Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2011

01-07-2011 | Original Article

Pure perceptual-based learning of second-, third-, and fourth-order sequential probabilities

Auteur: Gilbert Remillard

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

There is evidence that sequence learning in the traditional serial reaction time task (SRTT), where target location is the response dimension, and sequence learning in the perceptual SRTT, where target location is not the response dimension, are handled by different mechanisms. The ability of the latter mechanism to learn sequential contingencies that can be learned by the former mechanism was examined. Prior research has established that people can learn second-, third-, and fourth-order probabilities in the traditional SRTT. The present study reveals that people can learn such probabilities in the perceptual SRTT. This suggests that the two mechanisms may have similar architectures. A possible neural basis of the two mechanisms is discussed.
Voetnoten
1
There is a growing body of evidence that the visuospatial attention system and the oculomotor system are tightly linked. For example, a covert shift of attention to a location is accompanied by the preparation of an eye movement to that location (Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2009; Van der Stigchel & Theeuwes, 2007). Also, covert shifts of attention activate brain regions that are nearly identical to those activated by saccadic eye movements (de Haan, Morgan, & Rorden, 2008; Ikkai & Curtis, 2008). And, a covert shift of attention cannot be directed to a location that cannot be accessed physically by an eye movement (Craighero, Nascimben, & Fadiga, 2004; Smith, Ball, Ellison, & Schenk, 2010). Thus sequence learning in the perceptual SRTT might involve the oculomotor system. However, until more is known about the relationship between the visuospatial attention system and the oculomotor system, it will be difficult to establish the latter system's role in sequence learning in the perceptual SRTT.
 
2
There may also be some learning of the sequence of visuospatial locations in the traditional SRTT (Clegg, 2005; Keele, Jennings, Jones, Caulton, & Cohen, 1995; Stadler, 1989). Such learning, however, appears to have minimal influence on performance measures of sequence learning because, as noted earlier, the presence of a distractor that captures visuospatial attention impairs learning of a sequence of visuospatial locations but has no effect on performance measures of sequence learning in the traditional SRTT.
 
3
One might argue that the awareness survey is not optimally sensitive to explicit knowledge of the second-order adjacent probabilities because it does not reinstate all of the cues that were present during the perceptual SRTT (e.g., the underline moving from one target location to the next). The empirical evidence, however, suggests the survey is sensitive. A recognition test where sequences of target locations are presented as sequences of digits is (a) as sensitive as a recognition test that requires participants to respond to the sequences of target locations as in the SRTT (Willingham, Greeley, & Bardone, 1993), (b) capable of detecting explicit sequence knowledge after very limited exposure to the sequence during the SRTT, suggesting that such a recognition test can pick up relatively low levels of awareness (Perruchet, Bigand, & Benoit-Gonin, 1997, Experiment 3), and (c) able to discriminate participants who are instructed to try to explicitly learn a sequence while performing the SRTT from participants who are not given such instructions (Curran, 1997). Also, the awareness survey is able to detect explicit knowledge of subtle first- and second-order adjacent dependencies of 0.40 versus 0.60 after extended training on the SRTT (Remillard & Clark, 2001, Experiment 4).
 
4
The central value of 10.9 ms for the 90% CI is the RT difference between L and H successors (i.e., L − H) averaged across the three epochs. The central value of 7.9 ms/epoch for the 95% CI is the slope of the regression line through the L successor RTs minus the slope of the regression line through the H successor RTs.
 
5
RTs to the last element of each of the four types of runs in Table 4 were also examined. There were 96 (6 × 24) possible five-element sequences of which 24 were EE runs, 24 were UE runs, 24 were EU runs, and 24 were UU runs. For each participant and epoch, the median RTs to the last element of each of the 96 five-element sequences were computed, and then the 72 RTs for EE runs (24 RTs per epoch × 3 epochs) were averaged, as were the 72 RTs for UE runs, the 72 RTs for EU runs, and the 72 RTs for UU runs. Mean RTs on EE, UE, EU, and UU runs were, respectively, 496, 500, 497, and 490 ms. The RT difference between EE and UE runs was significant, p = 0.010, as was the difference between EU and UU runs, p = 0.001. The pattern was similar in Experiments 2 and 3.
 
6
An effect of successor in the absence of a Successor × Session interaction occurs frequently in the traditional SRTT when there are six target locations and each target location has two possible successors (Remillard, 2008a, 2010; Remillard & Clark, 2001). Thus, this pattern of results is not unusual. Indeed, Rowland and Shanks (2006a) note that "Learning tends to be quite rapid in the probabilistic SRT task… so the critical statistical evidence for learning is a main effect of target probability rather than a Probability × Block interaction" (p. 291).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Abrahamse, E. L., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Verwey, W. B. (2009). Sensory information in perceptual-motor sequence learning: Visual and/or tactile stimuli. Experimental Brain Research, 197, 175–183.CrossRef Abrahamse, E. L., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Verwey, W. B. (2009). Sensory information in perceptual-motor sequence learning: Visual and/or tactile stimuli. Experimental Brain Research, 197, 175–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Awh, E., Armstrong, K. M., & Moore, T. (2006). Visual and oculomotor selection: Links, causes and implications for spatial attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 124–130.PubMedCrossRef Awh, E., Armstrong, K. M., & Moore, T. (2006). Visual and oculomotor selection: Links, causes and implications for spatial attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 124–130.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Badgaiyan, R. D., Fischman, A. J., & Alpert, N. M. (2007). Striatal dopamine release in sequential learning. Neuroimage, 38, 549–556.PubMedCrossRef Badgaiyan, R. D., Fischman, A. J., & Alpert, N. M. (2007). Striatal dopamine release in sequential learning. Neuroimage, 38, 549–556.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). When are attention and saccade preparation dissociated? Psychological Science, 20, 1340–1347.PubMedCrossRef Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). When are attention and saccade preparation dissociated? Psychological Science, 20, 1340–1347.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bird, G., Osman, M., Saggerson, A., & Heyes, C. (2005). Sequence learning by action, observation, and action observation. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 371–388.PubMedCrossRef Bird, G., Osman, M., Saggerson, A., & Heyes, C. (2005). Sequence learning by action, observation, and action observation. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 371–388.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bischoff-Grethe, A., Goedert, K. M., Willingham, D. T., & Grafton, S. T. (2004). Neural substrates of response-based sequence learning using fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 127–138.PubMedCrossRef Bischoff-Grethe, A., Goedert, K. M., Willingham, D. T., & Grafton, S. T. (2004). Neural substrates of response-based sequence learning using fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 127–138.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bischoff-Grethe, A., Martin, M., Mao, H., & Berns, G. S. (2001). The context of uncertainty modulates the subcortical response to predictability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 986–993.PubMedCrossRef Bischoff-Grethe, A., Martin, M., Mao, H., & Berns, G. S. (2001). The context of uncertainty modulates the subcortical response to predictability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 986–993.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clegg, B. A. (2005). Stimulus-specific sequence representation in serial reaction time tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1087–1101.PubMed Clegg, B. A. (2005). Stimulus-specific sequence representation in serial reaction time tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1087–1101.PubMed
go back to reference Craighero, L., Nascimben, M., & Fadiga, L. (2004). Eye position affects orienting of visuospatial attention. Current Biology, 14, 331–333.PubMed Craighero, L., Nascimben, M., & Fadiga, L. (2004). Eye position affects orienting of visuospatial attention. Current Biology, 14, 331–333.PubMed
go back to reference Curran, T. (1997). Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: Accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge. Psychological Research, 60, 24–41.PubMedCrossRef Curran, T. (1997). Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: Accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge. Psychological Research, 60, 24–41.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Curran, T., & Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 189–202.CrossRef Curran, T., & Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 189–202.CrossRef
go back to reference Daselaar, S. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Veltman, D. J., Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Jonkers, C. (2003). Similar network activated by young and old adults during the acquisition of a motor sequence. Neurobiology of Aging, 24, 1013–1019.PubMedCrossRef Daselaar, S. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Veltman, D. J., Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Jonkers, C. (2003). Similar network activated by young and old adults during the acquisition of a motor sequence. Neurobiology of Aging, 24, 1013–1019.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference de Haan, B., Morgan, P. S., & Rorden, C. (2008). Covert orienting of attention and overt eye movements activate identical brain regions. Brain Research, 1204, 102–111.PubMedCrossRef de Haan, B., Morgan, P. S., & Rorden, C. (2008). Covert orienting of attention and overt eye movements activate identical brain regions. Brain Research, 1204, 102–111.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deroost, N., Coomans, D., & Soetens, E. (2009). Perceptual load improves the expression but not learning of relevant sequence information. Experimental Psychology, 56, 84–91.PubMed Deroost, N., Coomans, D., & Soetens, E. (2009). Perceptual load improves the expression but not learning of relevant sequence information. Experimental Psychology, 56, 84–91.PubMed
go back to reference Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006a). The role of response selection in sequence learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 449–456.PubMedCrossRef Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006a). The role of response selection in sequence learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 449–456.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006b). Perceptual or motor learning in SRT tasks with complex sequence structures. Psychological Research, 70, 88–102.PubMedCrossRef Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006b). Perceptual or motor learning in SRT tasks with complex sequence structures. Psychological Research, 70, 88–102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006c). Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks. Experimental Psychology, 53, 16–30.PubMed Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006c). Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks. Experimental Psychology, 53, 16–30.PubMed
go back to reference Gagnon, D., O’Driscoll, G. A., Petrides, M., & Pike, G. B. (2002). The effect of spatial and temporal information on saccades and neural activity in oculomotor structures. Brain, 125, 123–139.PubMedCrossRef Gagnon, D., O’Driscoll, G. A., Petrides, M., & Pike, G. B. (2002). The effect of spatial and temporal information on saccades and neural activity in oculomotor structures. Brain, 125, 123–139.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goschke, T., Friederici, A. D., Kotz, S. A., & van Kampen, A. (2001). Procedural learning in Broca’s aphasia: Dissociation between the implicit acquisition of spatio-motor and phoneme sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 370–388.PubMedCrossRef Goschke, T., Friederici, A. D., Kotz, S. A., & van Kampen, A. (2001). Procedural learning in Broca’s aphasia: Dissociation between the implicit acquisition of spatio-motor and phoneme sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 370–388.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. (1995). Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 497–510.CrossRef Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. (1995). Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 497–510.CrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Grafton, S. T., & Ivry, R. (1997). Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding: A PET study. Brain, 120, 123–140.PubMedCrossRef Hazeltine, E., Grafton, S. T., & Ivry, R. (1997). Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding: A PET study. Brain, 120, 123–140.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helmuth, L. L., Mayr, U., & Daum, I. (2000). Sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease: A comparison of spatial-attention and number-response sequences. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1443–1451.PubMedCrossRef Helmuth, L. L., Mayr, U., & Daum, I. (2000). Sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease: A comparison of spatial-attention and number-response sequences. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1443–1451.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ikkai, A., & Curtis, C. E. (2008). Cortical activity time locked to the shift and maintenance of spatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1384–1394.PubMedCrossRef Ikkai, A., & Curtis, C. E. (2008). Cortical activity time locked to the shift and maintenance of spatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1384–1394.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jimenez, L., & Vazquez, G. A. (2008). Implicit sequence learning in a search task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1650–1657.PubMedCrossRef Jimenez, L., & Vazquez, G. A. (2008). Implicit sequence learning in a search task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1650–1657.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30.CrossRef Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelly, S. W., Burton, A. M., Riedel, B., & Lynch, E. (2003). Sequence learning by action and observation: Evidence for separate mechanisms. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 355–372.PubMedCrossRef Kelly, S. W., Burton, A. M., Riedel, B., & Lynch, E. (2003). Sequence learning by action and observation: Evidence for separate mechanisms. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 355–372.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Bizot, E. (1988). Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 24–37.PubMedCrossRef Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Bizot, E. (1988). Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 24–37.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350–364.PubMedCrossRef Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350–364.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McDowall, J., Lustig, A., & Parkin, G. (1995). Indirect learning of event sequences: The effects of divided attention and stimulus continuity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 415–435.PubMed McDowall, J., Lustig, A., & Parkin, G. (1995). Indirect learning of event sequences: The effects of divided attention and stimulus continuity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 415–435.PubMed
go back to reference Middleton, F. A., & Strick, P. L. (2001). A revised neuroanatomy of frontal-subcortical circuits. In D. G. Lichter & J. L. Cummings (Eds.), Frontal-subcortical circuits in psychiatric and neurological disorders (pp. 44–58). New York, NY: Guilford Publications. Middleton, F. A., & Strick, P. L. (2001). A revised neuroanatomy of frontal-subcortical circuits. In D. G. Lichter & J. L. Cummings (Eds.), Frontal-subcortical circuits in psychiatric and neurological disorders (pp. 44–58). New York, NY: Guilford Publications.
go back to reference Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Peigneux, P., Maquet, P., Meulemans, T., Destrebecqz, A., Laureys, S., et al. (2000). Striatum forever, despite sequence learning variability: A random effect analysis of PET data. Human Brain Mapping, 10, 179–194.PubMedCrossRef Peigneux, P., Maquet, P., Meulemans, T., Destrebecqz, A., Laureys, S., et al. (2000). Striatum forever, despite sequence learning variability: A random effect analysis of PET data. Human Brain Mapping, 10, 179–194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perruchet, P., Bigand, E., & Benoit-Gonin, F. (1997). The emergence of explicit knowledge during the early phase of learning in sequential reaction time tasks. Psychological Research, 60, 4–13.CrossRef Perruchet, P., Bigand, E., & Benoit-Gonin, F. (1997). The emergence of explicit knowledge during the early phase of learning in sequential reaction time tasks. Psychological Research, 60, 4–13.CrossRef
go back to reference Price, A., & Shin, J. C. (2009). The impact of Parkinson’s disease on sequence learning: Perceptual pattern learning and executive function. Brain and Cognition, 69, 252–261.PubMedCrossRef Price, A., & Shin, J. C. (2009). The impact of Parkinson’s disease on sequence learning: Perceptual pattern learning and executive function. Brain and Cognition, 69, 252–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rauch, S. L., Whalen, P. J., Savage, C. R., Curran, T., Kendrick, A., et al. (1997). Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 124–132.PubMedCrossRef Rauch, S. L., Whalen, P. J., Savage, C. R., Curran, T., Kendrick, A., et al. (1997). Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 124–132.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 585–594.CrossRef Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 585–594.CrossRef
go back to reference Reiss, J. P., Campbell, D. W., Leslie, W. D., Paulus, M. P., Stroman, P. W., et al. (2005). The role of the striatum in implicit learning: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroReport, 16, 1291–1295.PubMedCrossRef Reiss, J. P., Campbell, D. W., Leslie, W. D., Paulus, M. P., Stroman, P. W., et al. (2005). The role of the striatum in implicit learning: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroReport, 16, 1291–1295.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Remillard, G. (2003). Pure perceptual-based sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 581–597.PubMedCrossRef Remillard, G. (2003). Pure perceptual-based sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 581–597.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Remillard, G. (2008a). Implicit learning of second-, third-, and fourth-order adjacent and nonadjacent sequential dependencies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 400–424.PubMedCrossRef Remillard, G. (2008a). Implicit learning of second-, third-, and fourth-order adjacent and nonadjacent sequential dependencies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 400–424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Remillard, G. (2008b). A program for generating randomized simple and context-sensitive sequences. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 484–492.PubMedCrossRef Remillard, G. (2008b). A program for generating randomized simple and context-sensitive sequences. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 484–492.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Remillard, G. (2009). Pure perceptual-based sequence learning: A role for visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 528–541.PubMedCrossRef Remillard, G. (2009). Pure perceptual-based sequence learning: A role for visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 528–541.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Remillard, G. (2010). Implicit learning of fifth- and sixth-order sequential probabilities. Memory & Cognition (in press). Remillard, G. (2010). Implicit learning of fifth- and sixth-order sequential probabilities. Memory & Cognition (in press).
go back to reference Remillard, G., & Clark, J. M. (2001). Implicit learning of first-, second-, and third-order transition probabilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 483–498.PubMedCrossRef Remillard, G., & Clark, J. M. (2001). Implicit learning of first-, second-, and third-order transition probabilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 483–498.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rowland, L. A., & Shanks, D. R. (2006a). Sequence learning and selection difficulty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 287–299.PubMedCrossRef Rowland, L. A., & Shanks, D. R. (2006a). Sequence learning and selection difficulty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 287–299.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rowland, L. A., & Shanks, D. R. (2006b). Attention modulates the learning of multiple contingencies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 643–648.CrossRef Rowland, L. A., & Shanks, D. R. (2006b). Attention modulates the learning of multiple contingencies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 643–648.CrossRef
go back to reference Russeler, J., Hennighausen, E., & Rosler, F. (2001). Response anticipation processes in the learning of a sensorimotor sequence. Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 95–105.CrossRef Russeler, J., Hennighausen, E., & Rosler, F. (2001). Response anticipation processes in the learning of a sensorimotor sequence. Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 95–105.CrossRef
go back to reference Schumacher, E. H., & Schwarb, H. (2009). Parallel response selection disrupts sequence learning under dual-task conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 270–290.CrossRef Schumacher, E. H., & Schwarb, H. (2009). Parallel response selection disrupts sequence learning under dual-task conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 270–290.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2009). Neural evidence of a role for spatial response selection in the learning of spatial sequences. Brain Research, 1247, 114–125.PubMedCrossRef Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2009). Neural evidence of a role for spatial response selection in the learning of spatial sequences. Brain Research, 1247, 114–125.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Seidler, R. D., Purushotham, A., Kim, S. G., Ugurbil, K., Willingham, D., et al. (2005). Neural correlates of encoding and expression in implicit sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 165, 114–124.CrossRef Seidler, R. D., Purushotham, A., Kim, S. G., Ugurbil, K., Willingham, D., et al. (2005). Neural correlates of encoding and expression in implicit sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 165, 114–124.CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, D. T., Ball, K., Ellison, A., & Schenk, T. (2010). Deficits of reflexive attention induced by abduction of the eye. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1269–1276.PubMedCrossRef Smith, D. T., Ball, K., Ellison, A., & Schenk, T. (2010). Deficits of reflexive attention induced by abduction of the eye. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1269–1276.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Song, S., Howard, J. H., Jr., & Howard, D. V. (2008). Perceptual sequence learning in a serial reaction time task. Experimental Brain Research, 189, 145–158.CrossRef Song, S., Howard, J. H., Jr., & Howard, D. V. (2008). Perceptual sequence learning in a serial reaction time task. Experimental Brain Research, 189, 145–158.CrossRef
go back to reference Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1061–1069.PubMedCrossRef Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1061–1069.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stadler, M. A. (1993). Implicit serial learning: Questions inspired by Hebb (1961). Memory & Cognition, 21, 819–827.CrossRef Stadler, M. A. (1993). Implicit serial learning: Questions inspired by Hebb (1961). Memory & Cognition, 21, 819–827.CrossRef
go back to reference Stadler, M. A. (1995). Role of attention in implicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 674–685.CrossRef Stadler, M. A. (1995). Role of attention in implicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 674–685.CrossRef
go back to reference Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231–270.PubMedCrossRef Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231–270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ullman, M. T. (2006). Is Broca’s area part of a basal ganglia thalamocortical circuit? Cortex, 42, 480–485.PubMedCrossRef Ullman, M. T. (2006). Is Broca’s area part of a basal ganglia thalamocortical circuit? Cortex, 42, 480–485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vakil, E., Shimon, K., Huberman, M., & Osimani, A. (2000). Motor and non-motor sequence learning in patients with basal ganglia lesions: The case of the serial reaction time (SRT). Neuropsychologia, 38, 1–10.PubMedCrossRef Vakil, E., Shimon, K., Huberman, M., & Osimani, A. (2000). Motor and non-motor sequence learning in patients with basal ganglia lesions: The case of the serial reaction time (SRT). Neuropsychologia, 38, 1–10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van der Stigchel, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). The relationship between covert and overt attention in endogenous cuing. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 719–731.CrossRef Van der Stigchel, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). The relationship between covert and overt attention in endogenous cuing. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 719–731.CrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory & Cognition, 27, 561–572.CrossRef Willingham, D. B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory & Cognition, 27, 561–572.CrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Greeley, T., & Bardone, A. M. (1993). Dissociation in a serial response time task using a recognition measure: Comment on Perruchet and Amorim (1992). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1424–1430.CrossRef Willingham, D. B., Greeley, T., & Bardone, A. M. (1993). Dissociation in a serial response time task using a recognition measure: Comment on Perruchet and Amorim (1992). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1424–1430.CrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366–375.CrossRef Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366–375.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Pure perceptual-based learning of second-, third-, and fourth-order sequential probabilities
Auteur
Gilbert Remillard
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0309-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2011

Psychological Research 4/2011 Naar de uitgave