Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2013

01-09-2013 | Original Article

Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: evidence for a basis in spatial coding

Auteurs: Dongbin Tobin Cho, Robert W. Proctor

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

It has been proposed that grasping affordances produce a Simon-type correspondence effect for left–right keypress responses and the location of the graspable part of an object for judgments based on action-relevant properties such as shape, but not on surface properties. We tested the implications of this grasping affordance account and contrasted them with the ones derived from a spatial coding account that distinguishes holistic processing of integral dimensions and analytic processing of separable dimensions. In Experiments 1–3, judgments about the color of a door handle showed a Simon effect relative to the handle’s base, whereas judgments about the handle’s shape showed no Simon effect. In Experiment 4, when the middle of the handle was colored, the Simon effect was obtained relative to the base, but when the color was at the tip of the handle or near the base, Simon effects were obtained relative to the color location. For Experiment 5, only the base was colored, and the Simon effect was larger for a passive rather than active handle state, as in the color-judgment conditions of Experiments 2–4 in which the colored region overlapped with the base. In Experiment 6, orientation judgments showed no Simon effect, as the shape judgments did in Experiments 1 and 2. The findings of (a) an absence of Simon effects for shape and orientation judgments, (b) no larger Simon effects for active than passive handle states, and (c) isolation of the changing component for color judgments are consistent with the spatial coding account, according to which the distinction between object shape/orientation and color is one of integral versus separable dimensions.
Voetnoten
1
The number of participants was doubled in this experiment. because subsequent experiments were designed based on the results of Experiment 2. Thus, the experiment was conducted originally with 40 participants, and then a replication was conducted with an additional 40 participants to ensure that the main results were reliable. The resulting RT data were analyzed in a single ANOVA of trial block (first half of trial blocks, second half) × state of handle (active, passive) × correspondence (corresponding, noncorresponding) × experiment (original, replication) × condition (color, shape). For this analysis, the only significant term involving experiment was the four-way interaction of trial block × correspondence × condition × experiment, F(1, 76) = 8.10, p = .006, η p 2  = .10. This interaction was due mainly to a somewhat different pattern of results across the two trial blocks for the shape-judgment condition in the two experiments. Because the main results were consistent across the replications, we report the combined data of the 80 participants for Experiment 2.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anzola, G. P., Bertoloni, G. G., Buchtel, H. A., & Rizzolatti, G. G. (1977). Spatial compatibility and anatomical factors in simple and choice reaction time. Neuropsychologia, 15, 295–302.PubMedCrossRef Anzola, G. P., Bertoloni, G. G., Buchtel, H. A., & Rizzolatti, G. G. (1977). Spatial compatibility and anatomical factors in simple and choice reaction time. Neuropsychologia, 15, 295–302.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Bonfiglioli, C., Lugli, L., Ricciardelli, P., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2007). Are visual stimuli sufficient to evoke motor information? Studies with hand primes. Neuroscience Letters, 411, 17–21.PubMedCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Bonfiglioli, C., Lugli, L., Ricciardelli, P., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2007). Are visual stimuli sufficient to evoke motor information? Studies with hand primes. Neuroscience Letters, 411, 17–21.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. J. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: On the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 341–358.PubMedCrossRef Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. J. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: On the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 341–358.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cant, J. S., & Goodale, M. A. (2009). Asymmetric interference between the perception of shape and the perception of surface properties. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–20.PubMedCrossRef Cant, J. S., & Goodale, M. A. (2009). Asymmetric interference between the perception of shape and the perception of surface properties. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cant, J. S., & Goodale, M. A. (2011). Scratching beneath the surface: New insights into the functional properties of the lateral occipital area and parahippocampal place area. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 8248–8258.PubMedCrossRef Cant, J. S., & Goodale, M. A. (2011). Scratching beneath the surface: New insights into the functional properties of the lateral occipital area and parahippocampal place area. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 8248–8258.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cant, J. S., Large, M. E., McCall, L., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Independent processing of form, colour, and texture in object perception. Perception, 37, 57–78.PubMedCrossRef Cant, J. S., Large, M. E., McCall, L., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Independent processing of form, colour, and texture in object perception. Perception, 37, 57–78.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cho, Y. S., Choi, J. M., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Likelihood of attending to the color word modulates Stroop interference. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 416–429.CrossRef Cho, Y. S., Choi, J. M., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Likelihood of attending to the color word modulates Stroop interference. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 416–429.CrossRef
go back to reference Cho, D. (T.), & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMedCrossRef Cho, D. (T.), & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cho, D. (T.), & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 737–749.PubMedCrossRef Cho, D. (T.), & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 737–749.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C–. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef De Jong, R., Liang, C–. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Derbyshire, N., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2006). The potentiation of two components of the reach-to-grasp action during object categorisation in visual memory. Acta Psychologica, 122, 74–98. Derbyshire, N., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2006). The potentiation of two components of the reach-to-grasp action during object categorisation in visual memory. Acta Psychologica, 122, 74–98.
go back to reference Dick, M., & Hochstein, S. (1988). Interactions in the discrimination and absolute judgment of orientation and length. Perception, 17, 177–189.PubMedCrossRef Dick, M., & Hochstein, S. (1988). Interactions in the discrimination and absolute judgment of orientation and length. Perception, 17, 177–189.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dykes, J. R., & Cooper, R. G. (1978). An investigation of the perceptual basis of redundancy gain and orthogonal interference for integral dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 36–42.CrossRef Dykes, J. R., & Cooper, R. G. (1978). An investigation of the perceptual basis of redundancy gain and orthogonal interference for integral dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 36–42.CrossRef
go back to reference Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordance: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 451–471.PubMedCrossRef Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordance: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 451–471.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Felfoldy, G. L. (1974). Repetition effects in choice reaction time to multidimensional stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 15, 453–459.CrossRef Felfoldy, G. L. (1974). Repetition effects in choice reaction time to multidimensional stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 15, 453–459.CrossRef
go back to reference Fitts, P. M., & Deininger, R. L. (1954). S–R compatibility: Correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 483–492.PubMedCrossRef Fitts, P. M., & Deininger, R. L. (1954). S–R compatibility: Correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 483–492.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Galpin, A., Tipper, S. P., Dick, J. P. R., & Poliakoff, E. (2011). Object affordance and spatial compatibility effects in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 47, 332–341.PubMedCrossRef Galpin, A., Tipper, S. P., Dick, J. P. R., & Poliakoff, E. (2011). Object affordance and spatial compatibility effects in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 47, 332–341.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426, 664–667.PubMedCrossRef Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426, 664–667.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum. Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Geyer, T., & Müller, H. J. (2009). Distinct, but top-down modulable color and positional priming mechanisms in visual pop-out search. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 73, 167–176.CrossRef Geyer, T., & Müller, H. J. (2009). Distinct, but top-down modulable color and positional priming mechanisms in visual pop-out search. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 73, 167–176.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Iani, C., Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., & Nicoletti, R. (2011). On the relationship between affordance and Simon effects: Are the effects really independent? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 121–131.CrossRef Iani, C., Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., & Nicoletti, R. (2011). On the relationship between affordance and Simon effects: Are the effects really independent? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 121–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loach, D., Frischen, A., Bruce, N., & Tsotsos, J. (2008). An attentional mechanism for selecting appropriate actions afforded by graspable objects. Psychological Science, 19, 1253–1257.PubMedCrossRef Loach, D., Frischen, A., Bruce, N., & Tsotsos, J. (2008). An attentional mechanism for selecting appropriate actions afforded by graspable objects. Psychological Science, 19, 1253–1257.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lu, C. –. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.CrossRef Lu, C. –. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.CrossRef
go back to reference Macmillan, N. A., & Ornstein, A. S. (1998). The mean-integral representation of rectangles. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 250–262.CrossRef Macmillan, N. A., & Ornstein, A. S. (1998). The mean-integral representation of rectangles. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 250–262.CrossRef
go back to reference Maetens, K., Henderickx, D., & Soetens, E. (2009). Binding of event files in a (go/no-go) Simon task with an accessory peripheral signal. Experimental Psychology, 56, 100–111.PubMedCrossRef Maetens, K., Henderickx, D., & Soetens, E. (2009). Binding of event files in a (go/no-go) Simon task with an accessory peripheral signal. Experimental Psychology, 56, 100–111.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., Anzola, G. P., Luppino, G., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1982). Spatial compatibility effects on the same side of body midline. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 664–673.PubMedCrossRef Nicoletti, R., Anzola, G. P., Luppino, G., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1982). Spatial compatibility effects on the same side of body midline. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 664–673.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pellicano, A., Iani, C., Borghi, A. M., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2010). Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: The effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2190–2201.CrossRef Pellicano, A., Iani, C., Borghi, A. M., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2010). Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: The effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2190–2201.CrossRef
go back to reference Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S–R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Time course and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9, 540–548.CrossRef Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S–R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Time course and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9, 540–548.CrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 242–266.CrossRef Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 242–266.CrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Pick, D. F., Vu, K.-P. L., & Anderson, R. E. (2005). The enhanced Simon effect for older adults is reduced when the irrelevant location information is conveyed by an accessory stimulus. Acta Psychologica, 119, 21–40.PubMedCrossRef Proctor, R. W., Pick, D. F., Vu, K.-P. L., & Anderson, R. E. (2005). The enhanced Simon effect for older adults is reduced when the irrelevant location information is conveyed by an accessory stimulus. Acta Psychologica, 119, 21–40.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (1990). Research on stimulus–response compatibility: Toward a comprehensive account. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 483–494). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (1990). Research on stimulus–response compatibility: Toward a comprehensive account. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 483–494). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Riggio, L., Iani, C., Gherri, E., Benatti, F., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2008). The role of attention in the occurrence of the affordance effect. Acta Psychologica, 127, 449–458.PubMedCrossRef Riggio, L., Iani, C., Gherri, E., Benatti, F., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2008). The role of attention in the occurrence of the affordance effect. Acta Psychologica, 127, 449–458.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rubichi, S., Iani, C., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1353–1364.PubMedCrossRef Rubichi, S., Iani, C., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1353–1364.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of human performance: Reaction processes and attention in human skill. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of human performance: Reaction processes and attention in human skill. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility. An integrated perspective (pp. 31-86). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility. An integrated perspective (pp. 31-86). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Simon, J. R., Craft, J. L., & Webster, J. B. (1973). Reactions toward the stimulus source: Analysis of correct responses and errors over a five-day period. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 175–178.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R., Craft, J. L., & Webster, J. B. (1973). Reactions toward the stimulus source: Analysis of correct responses and errors over a five-day period. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 175–178.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision for action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance Simon effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 493–498.CrossRef Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision for action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance Simon effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 493–498.CrossRef
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMedCrossRef Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8, 769–800.CrossRef Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8, 769–800.CrossRef
go back to reference Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus–response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–116). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus–response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–116). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Vainio, L., Symes, E., Ellis, R., Tucker, M., & Ottoboni, G. (2008). On the relations between action planning, object identification, and motor representations of observed actions and objects. Cognition, 108, 444–465.PubMedCrossRef Vainio, L., Symes, E., Ellis, R., Tucker, M., & Ottoboni, G. (2008). On the relations between action planning, object identification, and motor representations of observed actions and objects. Cognition, 108, 444–465.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Multidimensional vector model of stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Review, 119, 272–303.PubMedCrossRef Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Multidimensional vector model of stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Review, 119, 272–303.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong and Liang and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551.CrossRef Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong and Liang and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: evidence for a basis in spatial coding
Auteurs
Dongbin Tobin Cho
Robert W. Proctor
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2013
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0458-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2013

Psychological Research 5/2013 Naar de uitgave