Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 1/2023

Open Access 03-08-2022

Multi-Trajectories of Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity/Inattention, and Peer Problems Across Childhood: Results from the Growing Up in Scotland Birth Cohort

Auteurs: María Francisca Morales, Angus MacBeth, Samantha Swartzman, Lisa-Christine Girard

Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology | Uitgave 1/2023

Abstract

Using a person-centred approach, this study inspected multi-trajectories of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention and peer problems, and associated risk factors for group membership. The sample included 3,578 children (50.8% males) from a population birth cohort in Scotland (Growing Up in Scotland). The parental version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was used when children were 4, 5, 6, 7, and 10 years old. Antecedent factors at the perinatal, child, and family levels were collected using parental reports, observation, and standardised assessments at 10, 24, and 36 months. A group-based multi-trajectory analysis was employed. Findings showed that a six-group model best fit the data. Identified groups included non-engagers, normative, decreasing externalising/low peer problems, low externalising/moderate peer problems, moderate externalising/increasing peer problems and multimorbid moderate-high chronic. Findings suggest multimorbidity between externalising behaviours and peer problems in the more elevated groups. Two common protective factors emerged across all groups: caregiver mental health and parent-infant attachment. Identified risk factors were specific to group membership. Risk factors for the most elevated group included single-parent status, social deprivation, previous neonatal intensive care unit admission, child sex, whilst children’s expressive language was a protective factor. Taken together, findings contribute to the emerging literature modelling trajectories of externalising behaviours and peer problems simultaneously and have important practical implications for prevention of problems in childhood, by identifying targets at the perinatal, child, and family levels.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-022-00956-8.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Externalising problems, including conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention, are increasingly common behavioural difficulties, with an estimated ~ 3% of children displaying conduct disorders and ~ 4% presenting with hyperactivity/inattention (Barican et al., 2022; Fairchild et al., 2019). Early externalising problems in childhood have been linked to long-term difficulties such as peer problems and rejection, academic failure, substance abuse, and antisocial behaviours (Bevilacqua et al., 2018; Flouri et al., 2019; Hong et al., 2014; Murray et al., 2021; Pingault et al., 2013). Particular attention has been focused on unpacking the association between externalising behaviours and peer problems during childhood (Haas et al., 2018; Laird et al., 2001). Chen et al., (2015) highlighted both the prospective and concurrent risk association between conduct problems and peer problems such as rejection, while Hoza et al. (2005) demonstrated that children with hyperactivity/inattention score lower on social preference at school and are more likely to be rejected by peers. Reijntjes et al. (2011) conducted a meta-analysis to inspect two prospective paths for this association: the extent to which peer victimisation predicted future changes in externalising problems and whether externalising behaviours predicted later peer problems. They concluded that externalising behaviours are both antecedent and consequences of peer problems across time.
These findings support previous theory describing a bidirectional association between externalising behaviours and peer problems. For example, children exhibiting conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention difficulties may exasperate other children, subsequently inducing negative reactions from these other children towards them, such as being victimisation or rejection (Thorell et al., 2017). Whereas experiencing negative peer relationships could alternatively lead to difficulties in regulating children’s emotions in adverse social situations, resulting in the appearance of disruptive behaviours (Haltigan & Vaillancourt, 2014; Schwartz et al., 2001) or exhibiting externalising behaviours to defend themselves (Averdijk et al., 2016; Reijntjes et al., 2011). Moreover, the overlap between conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention has been associated with experiencing more peer problems in childhood, suggesting that negative peer relationships are associated with a range of externalising problems (Andrade & Tannock, 2013). In addition to the long-term consequences of externalising behaviours, peer problems have also been associated with later adverse outcomes, such as poor academic performance, lower self-esteem, and depressive symptoms (Isaacs et al., 2008; Laird et al., 2001). Therefore, considering the long-term negative outcomes of both externalising behaviours and peer problems and the identified association between these difficulties, it is important to gain a better understanding of their longitudinal presentations across childhood and to examine how these behaviours may feed into the other across time. Identification of associated risk and protective factors to understand aetiology of potential concomitant developmental trajectories of externalising problems and peer problems is also warranted.
Longitudinal research has identified multiple trajectory groups when examining individual trajectories of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems. For conduct problems, low, moderate-desisting, high-desisting and high-chronic have been described (Olson et al., 2017; Shaw et al., 2003, 2005). In these subgroups, a large majority of children display a normative peak of conduct problems from two to five years old, which then decrease through childhood once cognitive, language, and regulation abilities have been better developed (Cole et al., 2011; Tremblay, 2010). However, a small percentage of children, usually representing ~ 5% of the sampled populations have been found to display persistent conduct problems, which remain high across childhood and adolescence (Girard et al., 2019; Nagin & Tremblay, 1999). In the case of hyperactivity/inattention, studies examining developmental trajectories have suggested a group of children with a general decline in symptoms and another group with stable problems over time (Musser et al., 2016; Sasser et al., 2016; Vergunst et al., 2019). The stability of hyperactivity/inattention has been mainly explained through genetic factors, whereas fluctuations over time are thought to be influenced by environmental determinants (Kan et al., 2013). Finally, some longitudinal studies of peer problems have described subgroups of children with increasing trajectories of peer victimisation, including low/increasing, moderate/increasing, and high/increasing (Barker et al., 2008; Boivin et al., 2010), whereas others have shown a general decline across the school period, with trajectory subtypes identified as low, childhood limited, moderate-emerging stable, and high chronic (Geoffroy et al., 2018; Oncioiu et al., 2020).
Nevertheless, despite increased research on individual trajectories of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems, only one study has examined concomitant trajectories of these behaviours across childhood (Girard, 2021). Using a person-centred approach in a European cohort (Ireland; Growing Up in Ireland), Girard (2021) examined the longitudinal presentation of internalising, externalising, and peer problems from three to nine years old, identifying six subgroups who evidenced multimorbidity across behaviours. Within these groups, three presented no or low symptoms, and three showed more elevated problems across time, with the last one displaying persistent multimorbid difficulties (i.e., ‘moderate increasing internalising/moderate decreasing-stable externalising and peer problems’, ‘low increasing internalising/mixed externalising/stable peer problems’, ‘high chronic-increasing multimorbid’). However, as this study inspected joint presentations of internalising, externalising, and peer problems, there is still a need for findings reporting the unique concomitant trajectories of externalising behaviours and peer problems. To the best of our knowledge, no study has simultaneously examined the unique longitudinal presentation of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems across childhood.
Furthermore, considering multiple risk and protective factors to discriminate between developmental trajectories will provide new understanding around the factors specific to multimorbid conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention and peer problems. Following the ecological system theory approach (Bronfenbrenner, 1992), perinatal, child, and family risk and protective factors were considered in this study. Perinatal factors such as low birth weight and early experiences in a neonatal unit (NICU) have been associated with higher rates of conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention (Anderson et al., 2003; Greenley et al., 2007; Mathewson et al., 2017). These factors may reflect neurodevelopmental immaturity (Shum et al., 2008; Skranes et al., 2007) and stressful experiences for the baby in the NICU (Linsell et al., 2019), which may impact later development. At the child level, language abilities and sex have been described as important factors for externalising problems and peer problems outcomes. With respect to children’s language, population-based cohort samples have revealed significant bidirectional associations between language abilities and externalising problems such as conduct problems (Girard et al., 2014, 2016). In addition, the gender paradox theory highlighted that conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention vary among girls and boys, with boys displaying greater problems over time (Loeber & Keenan, 1994). Proposed reasons for these sex differences may be associated with social and genetic factors. Regarding social factors, it has been suggested that socialisation of gender roles for boys and girls start from early stages in development, with parents and the broader contextual environment, which impact upon distinct childhood problems (Tremblay & Côté, 2019). For example, boys may be socialised to compete and fight, which may increase externalising behaviours. Furthermore, genetic influences have also been studied to clarify sex differences (Dmitrieva et al., 2011), with evidence indicating that girls need more risk genes than boys before exhibiting externalising problems (Rhee & Waldman, 2004; Rhee et al., 1999). Concerning peer problems, there is mixed evidence for sex differences. Some research has suggested that girls are more likely to experience peer problems such as rejection and victimisation (e.g., Humphreys et al., 2013), while others have shown that boys are more likely to experience these types of problems (e.g., Nansel et al., 2001). A potential explanation for these mixed findings is that sex differences may be related to the specific types of peer problems examined. For example, girls may be more likely to experience a higher degree of relational peer problems whereas boys more experience higher levels of physical victimisation (Rose & Rudolph, 2006).
At the family level, several maternal characteristics have been related to children’s poorer outcomes, including young maternal age (Chang et al., 2014), single status (Alavi et al., 2017), lower educational attainment (Alavi et al., 2017), and mental health problems, such as depression (Kingston et al., 2018). Moreover, parent–child attachment style has also been associated with children’s externalising behaviours. For example, meta-analytic data demonstrated that insecure attachments were significantly associated with the development of externalising behaviours (Fearon et al., 2010). Maternal characteristics and interaction styles may cumulatively impact upon children’s development since multiple risk factors, instead of single factors, predict children’s behavioural and social outcomes (Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). Finally, the socioeconomic level (SES) represented by the family’s level of social deprivation has been reported as a significant factor for children’s externalising problems (i.e., conduct problems and attention/hyperactivity; Miller & Votruba-Drzal, 2017; Votruba-Drzal, 2006). Children living in environments characterised by high levels of social deprivation, such as poverty and unemployment, often manifest externalising behaviours, potentially because of higher levels of family stress, less investment in resources such as education and health, and social exclusion (Miller & Votruba-Drzal, 2017).
In summary, externalising behaviours and peer problems have been found to have long term negative consequences for children’s wellbeing (Bevilacqua et al., 2018; Flouri et al., 2019; Hong et al., 2014; Isaacs et al., 2008; Laird et al., 2001; Murray et al., 2021; Pingault et al., 2013). Moreover, associations have been described between externalising behaviours and peer problems (Chen et al., 2015; Healy et al., 2015; Hoza et al., 2005; Keiley et al., 2003; Reijntjes et al., 2011). However, to the best of our knowledge, only one study (Girard, 2021) has inspected multi-trajectories of these behaviours simultaneously, and none to date has focused uniquely on the longitudinal presentations of conduct, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems. Consequently, this study has two aims. First, to inspect developmental trajectories of concurrent conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems in children between four and 10 years of age to identify subgroups following distinct patterns of co- and/or multimorbid problems across childhood. Second, to detect risk and protective factors for group membership. Based on the existing literature (Olson et al., 2017; Shaw et al., 2003, 2005), a four-group model was hypothesised to best fit the data, including a never engaging group with no-to low problems, a normative group with low-moderate decreasing externalising problems and low peer problems, a high-decreasing group with initially high but decreasing externalising problems and moderate peer problems, and a persistent stable high group. We anticipate these different longitudinal presentations since, after the preschool stage, a general decline of externalising problems is expected in the majority of children, with only a small proportion of children exhibiting high stable problems over time. Moreover, according to the reported associations between externalising problems and peer problems, it is also expected that groups with high decreasing or stable conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention will present with more peer problems across childhood than groups with no to low externalising behaviours. Finally, according to the cumulative risk model (Atzaba-Poria et al., 2004), it was anticipated that these groups would have different aetiological antecedents. More precisely, we anticipated that groups with higher externalising problems and peer problems would present with more associated risk factors at the perinatal (i.e., low birth weight, NICU stay), child (i.e., boys, lower expressive language skills), and family levels (i.e., younger mothers, single parents, no educational qualifications, low mental health scores, low maternal-infant attachment, rural areas, most socially deprived families) than children within the no-engagers or normative groups.

Methods

Participants

This study used data from families enrolled in the Growing Up in Scotland (GUS) cohort study. The GUS dataset is a population-based cohort from Scotland launched in 2005 with children identified through the child benefit tax register. The initial sample included two cohorts, a birth cohort (N = 5,217) comprising infants born between 2004 and 2005 and a child cohort (N = 2,858) involving children born between 2002 and 2003. Additional details of recruitment and data collection can be found in Anderson et al. (2007). Data from sweeps 1 to 8 for the birth cohort were used, corresponding to when children were 10 months, two, three, four, five, six, eight and 10 years old. Inclusion criteria in the current study were (i) children who were enrolled in the birth cohort sample to ensure information on antecedents of early risk factors were available, and (ii) children having at least three assessments of conduct, hyperactivity/inattention and peer problems from sweeps 4 to 8, to model quadratic growth. Thus, the final sample in this study includes 3,578 families (68.6% of the total initially recruited sample). Sample characteristics of the included cohort as compared to those excluded from the current study can be found in Table 1. Comparison between samples revealed a greater degree of advantage across birth and demographic characteristics for children included in this study. Ethical approval was granted from Scotland’s Scottish “A” Multicentre Research Ethics Committee (MREC). Informed written parental consent was collected prior to each wave of data collection.
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the excluded sample and the included sample
 
Excluded sample
(n = 1,639)
Included sample
(n = 3,578)
p
Low birth weight (yes)
125 (7.7%)
214 (6.0%)
0.024
NICU stay (yes)
221 (13.5%)
383 (10.7%)
0.003
Child sex (male)
788 (52.7%)
1,819 (50.8%)
 
Child expressive language
67.9 (20.0)
73.2 (18.2)
 ≤ 0.001
Maternal age (under 20 years of age)
181 (12.1%)
160 (4.5%)
 ≤ 0.001
Single parent (yes)
487 (29.7%)
492 (13.8%)
 ≤ 0.001
Maternal education (no qualifications)
242 (14.8%)
229 (6.4%)
 ≤ 0.001
Caregiver mental health
0.1 (1.0)
-0.02 (0.9)
0.001
Maternal-infant attachment
24.6 (2.6)
24.2 (2.5)
 ≤ 0.001
Area (rural)
223 (13.6%)
737 (20.6%)
 ≤ 0.001
Social deprivation (most deprived)
471 (31.5%)
654 (18.3%)
 ≤ 0.001
Low birth weight < 2.500 gr. Child expressive language = British Ability Scales version II (BAS-II). Maternal education = having no qualifications as maximum educational level vs. having qualifications (e.g., standard grade, higher grade, vocational qualification, degree). Caregiver mental health = subset of six items from the Depression and Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS). Maternal-infant attachment = subset of six items from the Condon Maternal Postnatal Attachment Scale. Social deprivation = assessed by means of Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation (SIMD) scores. Most deprived quintile vs. other four quintiles. Means and (standard deviations) are presented for child expressive language and maternal-infant attachment

Measures

Children’s Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity/Inattention, and Peer Problems

Children’s conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems were assessed using the parent version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997) when children were four, five, six, eight, and 10 years old. The SDQ is a screening tool assessing multiple behavioural and emotional difficulties in children aged three to 16 years old, comprising 25 items classified into five subscales (i.e., emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer problems, and prosocial behaviours). In this study we use the conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems subscales. Parents rate each item on a 3-point scale from 0 (not true) to 2 (certainly true), with subscale scores ranging from 0 to 10. According to previously proposed cut-offs (Girard, 2021; YouthInMind, 2013), conduct problems scores were specified as low (0–2), moderate (3), and high (4–10), hyperactivity/inattention problems as low (0–4), moderate (5–6), and high (7–10), and peer problems as low (0–1), moderate (2), and high (3–10). The psychometric properties of the SDQ have been extensively researched and well validated (Goodman, 2001), and the SDQ parent version has demonstrated factorial invariance over time, which supports its use in longitudinal research (Sosu & Schmidt, 2017). Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample were 0.49, 0.66, 0.57, 0.64, 0.60 for the conduct problems subscale, 0.71, 0.79, 0.76, 0.80, 0.79 for the hyperactivity/inattention subscale, and 0.42, 0.52, 0.52, 0.55, 0.55 for the peer problems subscale at four, five, six, eight, and 10 years respectively.

Risk and Protective Factors

Antecedent factors were gathered from sweeps 1 to 3, when children were 10, 25, and 36 months, respectively. At sweep 1, parents and fieldworkers provided reports on birth outcomes, child, and family antecedents, including low birth weight (categorised as under 2.500 gr.; yes/no), time in the NICU after childbirth (yes/no), child sex (male/female), maternal age (20 years or less; yes/no), marital status (single/not single), maternal educational level (no educational qualifications; yes/no), area of family residence (urban/rural). Maternal-infant attachment was assessed with maternal reports using a subset of six items from the Condon Maternal Postnatal Attachment Scale (Condon & Corkindale, 1998), with higher scores indicating better attachment with the infant. Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample was 0.55. The social deprivation quintile was measured using Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation (SIMD) scores, which are assigned according to whether the postcode in which the primary caregiver lives are in the least, second-to-least, middle, second-to-most or most deprived fifths of postcodes overall across Scotland (most deprived; yes/no) (Scottish Government, 2020). At sweep 2, primary caregiver mental health was assessed using the z-scores of a subset of six items from the Depression and Anxiety Stress Scales answered by the primary caregiver (DASS; Lovibond et al., 1995), with positive scores reflecting better than average mental health and negative scores poorer than average mental health. Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample was 0.90. Finally, at sweep 3, children’s expressive language abilities were measured by fieldworkers using the Naming Vocabulary subtest from the British Ability Scales Version 2 (BAS-II; Elliott, Smith, & McCullock, 1997), with higher scores denoting better expressive language abilities. The BAS-II has demonstrated construct validity and correlations with similar measures of cognitive functioning, such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (Cook, 1988).

Statistical Analysis

To examine concomitant trajectories of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems, a group-based multi-trajectory modelling analysis (Nagin, 2005; Nagin et al., 2016) was conducted. The group-based multi-trajectory model is an extension to the original group-based modelling analysis (Nagin, 2005) and adheres to a person-centred approach (Magnusson, 2003; Nagin, 2005). This analysis is an application of finite mixture modelling for identifying trajectory groups following diverse developmental patterns and considering multiple variables at once (e.g., conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer problems). According to Nagin’s recommendations (Nagin, 2005), two steps were followed for group selection: defining the number of groups that best fit the data and selecting the shape of each trajectory by determining polynomial terms for each group. For the first step, two to eight groups were assessed to compare model fit according to the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), with a larger BIC (in contrast to alternative modelling approaches) indicating a better fitting model (see Nagin, 2005). The main fit improvement occurred with the six-group model since the following groups were characterised by a more minimal BIC change from group 6 to group 7 and from group 7 to group 8 (Table 2). Further inspection of each model’s graph showed no distinguishing group difference when adding more groups (i.e., 7 and 8 groups). Therefore, considering the main fit improvement of the BIC statistic and the principle of parsimony, a six-group multi-trajectory model was chosen as best describing the distinct characteristics of the data.
Table 2
Comparison of the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) for assessment of model fit
 
BIC
BIC
AIC
2-group
-86319.86 (N = 50370)
-86290.77 (N = 3578)
-86222.77
3-group
-84508.31 (N = 50370)
-84465.99 (N = 3578)
-84367.07
4-group
-83895.64 (N = 50370)
-83840.11 (N = 3578)
-83710.27
5-group
-83404.10 (N = 50,370)
-83335.34 (N = 3578)
-83174.59
6-group
-83089.28 (N = 50370)
-83007.30 (N = 3578)
-82815.64
7-group
-82846.49 (N = 50370)
-82751.28 (N = 3578)
-82528.71
8-group
-82667.44 (N = 50370)
-82559.01 (N = 3578)
-82305.52
Sample size in the first BIC column represents the total number of assessments used within the model estimation across time and participants. Sample size in the second BIC column denotes the actual sample size used in the trajectories. The theoretically correct BIC score lies in between these two indices (Nagin, 2005)
For the second step, polynomial terms (i.e., quadratic, linear, and constant) were tested in the six-group multi-trajectory model until each parameter estimate was statistically significant. After selecting the number of groups that best fit the data and selecting the polynomial terms for each group, further criteria, such as the average posterior probabilities and the odds of correct classification were applied to assess the fit of the selected model (Nagin, 2005). The average posterior probabilities denote the chances that an individual displaying a particular pattern of behaviours will be assigned to a specific trajectory group. Odds of correct classification quantifies how much better the model is at allocating participants to groups than randomly assigning participants based on group size. Nagin (2005) suggested that posterior probabilities greater than 0.7 (or 70%) and odds of correct classification higher than 5 indicate good model fit. In this study, both indices offered further support for the six-group multi-trajectory model (Table 3). After model selection, risk factors for group membership were assessed at the bivariate level using analysis of variance and chi-square tests. Subsequently, only statistically significant factors at the bivariate level were entered into the multi-trajectory model estimation. Analyses were conducted using Stata v17.0. The statistical threshold was set to p = 0.05. The term significance is used in place of statistical significance henceforward.
Table 3
Model fit criterion of conduct problems, attention/hyperactivity and peer problems multi-trajectories
Trajectory Group
n
Average Posterior Probability of Group Membership
Odds of Correct Classification
1
761
91.1
37.7
2
593
79.1
19.1
3
661
78.1
15.8
4
520
81.9
26.6
5
742
85.1
21.8
6
301
93.9
169.0
Membership probability greater than 70 and OCC greater than 5 represent good model fit

Results

Group-Based Multi-Trajectories

Six trajectory groups were identified, exhibiting distinctive patterns of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems from four to 10 years old. Group 1 represented 21.1% of the estimated cohort and was the largest group. This group was labelled ‘non-engagers’ and was characterised by low decreasing conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention, and low stable peer problems. Group 2 included an estimated 17.1% of children and was labelled ‘normative’. Compared to Group 1, this group also showed low decreasing conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention, and low stable peer problems. However, the level of both conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention were more elevated in Group 2 as compared to Group 1. Group 3 comprised an estimated 18.2% of children and was labelled ‘decreasing externalising/low peer problems’. In this group, both conduct and peer problems followed a quadratic trajectory, similar to the first and second groups, showing low decreasing conduct problems and low stable peer problems. Hyperactivity/inattention problems was more elevated in this group as compared to the previous two groups and followed a linear decreasing trajectory between four to ten years old. Group 4 included 14.9% of the estimated cohort and was labelled ‘low externalising/moderate peer problems’. Similar to group 3, conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention were initially low and followed a decreasing trajectory. In contrast, peer problems were moderate at four years old, remaining constant thereafter. Group 5 included an estimated 20.2% of children and was labelled ‘moderate externalising/increasing peer problems’. Conduct problems were initially moderate and followed a slightly decreasing trajectory, while hyperactivity/inattention started at a moderate level, following a peak at age seven and then marginally decreasing at age 10. Peer problems had a linear increasing trajectory over time, although at a lower initial level and approaching moderate levels at age 10. Finally, Group 6 included an estimated 8.6% of the cohort, labelled as ‘multimorbid moderate-high chronic’. This group was characterised by moderate conduct problems, following a peak at age seven and then slightly decreasing when children were 10 years old. Hyperactivity/inattention problems also followed a quadratic trajectory with a peak at age seven and a marginal decrease by age 10, albeit at a high level across time. Peer problems started at a moderate level and followed an increasing linear trajectory, with high levels of problems from 6 to 10 years old. Multi-trajectory groups are displayed in Fig. 1 and parameters estimates in Supplementary File 1.

Risk Factors for Group Membership

Bivariate analyses, including analysis of variance and chi-square tests, showed that low birth weight and area of the family dwelling were not significant factors in discriminating between group membership. All the other variables were significant for group membership (see Table 4). Subsequently, all significant factors at the bivariate level were modelled within the multi-trajectory estimation, using the non-engagers trajectory group (Group 1) as the reference group. At the perinatal level, having spent time in the NICU after childbirth increased the likelihood of membership only for the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group (Group 6). At the child level, male sex increased the likelihood of membership in all groups relative to the non-engagers reference group, with the exception of group 2, the normative group. Conversely, children with higher expressive language skills had a decreased likelihood of membership in groups 4, 5 and 6 as compared to the non-engagers group. At the family level, maternal age (i.e., younger mothers, under 20 years of age at childbirth) was not significant in discriminating between group membership. Single mother status increased children’s likelihood of membership in groups 4, 5 and 6. Low maternal educational level (i.e., leaving school with no qualifications) increased the likelihood of group membership only in the moderate externalising/increasing peer problems group (Group 5). Better than average caregiver mental health and higher maternal-infant attachment scores decreased the likelihood of membership in all groups as compared to the non-engagers group. Finally, having a family-of-origin categorised in the most socially deprived quintile (i.e., quintile five) increased the likelihood of membership in the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group (Group 6). Table 5 shows risk factors associated with group membership at the multivariable level.
Table 4
Bivariate Analysis of Antecedent Risk Factors by Group
 
Group 1
Group 2
Group 3
Group 4
Group 5
Group 6
p
Perinatal Factors
       
   Low birth weight (yes)
5.4%
5.4%
5.0%
6.4%
6.3%
9.3%
0.146
   NICU stay (yes)
9.1%
9.4%
9.5%
12.1%
10.4%
18.3%
 < 0.001
Child Factors
       
   Sex (male)
38.6%
38.8%
57.8%
52.9%
58.0%
69.1%
 < 0.001
   Expressive language
77.5 (16.4)
76.4 (16.8)
74.9 (15.8)
70.6 (19.2)
69.8 (18.0)
63.2 (23.0)
 < 0.001
Family factors
       
   Maternal age (under 20 years of age)
2.1%
3.6%
3.6%
4.4%
6.4%
9.7%
 < 0.001
   Single parent (yes)
7.6%
8.9%
10.9%
13.7%
19.5%
30.9%
 < 0.001
   Maternal education (no qualifications)
2.8%
4.6%
4.8%
6.7%
9.7%
14.1%
 < 0.001
   Caregiver mental health
-0.32 (0.6)
-0.13 (0.7)
-0.17 (0.7)
0.06 (0.9)
0.20 (1.0)
0.56 (1.2)
 < 0.001
   Maternal-infant attachment
24.8 (2.3)
24.3 (2.5)
24.2 (2.3)
24.1 (2.6)
23.9 (2.5)
23.5 (2.9)
 < 0.001
   Area (rural)
21.8%
21.8%
22.2%
18.9%
20.4%
15.3%
0.132
   Social deprivation (most deprived)
11.2%
15.2%
14.2%
21.4%
23.7%
32.6%
 < 0.001
Percentages of prevalence displayed for chi-square analysis; means and (standard deviations) presented in analysis of variance. Group 1 ‘non-engagers’ (n = 636), Group 2 ‘normative’ (n = 454), Group 3 ‘decreasing externalising/low peer problems’ (n = 949), Group 4 ‘low externalising/moderate peer problems’ (n = 455), Group 5 ‘moderate externalising/increasing peer problems’ (n = 523), and Group 6 ‘multimorbid moderate-high chronic’ (n = 302)
Table 5
Multivariable Analysis of Risk Factors by Group Membership
 
B
SE
p
OR
NICU stay (yes)
    
   Group 2
0.26
0.25
0.297
1.30
   Group 3
0.17
0.25
0.504
1.19
   Group 4
0.28
0.25
0.255
1.32
   Group 5
0.12
0.24
0.634
1.13
   Group 6
0.63
0.27
0.018
1.90
Child sex (male)
    
   Group 2
-0.18
0.16
0.254
0.84
   Group 3
0.87
0.15
 < 0.001
2.39
   Group 4
0.49
0.16
0.002
1.63
   Group 5
0.80
0.14
 < 0.001
2.23
   Group 6
1.12
0.19
 < 0.001
3.10
Child expressive language
    
   Group 2
-0.01
0.00
0.458
0.99
   Group 3
-0.01
0.00
0.096
0.99
   Group 4
-0.02
0.00
 < 0.001
0.98
   Group 5
-0.02
0.00
 < 0.001
0.98
   Group 6
-0.04
0.00
 < 0.001
0.96
Maternal age (under 20 years of age)
    
   Group 2
0.46
0.56
0.414
1.58
   Group 3
0.57
0.49
0.240
1.78
   Group 4
0.42
0.50
0.401
1.52
   Group 5
0.56
0.45
0.205
1.75
   Group 6
0.91
0.47
0.051
2.48
Single parent (yes)
    
   Group 2
-0.12
0.32
0.715
0.89
   Group 3
0.25
0.27
0.359
1.28
   Group 4
0.59
0.26
0.024
1.80
   Group 5
0.76
0.24
0.002
2.14
   Group 6
1.26
0.27
 < 0.001
3.53
Maternal education (no qualifications)
    
   Group 2
0.21
0.43
0.623
1.23
   Group 3
0.43
0.39
0.272
1.54
   Group 4
0.43
0.39
0.263
1.54
   Group 5
0.81
0.34
0.018
2.25
   Group 6
0.67
0.38
0.083
1.95
Caregiver mental health
    
   Group 2
-0.63
0.19
0.001
0.53
   Group 3
-0.41
0.19
0.026
0.66
   Group 4
-1.18
0.17
 < 0.001
0.31
   Group 5
-1.37
0.16
 < 0.001
0.25
   Group 6
-1.70
0.20
 < 0.001
0.18
Maternal-infant attachment
    
   Group 2
-0.10
0.03
0.001
0.90
   Group 3
-0.11
0.03
 < 0.001
0.90
   Group 4
-0.12
0.03
 < 0.001
0.89
   Group 5
-0.16
0.03
 < 0.001
0.85
   Group 6
-0.25
0.04
 < 0.001
0.78
Social deprivation (most deprived)
    
   Group 2
-0.00
0.25
0.995
1.00
   Group 3
-0.07
0.24
0.772
0.93
   Group 4
0.41
0.22
0.059
1.51
   Group 5
0.35
0.20
0.085
1.42
   Group 6
0.69
0.23
0.003
1.99
The comparison group was Group 1 ‘non-engagers’ (n = 636). Group 2 ‘normative’ (n = 454), Group 3 ‘decreasing externalising/low peer problems’ (n = 949), Group 4 ‘low externalising/moderate peer problems’ (n = 455), Group 5 ‘moderate externalising/increasing peer problems’ (n = 523), and Group 6 ‘multimorbid moderate-high chronic’ (n = 302). Table includes b coefficient, standard error, significant value, and odds ratio

Discussion

This study contributes new insights to the literature on externalising behaviours and peer problems, having examined concomitant trajectories of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study using a group-based multi-trajectory approach to model these three behaviours simultaneously across childhood and inspect early risk and protective factors associated with group membership. Using the GUS cohort, a nationally representative cohort from Scotland, results showed that a six-group model best fit the data. All six groups evidenced distinctive levels and progression of problems over time. Change across the developmental period examined mainly reflected decreasing and stable trajectories, although increasing trajectories were also evident. In the period of development under investigation, our results suggest multimorbidity between externalising behaviours and peer problems in the more elevated groups over time, with one exception. For group 4, we observed that despite decreasing externalising problems across time, the initially moderate levels of peer problems remained stable. We identified two common protective factors across all groups (i.e., caregiver mental health and maternal-infant attachment), while risk factors identified were specific to group membership. In line with the cumulative risk model, a greater number of risk factors were identified for the more severe problem groups (Group 5 and 6).

Group-Based Multi-Trajectories

The study’s first aim was to better understand subgroup prevalence and patterns of change within trajectories of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems across childhood. Accordingly, a four-group model was hypothesised to best fit the data, including a group with low problems, a normative group with low-moderate decreasing externalising problems and low peer problems, a high-decreasing group, and a high stable group. Findings partially supported the study hypotheses of group selection. Six groups emerged instead of four, with three groups matching our hypotheses (i.e., Groups 1, 2, and 6). The presence of the additional groups, ‘decreasing externalising/low peer problems’ (Group 3), ‘low externalising/moderate peer problems’ (Group 4), and ‘moderate externalising/increasing peer problems’ (Group 5) demonstrated greater developmental heterogeneity than anticipated. Group 3 displayed similar patterns to the first two groups’ conduct and peer problems. However, levels of hyperactivity/inattention in this group, while decreasing, were consistently more elevated across time compared to trajectories for Groups 1 and 2. Group 4 showed similar decreasing trajectories in the externalising problems domains to group 3, whereas peer problems were moderate but remained stable over time. This may reflect the influence of other factors associated with the maintenance of peer problems over time not included in this study, such as emotional problems (Forrest et al., 2020). Moreover, Girard (2021) reported two similar groups to Group 3 and 4 in the current study with respect to the level and progression of both conduct and peer problems over time, albeit with differing progressions of hyperactivity/inattention. In the current study, hyperactivity/inattention decreased over time in both groups whereas they remained stable across time in the former study. These findings suggest that externalising difficulties and peer problems are not occurring in isolation during childhood, and would support previous work demonstrating that behavioural and peer problems feed into the other across time (Hodges & Perry, 1999; Reijntjes et al., 2011; Schwartz et al., 2001).
The presence of multimorbidity in this study has practical and theoretical implications. In a practical sense, children exhibiting multimorbid problems (e.g., groups 5 and 6, combined estimated 28.8%) have been described at a greater risk for later maladaptive outcomes (Newman et al., 1998). Hence, children in the sampled population displaying elevated conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention and increasing levels of peer problems may develop problematic outcomes in the future, which can potentially carry a higher disease burden and economic costs to societies (Pelham et al., 2007). Therefore, longitudinal identification of multimorbid groups is important for targeting children that may need additional support. From a theoretical perspective, the presence of multimorbidity challenges the classic classification systems of psychopathology, which are characterised by discrete and exclusive problems categories. Thus, multimorbidity findings from this study add evidence for further elaboration of classification systems of behavioural problems (e.g., the hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology) and developmental psychopathology theories.
As anticipated, this study identified a non-engager (Group 1), normative (Group 2) and high-chronic (multimorbid moderate-high chronic group, Group 6) group. The non-engager and the normative group, a combined estimated 38.2% of children in the sample, exhibited low or declining symptoms in conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems. These two low risk groups are in line with previous findings in the developmental literature (e.g., Fanti & Henrich, 2010; Girard, 2021; Patalay et al., 2017), suggesting that behavioural problems in preschool children may be normative, particularly as children enter into the classroom setting and are learning to navigate social relationships with peers. For most children, these early difficulties are expected to decline as higher-order skills are better developed, resulting in low-risk trajectories of externalising behaviours and peer problems after entry into formal schooling. Group 3 also followed a low-risk trajectory. Combined, groups 1–3 included 56.4% of children that presented within a normal range of problems in this sample. Finally, as expected, we identified a smaller group of children following a high-risk multimorbid trajectory across all externalising behaviours and peer problems (i.e., the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group), which included an estimated 8.6% of children. Previous developmental studies examining individual and joint trajectories of externalising behaviours and peer problems have also identified smaller groups of children at high-risk for elevated levels of chronically stable and/or increasing problems, however identified groups in these studies have typically consisted of a smaller proportion of the sampled population (i.e., between 3–6%; Shaw et al., 2005; Barker et al., 2008; Girard, 2021; Girard et al., 2019; Nagin & Tremblay, 1999; Patalay et al., 2017). As compared to these other studies, a higher proportion of children in the GUS cohort were identified as belonging to the high-risk group with increasing-chronic problems across multiple domains in childhood. According to previous research in Scotland using the GUS cohort (Marryat et al., 2015; Parkes et at., 2016) and a review of the Glasgow effect (Cowley et al., 2016), the higher proportion of children belonging to group 6 may reflect the cumulative impact of multiple life stressors on children’s wellbeing, including deprivation factors (i.e., socioeconomic inequalities, family distress), urban effects (i.e., remote location with lack of social connectedness among neighbours, urban stress), and cultural stressors (i.e., social attitudes).

Risk and Protective Factors for Group Membership

The second aim of this study was to identify early risk and protective factors for group membership at multiple ecological levels, defining protective factors as influences that change a child’s reaction to environmental hazards that increase the risk of developing problematic outcomes (Rutter, 1985). Two protective factors at the family level were associated with membership in all groups relative to the non-engager group, which included primary caregiver mental health and maternal-infant attachment. Children with mothers reporting better than average mental health were less likely to belong to all groups compared to the non-engagers group, with odds ratio indicating a linear trend across higher problems groups. For example, children were 47%, 34%, 69%, 75% and 82% less likely to belong to groups 2, 3, 4,5 and 6, respectively. These findings are consistent with previous literature indicating that maternal mental health is an important factor in the development of children’s externalising behaviours and peer problems over time (Maruyama et al., 2019). Potential intergenerational transmission of risk mechanisms have been described, including genetic vulnerability, adverse socioeconomic factors, and parenting practices (Elgar et al., 2004). Moreover, maternal-infant attachment was also a protective factor across groups, with an increasing linear trend for groups display higher problems. For each point increase in attachment quality, children were 10%, 10%, 11%, 15% and 22% less likely to belong to groups 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6, respectively. These findings were in line with our expectations given the literature suggesting that caregiver mental health and attachment quality are associated factors that affect mother-infant interactions, and which impact upon children’s later behavioural outcomes (Kim-Cohen et al., 2005). Findings suggest that both protective factors are suitable targets for early prevention efforts, which can be especially beneficial for groups displaying greater problems (i.e., moderate externalising/increasing peer problems and multimorbid moderate-high chronic groups).
Three factors were common for elevated groups and included child sex, receptive language skills, and belonging to single parent households. In line with the literature (e.g., Girard, 2021; Girard et al., 2019; Teymoori et al., 2018), boys were more likely to belong to elevated groups with the largest odds (i.e., 3.1 higher odds) of belonging to the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group. It has been suggested that social determinants for males and females, such as parental and environmental socialisation, impact gender roles behaviours and, in turn, sex differences in childhood problems (Tremblay & Côté, 2019). Consequently, boys have been associated with greater conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention (Loeber & Keenan, 1994). Genetic influences have also been acknowledged as important for sex differences. For example, evidence from twin studies exhibited a greater genetic influence on hyperactivity/inattention problems in boys but not girls, with girls needing greater liability to manifest symptoms (Rhee & Waldman, 2004; Rhee et al., 1999). Moreover, the gender-environment interaction has also been inspected to explain sex differences in externalising problems, with evidence showing that the social context plays an important role in the gender-specific phenotypic expression of genes (Dmitrieva et al., 2011). Therefore, our study findings add that when conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems occur together, boys are more likely to belong to groups with greater problems than the non-engagers and normative groups. This result may reflect that boys are more vulnerable to multimorbid childhood problems, as similarly stated in studies modelling joint externalising problems and internalising problems (Fanti & Henrich, 2010; Girard, 2021; Murray et al., 2021). However, further research inspecting the role of social and genetic determinants is needed to better comprehend aetiology and sex differences in multimorbid problems.
In addition, better expressive language abilities at age three were identified as a protective factor for groups 4, 5 and 6. In line with previous research examining associations between children’s early language skills and externalising problems (e.g., Girard et al., 2014, 2016), our results highlight the importance of children’s early language ability to guard against subsequent externalising behaviours and peer problems. When children are better able to express themselves in social situations, the likelihood of using alternative forms of communication such as aggressive or acting out behaviours should decrease. This may consequently help to further reduce the likelihood of peer problems. Furthermore, children with single parents at 10 months old had greater odds (i.e., 1.8, 2.14, and 3.54 higher odds) of belonging to groups 4, 5 and 6, respectively. This finding may reflect the importance of early family support for mothers (Balaji et al., 2007) and its impact on later children’s outcomes. Single mothers may experience greater stress levels for facing parenthood, impacting mental health and mother-infant interactions, and subsequent offspring behavioural problems. However, further work inspecting this factor is necessary before asserting conclusions.
Finally, three factors were specific for group membership in groups 5 and 6. First, children whose mothers had no educational qualifications had an increased odds more than two-fold for group membership in group 5. Previous studies have reported the importance of maternal education in children’s behavioural outcomes independent of their socioeconomic status (Kalff et al., 2001). This association may be explained via shaping parenting practices related to having more knowledge and experience in the educational system. Surprisingly, maternal education was not similarly a risk factor for the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group (group 6), warranting further investigation. Two factors were significantly associated with increased likelihood of membership in group 6: NICU admission and social deprivation. Children who spent time in the NICU after birth had an increased odds of 1.9 for membership in the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group. Potential mechanisms suggested for the association between having stayed in the NICU and later externalising problems have included the stressful environment of the NICU, early experiences to invasive interventions, and the limited early interactions with caregivers, which may consequently reduce early stimulation and impact upon later social competencies (Linsell et al., 2019). Regarding social deprivation, children from families belonging to the most deprived quintile had two-fold odds of membership in the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group. Potential pathways to explain this association include the family stress model, which suggests that socioeconomic deprivation impacts parental mental health through economic stress, affecting parent–child interactions and later children’s outcomes (Elder, 1977).
Overall, the multimorbid moderate-high chronic group (Group 6) had more associated risk factors for group membership, followed by group 5 with some overlaps. This finding was expected since, according to the cumulative model (Atzaba-Poria et al., 2004; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000), groups with higher problems (i.e., groups 5 and 6) would have more associated risk factors than children within the no-engagers or normative groups. For these two groups, being a boy, staying in the NICU, displaying less developed language skills, having a single parent with no educational qualifications and with socioeconomic deprivation, cumulatively increases the risk for greater and more stable externalising problems and peer problems. In contrast, children with mothers reporting better than average mental health and attachment quality are less likely to develop these difficulties across childhood. All these risks and protective factors are already evident early in infancy and toddlerhood, suggesting clear implications for public health via social determinants of mental health and informing targets for prevention. For example, selective prevention strategies suggested by the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) included the following risk factors for externalising problems: low socioeconomic status, low school achievement, child abuse or parental conflict, separated parents, parental mental health or substance abuse, and parental contact with the criminal justice system (NICE, 2017). However, important antecedent factors identified in this study could complement the ones suggested by NICE, including postnatal health antecedents (i.e., NICU stay), child sex (i.e., boys), and children with lower expressive language skills. Regarding early intervention strategies, NICE guidelines recommend interventions based on social learning theory for conduct problems in children aged three to 11 years old (NICE, 2017) and parenting training programmes for hyperactivity/inattention difficulties in children aged five years or younger (NICE, 2018). Considering the importance of attachment quality and parents’ mental health as protective factors within the context of the current findings, we also recommend exploring hybrid parenting interventions based on cognitive and attachment theory such as Mellow Parenting (Levi et al., 2019) or social learning theory and attachment theory like the Incredible Years (Webster-Stratton, 2001). Furthermore, in light of the multimorbidity between externalising behaviours and peer problems in the elevated symptoms groups, prevention and intervention efforts should also evaluate and incorporate strategies for addressing negative peer relationships that may precede or emerge alongside or as a consequence of externalising problems.

Strengths and Limitations

This study has several strengths, including being the first one using a group-based multi-trajectory approach to model conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention and peer problems simultaneously across childhood, whilst using a large and representative population-based cohort from Scotland. In addition, we inspected several variables at multiple ecological levels as predictors for group membership, while using a multi-informant approach. Nonetheless, our findings are subject to some limitations to consider. First, the parent-report version of the SDQ was utilised to assess children’s outcomes, relying only on maternal reports. The combination of both maternal and teacher-reported SDQ would have been optimal considering that peer problems are generally detected at school. Nevertheless, maternal reports are frequently used in developmental research, and the parent-report of the SDQ has robust validity (Stone et al., 2010). Second, even when psychometric properties of the SDQ have been well validated (Goodman, 2001; Stone et al., 2010), for this study, Cronbach’s alpha was lower than the desirable threshold of ≥ 0.70 (Nunnally, 1978) in the conduct and peer problems scales. Moreover, the Condon Maternal Postnatal Attachment Scale also presented a Cronbach’s alpha below this threshold. A potential effect of this is the underestimation of the conduct and peer problems trajectories and their associated predictors – including maternal-infant attachment – because attenuation is more likely due to the diminution of maximal observable associations (Schmitt, 1996). Third, in order to properly apply quadratic growth, we only included children with at least three assessments in the period under investigation, reducing the final sample size used in this study. Attrition between the GUS birth cohort and the included sample exhibited several demographic disparities that may impact population representation. Comparison between samples revealed an underrepresentation of children with postnatal medical antecedents, younger mothers with lower educational levels, and socioeconomically deprived families, among other variables. Therefore, considering the underrepresentation of families facing more risk factors at the perinatal, child and family levels, it is possible that our findings may be specific to the subsample used in this study, requiring future replication to generalise results. Fourth, even though the inspection of risk and protective factors at the perinatal, child, and family levels provided further insights about each trajectory groups antecedent factors that were associated with group-membership, there are still ambiguities about the causal aetiology of co- and multi-comorbid trajectories of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems. Consequently, future research inspecting underlying causal mechanisms is needed to better understand the aetiology and developmental pathways of these problems. Finally, the GUS birth cohort data was only available until children were 10 years old. Thus, it was not possible to examine the progression of these developmental trajectories into adolescence, which is an important developmental period when changes in externalising behaviours and peer problems may become apparent.

Conclusions

Using a person-centred approach, this study reported a group-based multi-trajectory modelling analysis of conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems using a large population cohort of Scottish families (GUS). Findings showed that six groups best capture the distinct features in the data. Identified groups included children within a normal range of problems (56.4%), children with moderate externalising problems or/and peer problems (35%) and a group with high increasing/stable difficulties over time (8.6%). The more elevated groups suggest multimorbidity between externalising behaviours and peer problems. In addition, risk factors for group membership were identified, suggesting early targets for prevention and intervention programmes for children with the most elevated conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems. For example, at the child level, prevention strategies should target families with boys who stayed in NICU after childbirth and have lower language skills. At the family level, families with single parents, with poorer maternal mental health and lower levels of mother-infant attachment levels, and who are in the most deprived quintile should also be targeted by prevention strategies. Furthermore, early parenting intervention programmes (e.g., Mellow Parenting, Incredible Years) may increase protective factors such as attachment quality and parents’ mental health, helping to counterbalance the effects of early risk factors. Directions for future research to build upon the current work should include a focus on understanding casual mechanisms, particularly related to the higher risk groups, in an effort to better inform prevention efforts’.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all families who take time to participate in the Growing Up in Scotland survey.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference Alavi, N., Roberts, N., & DeGrace, E. (2017). Comparison of parental socio-demographic factors in children and adolescents presenting with internalizing and externalizing disorders. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 29(2). Alavi, N., Roberts, N., & DeGrace, E. (2017). Comparison of parental socio-demographic factors in children and adolescents presenting with internalizing and externalizing disorders. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 29(2).
go back to reference Averdijk, Malti, T., Eisner, M., Ribeaud, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2016). A Vicious Cycle of Peer Victimization? Problem Behavior Mediates Stability in Peer Victimization Over Time. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology, 2(2), 162–181 Averdijk, Malti, T., Eisner, M., Ribeaud, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2016). A Vicious Cycle of Peer Victimization? Problem Behavior Mediates Stability in Peer Victimization Over Time. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology, 2(2), 162–181
go back to reference Anderson, S., Bradshaw, P., Cunningham-Burley, S., Hayes, F., Jamieson, L., MacGregor, A., … Wasoff, F. (2007). Growing Up in Scotland: A Study Following the Lives of Scotland’s Children. Scottish Government. Anderson, S., Bradshaw, P., Cunningham-Burley, S., Hayes, F., Jamieson, L., MacGregor, A., … Wasoff, F. (2007). Growing Up in Scotland: A Study Following the Lives of Scotland’s Children. Scottish Government.
go back to reference Anderson, P., Doyle, L. W., & Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (2003). Neurobehavioral Outcomes of School-age Children Born Extremely Low Birth Weight or Very Preterm in the 1990s. JAMA, 289(24), 3264-3272. Anderson, P., Doyle, L. W., & Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (2003). Neurobehavioral Outcomes of School-age Children Born Extremely Low Birth Weight or Very Preterm in the 1990s. JAMA, 289(24), 3264-3272.
go back to reference Andrade, & Tannock, R. (2013). Sustained Impact of Inattention and Hyperactivity-Impulsivity on Peer Problems: Mediating Roles of Prosocial Skills and Conduct Problems in a Community Sample of Children. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 45(3), 318–328. Andrade, & Tannock, R. (2013). Sustained Impact of Inattention and Hyperactivity-Impulsivity on Peer Problems: Mediating Roles of Prosocial Skills and Conduct Problems in a Community Sample of Children. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 45(3), 318–328.
go back to reference Atzaba-Poria, N., Pike, A., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2004). Do risk factors for problem behaviour act in a cumulative manner? An examination of ethnic minority and majority children through an ecological perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 707–718.CrossRef Atzaba-Poria, N., Pike, A., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2004). Do risk factors for problem behaviour act in a cumulative manner? An examination of ethnic minority and majority children through an ecological perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 707–718.CrossRef
go back to reference Balaji, A. B., Claussen, A. H., Smith, D. C., Visser, S. N., Morales, M. J., & Perou, R. (2007). Social support networks and maternal mental health and well-being. Journal of Women’s Health, 16(10), 1386–1396.CrossRef Balaji, A. B., Claussen, A. H., Smith, D. C., Visser, S. N., Morales, M. J., & Perou, R. (2007). Social support networks and maternal mental health and well-being. Journal of Women’s Health, 16(10), 1386–1396.CrossRef
go back to reference Barican, J. L., Yung, D., Schwartz, C., Zheng, Y., Georgiades, K., & Waddell, C. (2022). Prevalence of childhood mental disorders in high-income countries: A systematic review and meta-analysis to inform policymaking. Evidence-Based Mental Health, 25(1), 36–44.CrossRef Barican, J. L., Yung, D., Schwartz, C., Zheng, Y., Georgiades, K., & Waddell, C. (2022). Prevalence of childhood mental disorders in high-income countries: A systematic review and meta-analysis to inform policymaking. Evidence-Based Mental Health, 25(1), 36–44.CrossRef
go back to reference Barker, E. D., Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Fontaine, N., Arseneault, L., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Predictive validity and early predictors of peer-victimization trajectories in preschool. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(10), 1185–1192.CrossRef Barker, E. D., Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Fontaine, N., Arseneault, L., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Predictive validity and early predictors of peer-victimization trajectories in preschool. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(10), 1185–1192.CrossRef
go back to reference Bevilacqua, L., Hale, D., Barker, E. D., & Viner, R. (2018). Conduct problems trajectories and psychosocial outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(10), 1239–1260.CrossRef Bevilacqua, L., Hale, D., Barker, E. D., & Viner, R. (2018). Conduct problems trajectories and psychosocial outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(10), 1239–1260.CrossRef
go back to reference Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. Six theories of child development: Revised formulations and current issues (pp. 187–249). Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. Six theories of child development: Revised formulations and current issues (pp. 187–249). Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
go back to reference Boivin, M., Petitclerc, A., Feng, B., & Barker, E. D. (2010). The developmental trajectories of peer victimization in middle to late childhood and the changing nature of their behavioral correlates. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982-), 231–260. Boivin, M., Petitclerc, A., Feng, B., & Barker, E. D. (2010). The developmental trajectories of peer victimization in middle to late childhood and the changing nature of their behavioral correlates. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982-), 231–260.
go back to reference Chang, Z., Lichtenstein, P., D’Onofrio, B. M., Almqvist, C., Kuja-Halkola, R., Sjölander, A., & Larsson, H. (2014). Maternal age at childbirth and risk for ADHD in offspring: A population-based cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(6), 1815–1824.CrossRef Chang, Z., Lichtenstein, P., D’Onofrio, B. M., Almqvist, C., Kuja-Halkola, R., Sjölander, A., & Larsson, H. (2014). Maternal age at childbirth and risk for ADHD in offspring: A population-based cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(6), 1815–1824.CrossRef
go back to reference Chen, D., Drabick, D. A., & Burgers, D. E. (2015). A developmental perspective on peer rejection, deviant peer affiliation, and conduct problems among youth. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 46(6), 823–838.CrossRef Chen, D., Drabick, D. A., & Burgers, D. E. (2015). A developmental perspective on peer rejection, deviant peer affiliation, and conduct problems among youth. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 46(6), 823–838.CrossRef
go back to reference Cole, P. M., Tan, P. Z., Hall, S. E., Zhang, Y., Crnic, K. A., Blair, C. B., & Li, R. (2011). Developmental Changes in Anger Expression and Attention Focus: Learning to Wait. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1078–1089.CrossRef Cole, P. M., Tan, P. Z., Hall, S. E., Zhang, Y., Crnic, K. A., Blair, C. B., & Li, R. (2011). Developmental Changes in Anger Expression and Attention Focus: Learning to Wait. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1078–1089.CrossRef
go back to reference Condon, J. T., & Corkindale, C. J. (1998). The assessment of parent-to-infant attachment: Development of a self-report questionnaire instrument. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 16(1), 57–76.CrossRef Condon, J. T., & Corkindale, C. J. (1998). The assessment of parent-to-infant attachment: Development of a self-report questionnaire instrument. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 16(1), 57–76.CrossRef
go back to reference Cook, J. (1988). An investigation of the validity of the British Ability Scales with respect to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised and the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised on a group of Canadian children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 212–216.CrossRef Cook, J. (1988). An investigation of the validity of the British Ability Scales with respect to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised and the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised on a group of Canadian children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 212–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Cowley, J., Kiely, J., & Collins, D. (2016). Unravelling the Glasgow effect: The relationship between accumulative bio-psychosocial stress, stress reactivity and Scotland’s health problems. Preventive Medicine Reports, 4, 370–375.CrossRef Cowley, J., Kiely, J., & Collins, D. (2016). Unravelling the Glasgow effect: The relationship between accumulative bio-psychosocial stress, stress reactivity and Scotland’s health problems. Preventive Medicine Reports, 4, 370–375.CrossRef
go back to reference Dmitrieva, J., Chen, C., Greenberger, E., Ogunseitan, O., & Ding, Y. C. (2011). Gender-specific expression of the DRD4 gene on adolescent delinquency, anger and thrill seeking. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(1), 82–89.CrossRef Dmitrieva, J., Chen, C., Greenberger, E., Ogunseitan, O., & Ding, Y. C. (2011). Gender-specific expression of the DRD4 gene on adolescent delinquency, anger and thrill seeking. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(1), 82–89.CrossRef
go back to reference Elder, G. H. (1977). Children of the Great Depression: Social change in life experience. Elder, Jr. University of Chicago Press. Elder, G. H. (1977). Children of the Great Depression: Social change in life experience. Elder, Jr. University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Elgar, F. J., McGrath, P. J., Waschbusch, D. A., Stewart, S. H., & Curtis, L. J. (2004). Mutual influences on maternal depression and child adjustment problems. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 441–459.CrossRef Elgar, F. J., McGrath, P. J., Waschbusch, D. A., Stewart, S. H., & Curtis, L. J. (2004). Mutual influences on maternal depression and child adjustment problems. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 441–459.CrossRef
go back to reference Elliott, C., Smith, P., & McCullock, K. (1997). British Abilities Scale (BAS II) Technical Manual. Windsor, United Kingdom: Nelson. Elliott, C., Smith, P., & McCullock, K. (1997). British Abilities Scale (BAS II) Technical Manual. Windsor, United Kingdom: Nelson.
go back to reference Fairchild, G., Hawes, D. J., Frick, P. J., Copeland, W. E., Odgers, C. L., Franke, B., & De Brito, S. A. (2019). Conduct disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 5(1), 1–25.CrossRef Fairchild, G., Hawes, D. J., Frick, P. J., Copeland, W. E., Odgers, C. L., Franke, B., & De Brito, S. A. (2019). Conduct disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 5(1), 1–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Fanti, K., & Henrich, C. (2010). Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: Findings from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1159–1175.CrossRef Fanti, K., & Henrich, C. (2010). Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: Findings from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1159–1175.CrossRef
go back to reference Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81(2), 435–456.CrossRef Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81(2), 435–456.CrossRef
go back to reference Flouri, E., Papachristou, E., Midouhas, E., Ploubidis, G. B., Lewis, G., & Joshi, H. (2019). Developmental cascades of internalising symptoms, externalising problems and cognitive ability from early childhood to middle adolescence. European Psychiatry, 57, 61–69.CrossRef Flouri, E., Papachristou, E., Midouhas, E., Ploubidis, G. B., Lewis, G., & Joshi, H. (2019). Developmental cascades of internalising symptoms, externalising problems and cognitive ability from early childhood to middle adolescence. European Psychiatry, 57, 61–69.CrossRef
go back to reference Forrest, C., Gibson, J. L., Halligan, S. L., & St Clair, M. C. (2020). A Cross-Lagged Analysis of Emotion Regulation, Peer Problems, and Emotional Problems in Children With and Without Early Language Difficulties: Evidence From the Millennium Cohort Study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(4), 1227–1239.CrossRef Forrest, C., Gibson, J. L., Halligan, S. L., & St Clair, M. C. (2020). A Cross-Lagged Analysis of Emotion Regulation, Peer Problems, and Emotional Problems in Children With and Without Early Language Difficulties: Evidence From the Millennium Cohort Study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(4), 1227–1239.CrossRef
go back to reference Geoffroy, M.-C.P., Boivin, M. P., Arseneault, L. P., Renaud, J. M. D. M., Perret, L. C. M., Turecki, G. M., Côté, D., & S. M. P. (2018). Childhood trajectories of peer victimization and prediction of mental health outcomes in midadolescence: A longitudinal population-based study. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 190(2), E37–E43.CrossRef Geoffroy, M.-C.P., Boivin, M. P., Arseneault, L. P., Renaud, J. M. D. M., Perret, L. C. M., Turecki, G. M., Côté, D., & S. M. P. (2018). Childhood trajectories of peer victimization and prediction of mental health outcomes in midadolescence: A longitudinal population-based study. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 190(2), E37–E43.CrossRef
go back to reference Girard, L.-C., Pingault, J.-B., Doyle, O., Falissard, B., & Tremblay, R. E. (2016). Developmental associations between conduct problems and expressive language in early childhood: A population-based study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(6), 1033–1043.CrossRef Girard, L.-C., Pingault, J.-B., Doyle, O., Falissard, B., & Tremblay, R. E. (2016). Developmental associations between conduct problems and expressive language in early childhood: A population-based study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(6), 1033–1043.CrossRef
go back to reference Girard, L.-C., Pingault, J.-B., Falissard, B., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., & Tremblay, R. E. (2014). Physical Aggression and Language Ability from 17 to 72 Months: Cross-Lagged Effects in a Population Sample. PLoS ONE, 9(11), e112185.CrossRef Girard, L.-C., Pingault, J.-B., Falissard, B., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., & Tremblay, R. E. (2014). Physical Aggression and Language Ability from 17 to 72 Months: Cross-Lagged Effects in a Population Sample. PLoS ONE, 9(11), e112185.CrossRef
go back to reference Girard, L.-C., Tremblay, R. E., Nagin, D., & Côté, S. M. (2019). Development of aggression subtypes from childhood to adolescence: A group-based multi-trajectory modelling perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(5), 825–838.CrossRef Girard, L.-C., Tremblay, R. E., Nagin, D., & Côté, S. M. (2019). Development of aggression subtypes from childhood to adolescence: A group-based multi-trajectory modelling perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(5), 825–838.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345.CrossRef Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.CrossRef Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.CrossRef
go back to reference Greenley, R. N., Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., & Minich, N. M. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between early adolescent family functioning and youth adjustment: An examination of the moderating role of very low birth weight. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(4), 453–462.CrossRef Greenley, R. N., Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., & Minich, N. M. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between early adolescent family functioning and youth adjustment: An examination of the moderating role of very low birth weight. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(4), 453–462.CrossRef
go back to reference Haas, S. M., Becker, S. P., Epstein, J. N., & Frick, P. J. (2018). Callous-unemotional traits are uniquely associated with poorer peer functioning in school-aged children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(4), 781–793.CrossRef Haas, S. M., Becker, S. P., Epstein, J. N., & Frick, P. J. (2018). Callous-unemotional traits are uniquely associated with poorer peer functioning in school-aged children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(4), 781–793.CrossRef
go back to reference Haltigan, & Vaillancourt, T. (2014). Joint Trajectories of Bullying and Peer Victimization Across Elementary and Middle School and Associations With Symptoms of Psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2426–2436. Haltigan, & Vaillancourt, T. (2014). Joint Trajectories of Bullying and Peer Victimization Across Elementary and Middle School and Associations With Symptoms of Psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2426–2436.
go back to reference Healy, K. L., Sanders, M. R., & Iyer, A. (2015). Parenting practices, children’s peer relationships and being bullied at school. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(1), 127–140.CrossRef Healy, K. L., Sanders, M. R., & Iyer, A. (2015). Parenting practices, children’s peer relationships and being bullied at school. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(1), 127–140.CrossRef
go back to reference Hong, S.-B., Dwyer, D., Kim, J.-W., Park, E.-J., Shin, M.-S., & Kim… Cho, S.-C. (2014). Subthreshold attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is associated with functional impairments across domains: A comprehensive analysis in a large-scale community study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(8), 627–636.CrossRef Hong, S.-B., Dwyer, D., Kim, J.-W., Park, E.-J., Shin, M.-S., & Kim… Cho, S.-C. (2014). Subthreshold attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is associated with functional impairments across domains: A comprehensive analysis in a large-scale community study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(8), 627–636.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Bukowski, W. M., Gold, J. A., & Arnold, L. E. (2005). What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 411–423.CrossRef Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Bukowski, W. M., Gold, J. A., & Arnold, L. E. (2005). What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 411–423.CrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, K. L., Katz, S. J., Lee, S. S., Hammen, C., Brennan, P. A., & Najman, J. M. (2013). The association of ADHD and depression: Mediation by peer problems and parent–child difficulties in two complementary samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 854–867.CrossRef Humphreys, K. L., Katz, S. J., Lee, S. S., Hammen, C., Brennan, P. A., & Najman, J. M. (2013). The association of ADHD and depression: Mediation by peer problems and parent–child difficulties in two complementary samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 854–867.CrossRef
go back to reference Isaacs, J., Hodges, E. V. E., & Salmivalli, C. (2008). Long-term consequences of victimization by peers: A follow-up from adolescence to young adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2(4), 387–397. Isaacs, J., Hodges, E. V. E., & Salmivalli, C. (2008). Long-term consequences of victimization by peers: A follow-up from adolescence to young adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2(4), 387–397.
go back to reference Kalff, A.C., Kroes, M., Vles, J.S., Bosma, H., Feron, F.J., Hendriksen Jolles, J. (2001). Factors affecting the relation between parental education as well as occupation and problem behaviour in Dutch 5- to 6-year-old children. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 36(7), 324–31. Kalff, A.C., Kroes, M., Vles, J.S., Bosma, H., Feron, F.J., Hendriksen Jolles, J. (2001). Factors affecting the relation between parental education as well as occupation and problem behaviour in Dutch 5- to 6-year-old children. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 36(7), 324–31.
go back to reference Kan, K.-J., Dolan, C. V., Nivard, M. G., Middeldorp, C. M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., Willemsen, G. P., & Boomsma, D. I. (2013). Genetic and Environmental Stability in Attention Problems Across the Lifespan: Evidence From the Netherlands Twin Register. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(1), 12–25.CrossRef Kan, K.-J., Dolan, C. V., Nivard, M. G., Middeldorp, C. M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., Willemsen, G. P., & Boomsma, D. I. (2013). Genetic and Environmental Stability in Attention Problems Across the Lifespan: Evidence From the Netherlands Twin Register. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(1), 12–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Keiley, M. K., Lofthouse, N., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). Differential risks of covarying and pure components in mother and teacher reports of externalizing and internalizing behavior across ages 5 to 14. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(3), 267–283.CrossRef Keiley, M. K., Lofthouse, N., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). Differential risks of covarying and pure components in mother and teacher reports of externalizing and internalizing behavior across ages 5 to 14. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(3), 267–283.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Pawlby, S. J., & Caspi, A. (2005). Maternal depression and children’s antisocial behavior: Nature and nurture effects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(2), 173–181.CrossRef Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Pawlby, S. J., & Caspi, A. (2005). Maternal depression and children’s antisocial behavior: Nature and nurture effects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(2), 173–181.CrossRef
go back to reference Kingston, D., Kehler, H., Austin, M.-P., Mughal, M. K., Wajid, A., Vermeyden, L., Benzies, K., Brown, S., Stuart, S., & Giallo, R. (2018). Trajectories of maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy and the first 12 months postpartum and child externalizing and internalizing behavior at three years. PLoS ONE, 13(4), e0195365.CrossRef Kingston, D., Kehler, H., Austin, M.-P., Mughal, M. K., Wajid, A., Vermeyden, L., Benzies, K., Brown, S., Stuart, S., & Giallo, R. (2018). Trajectories of maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy and the first 12 months postpartum and child externalizing and internalizing behavior at three years. PLoS ONE, 13(4), e0195365.CrossRef
go back to reference Laird, R. D., Jordan, K. Y., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2001). Peer rejection in childhood, involvement with antisocial peers in early adolescence, and the development of externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 13(2), 337–354.CrossRef Laird, R. D., Jordan, K. Y., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2001). Peer rejection in childhood, involvement with antisocial peers in early adolescence, and the development of externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 13(2), 337–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Levi, D., Ibrahim, R., Malcolm, R., & MacBeth, A. (2019). Mellow Babies and Mellow Toddlers: Effects on maternal mental health of a group-based parenting intervention for at-risk families with young children. Journal of Affective Disorders, 246, 820–827.CrossRef Levi, D., Ibrahim, R., Malcolm, R., & MacBeth, A. (2019). Mellow Babies and Mellow Toddlers: Effects on maternal mental health of a group-based parenting intervention for at-risk families with young children. Journal of Affective Disorders, 246, 820–827.CrossRef
go back to reference Linsell, L., Johnson, S., Wolke, D., Morris, J., Kurinczuk, J. J., & Marlow, N. (2019). Trajectories of behavior, attention, social and emotional problems from childhood to early adulthood following extremely preterm birth: A prospective cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4), 531–542.CrossRef Linsell, L., Johnson, S., Wolke, D., Morris, J., Kurinczuk, J. J., & Marlow, N. (2019). Trajectories of behavior, attention, social and emotional problems from childhood to early adulthood following extremely preterm birth: A prospective cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4), 531–542.CrossRef
go back to reference Loeber, R., & Keenan, K. (1994). Interaction between conduct disorder and its comorbid conditions: Effects of age and gender. Clinical Psychology Review, 14(6), 497–523.CrossRef Loeber, R., & Keenan, K. (1994). Interaction between conduct disorder and its comorbid conditions: Effects of age and gender. Clinical Psychology Review, 14(6), 497–523.CrossRef
go back to reference Lovibond, S. H., Lovibond, P. F., & Psychology Foundation of Australia. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. Sydney, N.S.W.: Psychology Foundation of Australia. Lovibond, S. H., Lovibond, P. F., & Psychology Foundation of Australia. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. Sydney, N.S.W.: Psychology Foundation of Australia.
go back to reference Magnusson, D. (2003). The Person Approach: Concepts, Measurement Models, and Research Strategy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 101, 3–23.CrossRef Magnusson, D. (2003). The Person Approach: Concepts, Measurement Models, and Research Strategy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 101, 3–23.CrossRef
go back to reference Marryat, L., Thompson, L., Minnis, H., & Wilson, P. (2015). Exploring the social, emotional and behavioural development of preschool children: Is Glasgow different? International Journal for Equity in Health, 14, 3.CrossRef Marryat, L., Thompson, L., Minnis, H., & Wilson, P. (2015). Exploring the social, emotional and behavioural development of preschool children: Is Glasgow different? International Journal for Equity in Health, 14, 3.CrossRef
go back to reference Maruyama, J. M., Pastor-Valero, M., Santos, I. S., Munhoz, T. N., Barros, F. C., & Matijasevich, A. (2019). Impact of maternal depression trajectories on offspring socioemotional competences at age 11: 2004 Pelotas Birth Cohort. Journal of Affective Disorders, 253, 8–17.CrossRef Maruyama, J. M., Pastor-Valero, M., Santos, I. S., Munhoz, T. N., Barros, F. C., & Matijasevich, A. (2019). Impact of maternal depression trajectories on offspring socioemotional competences at age 11: 2004 Pelotas Birth Cohort. Journal of Affective Disorders, 253, 8–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Mathewson, K. J., Chow, C. H., Dobson, K. G., Pope, E. I., Schmidt, L. A., & Van Lieshout, R. J. (2017). Mental health of extremely low birth weight survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 143(4), 347–383.CrossRef Mathewson, K. J., Chow, C. H., Dobson, K. G., Pope, E. I., Schmidt, L. A., & Van Lieshout, R. J. (2017). Mental health of extremely low birth weight survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 143(4), 347–383.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, P., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017). The Role of Family Income Dynamics in Predicting Trajectories of Internalizing and Externalizing Problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(3), 543–556.CrossRef Miller, P., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017). The Role of Family Income Dynamics in Predicting Trajectories of Internalizing and Externalizing Problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(3), 543–556.CrossRef
go back to reference Murray, A. L., Obsuth, I., Speyer, L., Murray, G., & McKenzie, K… Ribeaud, D. (2021). Developmental Cascades from Aggression to Internalizing Problems via Peer and Teacher Relationships from Early to Middle Adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50(4), 663–673.CrossRef Murray, A. L., Obsuth, I., Speyer, L., Murray, G., & McKenzie, K… Ribeaud, D. (2021). Developmental Cascades from Aggression to Internalizing Problems via Peer and Teacher Relationships from Early to Middle Adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50(4), 663–673.CrossRef
go back to reference Musser, E. D., Karalunas, S. L., Dieckmann, N., Peris, T. S., & Nigg, J. T. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder developmental trajectories related to parental expressed emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 182–195.CrossRef Musser, E. D., Karalunas, S. L., Dieckmann, N., Peris, T. S., & Nigg, J. T. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder developmental trajectories related to parental expressed emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 182–195.CrossRef
go back to reference Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-Based Modeling of Development. Harvard University Press.CrossRef Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-Based Modeling of Development. Harvard University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Nagin, D. S., Jones, B., Passos, V. L., & Tremblay, R. E. (2016). Group-based multi-trajectory modeling. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 27(7), 2015–2023.CrossRef Nagin, D. S., Jones, B., Passos, V. L., & Tremblay, R. E. (2016). Group-based multi-trajectory modeling. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 27(7), 2015–2023.CrossRef
go back to reference Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70(5), 1181–1196.CrossRef Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70(5), 1181–1196.CrossRef
go back to reference Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. JAMA, 285(16), 2094–2100.CrossRef Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. JAMA, 285(16), 2094–2100.CrossRef
go back to reference Newman, D. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Comorbid mental disorders: Implications for treatment and sample selection. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(2), 305.CrossRef Newman, D. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Comorbid mental disorders: Implications for treatment and sample selection. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(2), 305.CrossRef
go back to reference Nunnally, J. (1978). An Overview of Psychological Measurement. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Clinical Diagnosis of Mental Disorders: A Handbook. Springer US. Nunnally, J. (1978). An Overview of Psychological Measurement. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Clinical Diagnosis of Mental Disorders: A Handbook. Springer US.
go back to reference Olson, S. L., Choe, D. E., & Sameroff, A. J. (2017). Trajectories of child externalizing problems between ages 3 and 10 years: Contributions of children’s early effortful control, theory of mind, and parenting experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 29(4), 1333–1351.CrossRef Olson, S. L., Choe, D. E., & Sameroff, A. J. (2017). Trajectories of child externalizing problems between ages 3 and 10 years: Contributions of children’s early effortful control, theory of mind, and parenting experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 29(4), 1333–1351.CrossRef
go back to reference Oncioiu, S., Orri, M., Boivin, M., Geoffroy, M.-C., Arseneault, L., Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Navarro, M. C., Galera, C., Tremblay, R. E., & Cote, S. M. (2020). Early Childhood Factors Associated With Peer Victimization Trajectories From 6 to 17 Years of Age. Pediatrics, 145(5), e20192654.CrossRef Oncioiu, S., Orri, M., Boivin, M., Geoffroy, M.-C., Arseneault, L., Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Navarro, M. C., Galera, C., Tremblay, R. E., & Cote, S. M. (2020). Early Childhood Factors Associated With Peer Victimization Trajectories From 6 to 17 Years of Age. Pediatrics, 145(5), e20192654.CrossRef
go back to reference Parkes, A., Sweeting, H., & Wight, D. (2016). What shapes 7-year-olds’ subjective well-being? Prospective analysis of early childhood and parenting using the Growing Up in Scotland study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51, 1417–1428.CrossRef Parkes, A., Sweeting, H., & Wight, D. (2016). What shapes 7-year-olds’ subjective well-being? Prospective analysis of early childhood and parenting using the Growing Up in Scotland study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51, 1417–1428.CrossRef
go back to reference Patalay, P., Moulton, V., Goodman, A., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2017). Cross-Domain Symptom Development Typologies and Their Antecedents: Results From the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(9), 765–776 e762. Patalay, P., Moulton, V., Goodman, A., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2017). Cross-Domain Symptom Development Typologies and Their Antecedents: Results From the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(9), 765–776 e762.
go back to reference Pelham, W. E., Foster, E. M., & Robb, J. A. (2007). The economic impact of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of the Ambulatory Pediatric Association, 7(1 Suppl), 121–131.CrossRef Pelham, W. E., Foster, E. M., & Robb, J. A. (2007). The economic impact of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of the Ambulatory Pediatric Association, 7(1 Suppl), 121–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Pingault, J. B., Côté, S. M., Galéra, C., Genolini, C., Falissard, B, Tremblay, R. E. (2013). Childhood trajectories of inattention, hyperactivity and oppositional behaviors and prediction of substance abuse/dependence: A 15-year longitudinal population-based study. Molecular psychiatry, 18(7), 806-812. Pingault, J. B., Côté, S. M., Galéra, C., Genolini, C., Falissard, B, Tremblay, R. E. (2013). Childhood trajectories of inattention, hyperactivity and oppositional behaviors and prediction of substance abuse/dependence: A 15-year longitudinal population-based study. Molecular psychiatry, 18(7), 806-812.
go back to reference Rhee, & Waldman, I. D. (2004). Etiology of sex differences in the prevalence of ADHD: An examination of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 127B(1), 60–64. Rhee, & Waldman, I. D. (2004). Etiology of sex differences in the prevalence of ADHD: An examination of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 127B(1), 60–64.
go back to reference Rhee, S. H., Waldman, I. D., Hay, D. A., & Levy, F. (1999). Sex differences in genetic and environmental influences on DSM-III-R attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(1), 24–41.CrossRef Rhee, S. H., Waldman, I. D., Hay, D. A., & Levy, F. (1999). Sex differences in genetic and environmental influences on DSM-III-R attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(1), 24–41.CrossRef
go back to reference Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., Boelen, P. A., van der Schoot, M., & Telch, M. J. (2011). Prospective linkages between peer victimization and externalizing problems in children: A meta-analysis. Aggressive Behavior, 37(3), 215–222.CrossRef Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., Boelen, P. A., van der Schoot, M., & Telch, M. J. (2011). Prospective linkages between peer victimization and externalizing problems in children: A meta-analysis. Aggressive Behavior, 37(3), 215–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 98–131.CrossRef Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 98–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Rutter. (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity. Protective factors and resistance to psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 147(6), 598–611. Rutter. (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity. Protective factors and resistance to psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 147(6), 598–611.
go back to reference Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 265–296.CrossRef Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 265–296.CrossRef
go back to reference Sasser, T. R., Kalvin, C. B., & Bierman, K. L. (2016). Developmental trajectories of clinically significant attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms from grade 3 through 12 in a high-risk sample: Predictors and outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 207–219.CrossRef Sasser, T. R., Kalvin, C. B., & Bierman, K. L. (2016). Developmental trajectories of clinically significant attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms from grade 3 through 12 in a high-risk sample: Predictors and outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 207–219.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychological Assessment, 8(4), 350–353.CrossRef Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychological Assessment, 8(4), 350–353.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwartz, D., Proctor, L. J., & Chien, D. H. (2001). The aggressive victim of bullying: Emotional and behavioral dysregulation as a pathway to victimization by peers. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 147–174). Guilford Press. Schwartz, D., Proctor, L. J., & Chien, D. H. (2001). The aggressive victim of bullying: Emotional and behavioral dysregulation as a pathway to victimization by peers. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 147–174). Guilford Press.
go back to reference Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003). Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 189–200.CrossRef Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003). Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 189–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Shaw, D. S., Lacourse, E., & Nagin, D. S. (2005). Developmental trajectories of conduct problems and hyperactivity from ages 2 to 10. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 46(9), 931–942.CrossRef Shaw, D. S., Lacourse, E., & Nagin, D. S. (2005). Developmental trajectories of conduct problems and hyperactivity from ages 2 to 10. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 46(9), 931–942.CrossRef
go back to reference Shum, D., Neulinger, K., O’Callaghan, M., & Mohay, H. (2008). Attentional problems in children born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight at 7–9 years. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(1), 103–112.CrossRef Shum, D., Neulinger, K., O’Callaghan, M., & Mohay, H. (2008). Attentional problems in children born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight at 7–9 years. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(1), 103–112.CrossRef
go back to reference Skranes, J., Vangberg, T. R., Kulseng, S., Indredavik, M. S., Evensen, K. A. I., Martinussen, M., Brubakk, A. M. (2007). Clinical findings and white matter abnormalities seen on diffusion tensor imaging in adolescents with very low birth weight. Brain, 130(3), 654-666. Skranes, J., Vangberg, T. R., Kulseng, S., Indredavik, M. S., Evensen, K. A. I., Martinussen, M., Brubakk, A. M. (2007). Clinical findings and white matter abnormalities seen on diffusion tensor imaging in adolescents with very low birth weight. Brain, 130(3), 654-666.
go back to reference Sosu, E. M., & Schmidt, P. (2017). Tracking Emotional and Behavioral Changes in Childhood: Does the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire Measure the Same Constructs Across Time? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 35(7), 643–656.CrossRef Sosu, E. M., & Schmidt, P. (2017). Tracking Emotional and Behavioral Changes in Childhood: Does the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire Measure the Same Constructs Across Time? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 35(7), 643–656.CrossRef
go back to reference Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C., Vermulst, A. A., & Janssens, J. M. (2010). Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4- to 12-year-olds: A review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13(3), 254–274.CrossRef Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C., Vermulst, A. A., & Janssens, J. M. (2010). Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4- to 12-year-olds: A review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13(3), 254–274.CrossRef
go back to reference Teymoori, A., Côté, S. M., Jones, B. L., Nagin, D. S., Boivin, M., Vitaro., Tremblay, R. E. (2018). Risk Factors Associated With Boys' and Girls' Developmental Trajectories of Physical Aggression From Early Childhood Through Early Adolescence. JAMA Network Open, 1(8), e186364. Teymoori, A., Côté, S. M., Jones, B. L., Nagin, D. S., Boivin, M., Vitaro., Tremblay, R. E. (2018). Risk Factors Associated With Boys' and Girls' Developmental Trajectories of Physical Aggression From Early Childhood Through Early Adolescence. JAMA Network Open, 1(8), e186364.
go back to reference Thorell, Sjöwall, D., Diamatopoulou, S., Rydell, A.-M., & Bohlin, G. (2017). Emotional functioning, ADHD symptoms, and peer problems: A longitudinal investigation of children age 6–9.5 years: Emotional functioning, ADHD symptoms and peer problems. Infant and Child Development, 26(4). Thorell, Sjöwall, D., Diamatopoulou, S., Rydell, A.-M., & Bohlin, G. (2017). Emotional functioning, ADHD symptoms, and peer problems: A longitudinal investigation of children age 6–9.5 years: Emotional functioning, ADHD symptoms and peer problems. Infant and Child Development, 26(4).
go back to reference Tremblay, R. E., & Côté, S. M. (2019). Sex differences in the development of physical aggression: An intergenerational perspective and implications for preventive interventions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 40(1), 129–140.CrossRef Tremblay, R. E., & Côté, S. M. (2019). Sex differences in the development of physical aggression: An intergenerational perspective and implications for preventive interventions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 40(1), 129–140.CrossRef
go back to reference Tremblay, R. (2010). Developmental origins of disruptive behaviour problems: The “original sin” hypothesis, epigenetics and their consequences for prevention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51(4), 341–367.CrossRef Tremblay, R. (2010). Developmental origins of disruptive behaviour problems: The “original sin” hypothesis, epigenetics and their consequences for prevention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51(4), 341–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Vergunst, F., Tremblay, R. E., Galera, C., Nagin, D., Vitaro, F… Cote, S. M. (2019). Multi-rater developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms from 1.5 to 17 years: a population-based birth cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(7), 973–983. Vergunst, F., Tremblay, R. E., Galera, C., Nagin, D., Vitaro, F… Cote, S. M. (2019). Multi-rater developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms from 1.5 to 17 years: a population-based birth cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(7), 973–983.
go back to reference Votruba-Drzal, E. (2006). Economic disparities in middle childhood development: Does income matter? Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1154–1167.CrossRef Votruba-Drzal, E. (2006). Economic disparities in middle childhood development: Does income matter? Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1154–1167.CrossRef
go back to reference Webster-Stratton. (2001). The Incredible Years: Parents, Teachers, and Children Training Series. Residential Treatment for Children & Youth, 18(3), 31–45. Webster-Stratton. (2001). The Incredible Years: Parents, Teachers, and Children Training Series. Residential Treatment for Children & Youth, 18(3), 31–45.
Metagegevens
Titel
Multi-Trajectories of Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity/Inattention, and Peer Problems Across Childhood: Results from the Growing Up in Scotland Birth Cohort
Auteurs
María Francisca Morales
Angus MacBeth
Samantha Swartzman
Lisa-Christine Girard
Publicatiedatum
03-08-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology / Uitgave 1/2023
Print ISSN: 2730-7166
Elektronisch ISSN: 2730-7174
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-022-00956-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2023

Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 1/2023 Naar de uitgave