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V., Logie, R. H., & Della Sala, S. (1999). Planning processes and age in the five-disc Tower of London task. Thinking and Reasoning, 5(4), 339–361.CrossRef Gilhooly, K. J., Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V., Logie, R. H., & Della Sala, S. (1999). Planning processes and age in the five-disc Tower of London task. Thinking and Reasoning, 5(4), 339–361.CrossRef
go back to reference Glosser, G., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disorders in executive control functions among aphasic and other brain-damaged patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12(4), 485–501.CrossRefPubMed Glosser, G., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disorders in executive control functions among aphasic and other brain-damaged patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12(4), 485–501.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Green, A. E., Fugelsand, J. A., Kraemer, D. J. M., Shamosh, N. A., & Dunbar, K. N. (2006). Frontopolar cortex mediates abstract integration in analogy. Brain Research, 1096, 125–137.CrossRefPubMed Green, A. E., Fugelsand, J. A., Kraemer, D. J. M., Shamosh, N. A., & Dunbar, K. N. (2006). Frontopolar cortex mediates abstract integration in analogy. Brain Research, 1096, 125–137.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hartman, E., Houwen, S., Scherder, E., & Visscher, C. (2010). On the relationship between motor performance and executive functioning in children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 468–477.CrossRefPubMed Hartman, E., Houwen, S., Scherder, E., & Visscher, C. (2010). On the relationship between motor performance and executive functioning in children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 468–477.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heaton, R. K. (1981). A manual for the wisconsin card sorting test. Odessa FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Heaton, R. K. (1981). A manual for the wisconsin card sorting test. Odessa FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
go back to reference Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, Whole No. 89. Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, Whole No. 89.
go back to reference Kaller, C. P., Rahm, B., Köstering, L., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2011a). Reviewing the impact of problem structure on planning: a software tool for analyzing tower tasks. Behavioural Brain Research, 216(1), 1–8.CrossRefPubMed Kaller, C. P., Rahm, B., Köstering, L., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2011a). Reviewing the impact of problem structure on planning: a software tool for analyzing tower tasks. Behavioural Brain Research, 216(1), 1–8.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kaller, C. P., Rahm, B., Spreer, J., Mader, I., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2008). Thinking around the corner: the development of planning abilities. Brain and Cognition, 67(3), 360–370.CrossRefPubMed Kaller, C. P., Rahm, B., Spreer, J., Mader, I., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2008). Thinking around the corner: the development of planning abilities. Brain and Cognition, 67(3), 360–370.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kaller, C. P., Rahm, B., Spreer, J., Weiller, C., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2011b). Dissociable contributions of left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in planning. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 307–317.CrossRefPubMed Kaller, C. P., Rahm, B., Spreer, J., Weiller, C., & Unterrainer, J. M. (2011b). Dissociable contributions of left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in planning. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 307–317.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klahr, D., & Robinson, M. (1981). Formal assessment of problem-solving and planning processes in preschool children. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 113–148.CrossRef Klahr, D., & Robinson, M. (1981). Formal assessment of problem-solving and planning processes in preschool children. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 113–148.CrossRef
go back to reference Köstering, L., McKinlay, A., Stahl, C., & Kaller, C. P. (2012). Differential patterns of planning impairments in Parkinson’s disease and sub-clinical signs of dementia? A latent-class model-based approach. PloS One, 7(6), e38855.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Köstering, L., McKinlay, A., Stahl, C., & Kaller, C. P. (2012). Differential patterns of planning impairments in Parkinson’s disease and sub-clinical signs of dementia? A latent-class model-based approach. PloS One, 7(6), e38855.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action. In: Dagenbach, D. & Carr, T.H., (Eds.) Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language, Academic Press, NewYork, pp 189–239. Logan, G. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action. In: Dagenbach, D. & Carr, T.H., (Eds.) Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language, Academic Press, NewYork, pp 189–239.
go back to reference Luciana, M., Collins, P. F., Olson, E. A., & Schissel, A. M. (2009). Tower of London performance in healthy adolescents: the development of planning skills and associations with self-reported inattention and impulsivity. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(4), 461–475.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Luciana, M., Collins, P. F., Olson, E. A., & Schissel, A. M. (2009). Tower of London performance in healthy adolescents: the development of planning skills and associations with self-reported inattention and impulsivity. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(4), 461–475.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Marzocchi, G. M., Oosterlaan, J., Zuddas, A., Cavolina, P., Geurts, H., Redigolo, D., & Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Contrasting deficits on executive functions between ADHD and reading disabled children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 543–552.CrossRefPubMed Marzocchi, G. M., Oosterlaan, J., Zuddas, A., Cavolina, P., Geurts, H., Redigolo, D., & Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Contrasting deficits on executive functions between ADHD and reading disabled children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 543–552.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1423–1442.CrossRef Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1423–1442.CrossRef
go back to reference Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.CrossRefPubMed Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Morris, R. G., Miotto, E. C., Feigenbaum, J. D., Bullock, P., & Polkey, C. E. (1997). The effect of goal-subgoal conflict on planning ability after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in humans. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1147–1157.CrossRefPubMed Morris, R. G., Miotto, E. C., Feigenbaum, J. D., Bullock, P., & Polkey, C. E. (1997). The effect of goal-subgoal conflict on planning ability after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in humans. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1147–1157.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., & Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1668–1682.CrossRefPubMed Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., & Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1668–1682.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Newman, S. D., & Pittman, G. (2007). The Tower of London: a study of the effect of problem structure on planning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(3), 333–342.CrossRefPubMed Newman, S. D., & Pittman, G. (2007). The Tower of London: a study of the effect of problem structure on planning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(3), 333–342.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference O’Reilly, R. C., & Frank, M. J. (2006). Making working memory work: a computational model of learning in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Neural Computation, 18(2), 283–328.CrossRefPubMed O’Reilly, R. C., & Frank, M. J. (2006). Making working memory work: a computational model of learning in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Neural Computation, 18(2), 283–328.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V., Gilhooly, K. J., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. H. (1999). The role of memory in the Tower of London task. Memory, 7(2), 209–231.CrossRefPubMed Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V., Gilhooly, K. J., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. H. (1999). The role of memory in the Tower of London task. Memory, 7(2), 209–231.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V. E., McPherson, S., & Gilhooly, K. J. (2001). Mental planning and the Tower of London task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 54(2), 579–597.CrossRef Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V. E., McPherson, S., & Gilhooly, K. J. (2001). Mental planning and the Tower of London task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 54(2), 579–597.CrossRef
go back to reference Rainville, C., Lepage, E., Gauthier, W., Kergoal, M.-J., & Belleville, S. (2012). Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: a study with a Tower of London task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 306–324.CrossRefPubMed Rainville, C., Lepage, E., Gauthier, W., Kergoal, M.-J., & Belleville, S. (2012). Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: a study with a Tower of London task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 306–324.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Raye, C., Johnson, M., Mitchell, K., Greene, E., & Johnson, M. (2007). Refreshing: a minimal executive function. Cortex, 43(1), 135–145.CrossRefPubMed Raye, C., Johnson, M., Mitchell, K., Greene, E., & Johnson, M. (2007). Refreshing: a minimal executive function. Cortex, 43(1), 135–145.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Reverberi, C., D’Agostini, S., Skrap, M., & Shallice, T. (2005). Generation and recognition of abstract rules in different frontal lobe subgroups. Neuropsychologia, 43(13), 1924–1937.CrossRefPubMed Reverberi, C., D’Agostini, S., Skrap, M., & Shallice, T. (2005). Generation and recognition of abstract rules in different frontal lobe subgroups. Neuropsychologia, 43(13), 1924–1937.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 298, 199–209.CrossRefPubMed Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 298, 199–209.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shallice, T., Stuss, D. T., Picton, T. W., Alexander, M. P., & Gillingham, S. (2008). Mapping task switching in frontal cortex through neuropsychological group studies. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 2, 79–85.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Shallice, T., Stuss, D. T., Picton, T. W., Alexander, M. P., & Gillingham, S. (2008). Mapping task switching in frontal cortex through neuropsychological group studies. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 2, 79–85.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Simon, H. A. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7(2), 268–288.CrossRef Simon, H. A. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7(2), 268–288.CrossRef
go back to reference Sullivan, J. R., Riccio, C. A., & Castillo, C. L. (2009). Concurrent validity of the tower tasks as measures of executive function in adults: a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 62–75.CrossRefPubMed Sullivan, J. R., Riccio, C. A., & Castillo, C. L. (2009). Concurrent validity of the tower tasks as measures of executive function in adults: a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 62–75.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Towse, J. N., & Neil, D. (1998). Analyzing human random generation behaviour: a review of methods used and a computer program for describing performance. Behaviour Research, Instruments and Computers, 30(4), 583–591.CrossRef Towse, J. N., & Neil, D. (1998). Analyzing human random generation behaviour: a review of methods used and a computer program for describing performance. Behaviour Research, Instruments and Computers, 30(4), 583–591.CrossRef
go back to reference Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 601–626.CrossRefPubMed Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 601–626.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Waldau, R. (1999). A developmental study of problem solving in children aged 3–6: Development of planning strategies. Unpublished BSc Thesis, University of London. Waldau, R. (1999). A developmental study of problem solving in children aged 3–6: Development of planning strategies. Unpublished BSc Thesis, University of London.
go back to reference Welsh, M. C., Satterlee-Cartmell, T., & Stine, M. (1999). Towers of Hanoi and London: contribution of working memory and inhibition to performance. Brain and Cognition, 41(2), 231–242.CrossRefPubMed Welsh, M. C., Satterlee-Cartmell, T., & Stine, M. (1999). Towers of Hanoi and London: contribution of working memory and inhibition to performance. Brain and Cognition, 41(2), 231–242.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wilson, M. (1988). MRC Psycholinguistic Database: Machine-usable dictionary, version 2.00. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20(1), 6–10.CrossRef Wilson, M. (1988). MRC Psycholinguistic Database: Machine-usable dictionary, version 2.00. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20(1), 6–10.CrossRef
go back to reference Zook, N. A., Davalos, D. B., DeLosh, E. L., & Davis, H. P. (2004). Working memory, inhibition, and fluid intelligence as predictors of performance on Tower of Hanoi and London tasks. Brain and Cognition, 56(3), 286–292.CrossRefPubMed Zook, N. A., Davalos, D. B., DeLosh, E. L., & Davis, H. P. (2004). Working memory, inhibition, and fluid intelligence as predictors of performance on Tower of Hanoi and London tasks. Brain and Cognition, 56(3), 286–292.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Set-shifting as a component process of goal-directed problem-solving
Auteurs
Richard P. Cooper
Verity Marsh
Publicatiedatum
17-02-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0652-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2016

Psychological Research 2/2016 Naar de uitgave