Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2012

01-07-2012 | Original Article

Predicting others’ actions via grasp and gaze: evidence for distinct brain networks

Auteurs: Richard Ramsey, Emily S. Cross, Antonia F. de C. Hamilton

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

During social interactions, how do we predict what other people are going to do next? One view is that we use our own motor experience to simulate and predict other people’s actions. For example, when we see Sally look at a coffee cup or grasp a hammer, our own motor system provides a signal that anticipates her next action. Previous research has typically examined such gaze and grasp-based simulation processes separately, and it is not known whether similar cognitive and brain systems underpin the perception of object-directed gaze and grasp. Here we use functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine to what extent gaze- and grasp-perception rely on common or distinct brain networks. Using a ‘peeping window’ protocol, we controlled what an observed actor could see and grasp. The actor could peep through one window to see if an object was present and reach through a different window to grasp the object. However, the actor could not peep and grasp at the same time. We compared gaze and grasp conditions where an object was present with matched conditions where the object was absent. When participants observed another person gaze at an object, left anterior inferior parietal lobule (aIPL) and parietal operculum showed a greater response than when the object was absent. In contrast, when participants observed the actor grasp an object, premotor, posterior parietal, fusiform and middle occipital brain regions showed a greater response than when the object was absent. These results point towards a division in the neural substrates for different types of motor simulation. We suggest that left aIPL and parietal operculum are involved in a predictive process that signals a future hand interaction with an object based on another person’s eye gaze, whereas a broader set of brain areas, including parts of the action observation network, are engaged during observation of an ongoing object-directed hand action.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 693–716.PubMedCrossRef Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 693–716.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Alaerts, K., de Beukelaar, T. T., Swinnen, S. P., & Wenderoth, N. (2011) Observing how others lift light or heavy objects: Time-dependent encoding of grip force in the primary motor cortex. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0380-1 (this issue) Alaerts, K., de Beukelaar, T. T., Swinnen, S. P., & Wenderoth, N. (2011) Observing how others lift light or heavy objects: Time-dependent encoding of grip force in the primary motor cortex. Psychological Research. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-011-0380-1 (this issue)
go back to reference Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 267–278.PubMedCrossRef Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 267–278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Becchio, C., Bertone, C., & Castiello, U. (2008). How the gaze of others influences object processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 254–258.PubMedCrossRef Becchio, C., Bertone, C., & Castiello, U. (2008). How the gaze of others influences object processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 254–258.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blakemore, S.-J., & Frith, C. (2005). The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action. Neuropsychologia, 43(2), 260–267.PubMedCrossRef Blakemore, S.-J., & Frith, C. (2005). The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action. Neuropsychologia, 43(2), 260–267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bristow, D., Rees, G., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Social interaction modifies neural response to gaze shifts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–61.PubMedCrossRef Bristow, D., Rees, G., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Social interaction modifies neural response to gaze shifts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Calder, A. J., Beaver, J. D., Winston, J. S., Dolan, R. J., Jenkins, R., Eger, E., et al. (2007). Separate coding of different gaze directions in the superior temporal sulcus and inferior parietal lobule. Current Biology, 17(1), 20–25.PubMedCrossRef Calder, A. J., Beaver, J. D., Winston, J. S., Dolan, R. J., Jenkins, R., Eger, E., et al. (2007). Separate coding of different gaze directions in the superior temporal sulcus and inferior parietal lobule. Current Biology, 17(1), 20–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage, 33(2), 430–448.PubMedCrossRef Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage, 33(2), 430–448.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1148–1167.PubMedCrossRef Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1148–1167.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.PubMedCrossRef Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215.PubMedCrossRef Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cross, E. S., Stadler, W., Parkinson, J., Schutz-Boabach, S., & Prinz, W. (2011). The influence of visual training on predicting complex action sequences. Human Brain Mapping. doi:10.1002/hbm.21450. Cross, E. S., Stadler, W., Parkinson, J., Schutz-Boabach, S., & Prinz, W. (2011). The influence of visual training on predicting complex action sequences. Human Brain Mapping. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​21450.
go back to reference Culham, J. C., Cavina-Pratesi, C., & Singhal, A. (2006). The role of parietal cortex in visuomotor control: What have we learned from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia, 44(13), 2668–2684.PubMedCrossRef Culham, J. C., Cavina-Pratesi, C., & Singhal, A. (2006). The role of parietal cortex in visuomotor control: What have we learned from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia, 44(13), 2668–2684.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Desmurget, M., & Grafton, S. (2000). Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 423–431.PubMedCrossRef Desmurget, M., & Grafton, S. (2000). Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 423–431.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 509–540.CrossRef Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 509–540.CrossRef
go back to reference Duvernoy, H. M. (1999). The human brain: surface, blood supply, and three-dimensional sectional anatomy. New York: Springer. Duvernoy, H. M. (1999). The human brain: surface, blood supply, and three-dimensional sectional anatomy. New York: Springer.
go back to reference Eickhoff, S. B., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., & Amunts, K. (2006). The human parietal operculum. I. Cytoarchitectonic mapping of subdivisions. Cerebral Cortex, 16(2), 254–267.PubMedCrossRef Eickhoff, S. B., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., & Amunts, K. (2006). The human parietal operculum. I. Cytoarchitectonic mapping of subdivisions. Cerebral Cortex, 16(2), 254–267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eickhoff, S., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G, R., Amunts, K., Zilles, K. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage 25(4), 1325–1335. Eickhoff, S., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G, R., Amunts, K., Zilles, K. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage 25(4), 1325–1335.
go back to reference Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604.PubMedCrossRef Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424(6950), 769–771.PubMedCrossRef Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424(6950), 769–771.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.PubMedCrossRef Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Friesen, C., K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 490–495. Friesen, C., K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 490–495.
go back to reference Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724.PubMedCrossRef Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Friston, K. J., Worsley, K. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Mazziotta, J. C., & Evans, A. C. (1994). Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Human Brain Mapping, 1(3), 210–220.CrossRef Friston, K. J., Worsley, K. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Mazziotta, J. C., & Evans, A. C. (1994). Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Human Brain Mapping, 1(3), 210–220.CrossRef
go back to reference Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2009). The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: Single-subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data. Cerebral Cortex, 19(6), 1239–1255.PubMedCrossRef Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2009). The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: Single-subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data. Cerebral Cortex, 19(6), 1239–1255.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 1–19.PubMedCrossRef Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 1–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grosbras, M.-H., Laird, A., R., & Paus, T. (2005). Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 140–154. Grosbras, M.-H., Laird, A., R., & Paus, T. (2005). Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 140–154.
go back to reference Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(03), 377–396.PubMed Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(03), 377–396.PubMed
go back to reference Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S., T.(2008). Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1160–1168. Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S., T.(2008). Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1160–1168.
go back to reference Hietanen, J. K., Nummenmaa, L., Nyman, M. J., Parkkola, R., & Hämäläinen, H. (2006). Automatic attention orienting by social and symbolic cues activates different neural networks: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 33(1), 406–413.PubMedCrossRef Hietanen, J. K., Nummenmaa, L., Nyman, M. J., Parkkola, R., & Hämäläinen, H. (2006). Automatic attention orienting by social and symbolic cues activates different neural networks: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 33(1), 406–413.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 80–84.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 80–84.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.PubMedCrossRef Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14(1), S103–S109.PubMedCrossRef Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14(1), S103–S109.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Johansson, R. S., Westling, G., Backstrom, A., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Eye–hand coordination in object manipulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(17), 6917–6932.PubMed Johansson, R. S., Westling, G., Backstrom, A., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Eye–hand coordination in object manipulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(17), 6917–6932.PubMed
go back to reference Keysers, C., Kaas, J. H., & Gazzola, V. (2010). Somatosensation in social perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(6), 417–428.PubMedCrossRef Keysers, C., Kaas, J. H., & Gazzola, V. (2010). Somatosensation in social perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(6), 417–428.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J. L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron, 42(2), 335–346.PubMedCrossRef Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J. L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron, 42(2), 335–346.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kilner, J. M., Vargas, C., Duval, S., Blakemore, S. J., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement. Nature Neuroscience, 7(12), 1299–1301.PubMedCrossRef Kilner, J. M., Vargas, C., Duval, S., Blakemore, S. J., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement. Nature Neuroscience, 7(12), 1299–1301.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Land, M., Mennie, N., & Rusted, J. (1999). The roles of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception, 28(11), 1311–1328.PubMedCrossRef Land, M., Mennie, N., & Rusted, J. (1999). The roles of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception, 28(11), 1311–1328.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Langton, S. R. H., & Bruce, V. (1999). Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 541–567.CrossRef Langton, S. R. H., & Bruce, V. (1999). Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 541–567.CrossRef
go back to reference Marsh, L. E., & Hamilton, A. F. d. C. (2011). Dissociation of mirroring and mentalising systems in autism. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1511–1519.PubMedCrossRef Marsh, L. E., & Hamilton, A. F. d. C. (2011). Dissociation of mirroring and mentalising systems in autism. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1511–1519.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., & Thier, P. (2008). Dissociable roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus in joint attention: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 108–119.PubMedCrossRef Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., & Thier, P. (2008). Dissociable roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus in joint attention: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 108–119.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morin, O., & Grezes, J. (2008). What is “mirror” in the premotor cortex? A review. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology, 38(3), 189–195.CrossRef Morin, O., & Grezes, J. (2008). What is “mirror” in the premotor cortex? A review. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology, 38(3), 189–195.CrossRef
go back to reference Nummenmaa, L., & Calder, A. J. (2009). Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 135–143.PubMedCrossRef Nummenmaa, L., & Calder, A. J. (2009). Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 135–143.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perrett, D. I., Smith, P. A. J., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Milner, A. D., et al. (1985). Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 223(1232), 293–317.CrossRef Perrett, D. I., Smith, P. A. J., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Milner, A. D., et al. (1985). Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 223(1232), 293–317.CrossRef
go back to reference Pierno, A. C., Becchio, C., Tubaldi, F., Turella, L., & Castiello, U. (2008). Motor ontology in representing gaze–object relations. Neuroscience Letters, 430(3), 246–251.PubMedCrossRef Pierno, A. C., Becchio, C., Tubaldi, F., Turella, L., & Castiello, U. (2008). Motor ontology in representing gaze–object relations. Neuroscience Letters, 430(3), 246–251.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pierno, A. C., Becchio, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., Turella, L., & Castiello, U. (2006). When gaze turns into grasp. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2130–2137.PubMedCrossRef Pierno, A. C., Becchio, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., Turella, L., & Castiello, U. (2006). When gaze turns into grasp. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2130–2137.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Ramsey, R., Cross, E. S., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2011). Eye can see what you want: Posterior intraparietal sulcus encodes the object of an actor’s gaze. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3400–3409. Ramsey, R., Cross, E. S., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2011). Eye can see what you want: Posterior intraparietal sulcus encodes the object of an actor’s gaze. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3400–3409.
go back to reference Redcay, E., Dodell-Feder, D., Pearrow, M. J., Mavros, P. L., Kleiner, M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., et al. (2010). Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: A new tool for social cognitive neuroscience. NeuroImage, 50(4), 1639–1647.PubMedCrossRef Redcay, E., Dodell-Feder, D., Pearrow, M. J., Mavros, P. L., Kleiner, M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., et al. (2010). Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: A new tool for social cognitive neuroscience. NeuroImage, 50(4), 1639–1647.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4), 264–274.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4), 264–274.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schilbach, L., Wilms, M., Eickhoff, S. B., Romanzetti, S., Tepest, R., Bente, G., et al. (2010). Minds made for sharing: Initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2702–2715.PubMedCrossRef Schilbach, L., Wilms, M., Eickhoff, S. B., Romanzetti, S., Tepest, R., Bente, G., et al. (2010). Minds made for sharing: Initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2702–2715.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 211–218. Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 211–218.
go back to reference Shepherd, S. V. (2010). Following gaze: Gaze-following behavior as a window into social cognition. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4. Shepherd, S. V. (2010). Following gaze: Gaze-following behavior as a window into social cognition. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4.
go back to reference Shepherd, S. V., Klein, J. T., Deaner, R. O., & Platt, M. L. (2009). Mirroring of attention by neurons in macaque parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(23), 9489–9494.CrossRef Shepherd, S. V., Klein, J. T., Deaner, R. O., & Platt, M. L. (2009). Mirroring of attention by neurons in macaque parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(23), 9489–9494.CrossRef
go back to reference Silver, M. A. and S. Kastner (2009). Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,13, 488–495. Silver, M. A. and S. Kastner (2009). Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,13, 488–495.
go back to reference Stadler, W., Schubotz, R. I., von Cramon, D. Y., Springer, A., Graf, M., & Prinz, W. (2011). Predicting and memorizing observed action: Differential premotor cortex involvement. Human Brain Mapping, 32(5), 677–687.PubMedCrossRef Stadler, W., Schubotz, R. I., von Cramon, D. Y., Springer, A., Graf, M., & Prinz, W. (2011). Predicting and memorizing observed action: Differential premotor cortex involvement. Human Brain Mapping, 32(5), 677–687.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (2010). From observation to action simulation: The role of attention, eye–gaze, emotion, and body state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2081–2105.CrossRef Tipper, S. P. (2010). From observation to action simulation: The role of attention, eye–gaze, emotion, and body state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2081–2105.CrossRef
go back to reference Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behaviour Brain Science, 28(5), 675–691. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behaviour Brain Science, 28(5), 675–691.
go back to reference Verfaillie, K., & Daems, A. (2002). Representing and anticipating human actions in vision. Visual Cognition, 9(1–2), 217–232.CrossRef Verfaillie, K., & Daems, A. (2002). Representing and anticipating human actions in vision. Visual Cognition, 9(1–2), 217–232.CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, J. H., Waiter, G. D., Perra, O., Perrett, D. I., & Whiten, A. (2005). An fMRI study of joint attention experience. NeuroImage, 25(1), 133–140.PubMedCrossRef Williams, J. H., Waiter, G. D., Perra, O., Perrett, D. I., & Whiten, A. (2005). An fMRI study of joint attention experience. NeuroImage, 25(1), 133–140.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460–473.PubMedCrossRef Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460–473.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11, 1317–1329.PubMedCrossRef Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11, 1317–1329.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Predicting others’ actions via grasp and gaze: evidence for distinct brain networks
Auteurs
Richard Ramsey
Emily S. Cross
Antonia F. de C. Hamilton
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0393-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2012

Psychological Research 4/2012 Naar de uitgave