Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2011

01-07-2011 | Original Article

Bottom-up effects modulate saccadic latencies in well-known eye movement paradigm

Auteurs: Saskia van Stockum, Michael R. MacAskill, Tim J. Anderson

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A well-known eye movement paradigm combines saccades (fast eye movements) with a perceptual discrimination task. At a variable time after the onset of a central arrow cue indicating the target direction [the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA)], discrimination symbols appear briefly at saccade target and non-target locations. A previous study revealed an unexpected effect of SOA on saccadic latencies: latencies were longer in trials with longer SOAs. It was suggested that this effect reflects a top-down process as observers may wait for the discrimination symbol to appear before executing saccades. However, symbol onsets may also modulate saccade latencies from the bottom-up. To clarify the origin of the SOA effect on latencies in this paradigm, we used a simplified version of the original task plus two new symbol onset conditions for comparison. The results indicate that the modulation of saccadic latencies was not due to a top-down strategy, but to a combination of two opposing bottom-up effects: the symbol onsets at the target location shortened saccade latencies, while symbol onsets at non-target locations lengthened saccade latencies.
Literatuur
go back to reference Awh, E., Armstrong, K. M., & Moore, T. (2006). Visual and oculomotor selection: Links, causes and implications for spatial attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(3), 124–130.PubMedCrossRef Awh, E., Armstrong, K. M., & Moore, T. (2006). Visual and oculomotor selection: Links, causes and implications for spatial attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(3), 124–130.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Boehnke, S. E., & Munoz, D. P. (2008). On the importance of the transient visual response in the superior colliculus. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(6), 544–551.PubMedCrossRef Boehnke, S. E., & Munoz, D. P. (2008). On the importance of the transient visual response in the superior colliculus. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(6), 544–551.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bompas, A., & Sumner, P. (2009a). Oculomotor distraction by signals invisible to the retinotectal and magnocellular pathways. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(4), 2387–2395.PubMedCrossRef Bompas, A., & Sumner, P. (2009a). Oculomotor distraction by signals invisible to the retinotectal and magnocellular pathways. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(4), 2387–2395.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bompas, A., & Sumner, P. (2009b). Temporal dynamics of saccadic distraction. Journal of Vision, 9(9), 11–14.CrossRef Bompas, A., & Sumner, P. (2009b). Temporal dynamics of saccadic distraction. Journal of Vision, 9(9), 11–14.CrossRef
go back to reference Buonocore, A., & McIntosh, R. D. (2008). Saccadic inhibition underlies the remote distractor effect. Experimental Brain Research, 191(1), 117–122.CrossRef Buonocore, A., & McIntosh, R. D. (2008). Saccadic inhibition underlies the remote distractor effect. Experimental Brain Research, 191(1), 117–122.CrossRef
go back to reference Deubel, H. (2008). The time course of presaccadic attention shifts. Psychological Research, 72(6), 630–640.PubMedCrossRef Deubel, H. (2008). The time course of presaccadic attention shifts. Psychological Research, 72(6), 630–640.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36(12), 1827–1837.PubMedCrossRef Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36(12), 1827–1837.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dorris, M. C., Olivier, E., & Munoz, D. P. (2007). Competitive integration of visual and preparatory signals in the superior colliculus during saccadic programming. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(19), 5053–5062.PubMedCrossRef Dorris, M. C., Olivier, E., & Munoz, D. P. (2007). Competitive integration of visual and preparatory signals in the superior colliculus during saccadic programming. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(19), 5053–5062.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Edelman, J. A., & Xu, K. Z. (2009). Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101(3), 1222–1234.PubMedCrossRef Edelman, J. A., & Xu, K. Z. (2009). Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101(3), 1222–1234.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Everling, S., Pare, M., Dorris, M. C., & Munoz, D. P. (1998). Comparison of the discharge characteristics of brain stem omnipause neurons and superior colliculus fixation neurons in monkey: Implications for control of fixation and saccade behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(2), 511–528.PubMed Everling, S., Pare, M., Dorris, M. C., & Munoz, D. P. (1998). Comparison of the discharge characteristics of brain stem omnipause neurons and superior colliculus fixation neurons in monkey: Implications for control of fixation and saccade behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(2), 511–528.PubMed
go back to reference Fecteau, J. H., Bell, A. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2004). Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92(3), 1728–1737.PubMedCrossRef Fecteau, J. H., Bell, A. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2004). Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92(3), 1728–1737.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fecteau, J. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2006). Salience, relevance, and firing: A priority map for target selection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 382–390.PubMedCrossRef Fecteau, J. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2006). Salience, relevance, and firing: A priority map for target selection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 382–390.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(5), 1039–1054.PubMedCrossRef Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(5), 1039–1054.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hikosaka, O., Takikawa, Y., & Kawagoe, R. (2000). Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. Physiological Reviews, 80(3), 953–978.PubMed Hikosaka, O., Takikawa, Y., & Kawagoe, R. (2000). Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. Physiological Reviews, 80(3), 953–978.PubMed
go back to reference Irwin, D. E., Colcombe, A. M., Kramer, A. F., & Hahn, S. (2000). Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons. Vision Research, 40(10–12), 1443–1458.PubMedCrossRef Irwin, D. E., Colcombe, A. M., Kramer, A. F., & Hahn, S. (2000). Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons. Vision Research, 40(10–12), 1443–1458.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Isoda, M., & Hikosaka, O. (2008). A neural correlate of motivational conflict in the superior colliculus of the macaque. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100(3), 1332–1342.PubMedCrossRef Isoda, M., & Hikosaka, O. (2008). A neural correlate of motivational conflict in the superior colliculus of the macaque. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100(3), 1332–1342.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koelewijn, T., Bronkhorst, A., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). Auditory and visual capture during focused visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1303–1315.PubMedCrossRef Koelewijn, T., Bronkhorst, A., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). Auditory and visual capture during focused visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1303–1315.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35(13), 1897–1916.PubMedCrossRef Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35(13), 1897–1916.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Li, X., Kim, B., & Basso, M. A. (2006). Transient pauses in delay-period activity of superior colliculus neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95(4), 2252–2264.PubMedCrossRef Li, X., Kim, B., & Basso, M. A. (2006). Transient pauses in delay-period activity of superior colliculus neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95(4), 2252–2264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ludwig, C. J., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Stimulus-driven and goal-driven control over visual selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(4), 902–912.PubMedCrossRef Ludwig, C. J., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Stimulus-driven and goal-driven control over visual selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(4), 902–912.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ludwig, C. J., Gilchrist, I. D., & McSorley, E. (2005). The remote distractor effect in saccade programming: Channel interactions and lateral inhibition. Vision Research, 45(9), 1177–1190.PubMedCrossRef Ludwig, C. J., Gilchrist, I. D., & McSorley, E. (2005). The remote distractor effect in saccade programming: Channel interactions and lateral inhibition. Vision Research, 45(9), 1177–1190.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McPeek, R. M. (2008). Reversal of a distractor effect on saccade target selection after superior colliculus inactivation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99(5), 2694–2702.PubMedCrossRef McPeek, R. M. (2008). Reversal of a distractor effect on saccade target selection after superior colliculus inactivation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99(5), 2694–2702.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Montagnini, A., & Castet, E. (2007). Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. Journal of Vision, 7(14), 1–16.PubMedCrossRef Montagnini, A., & Castet, E. (2007). Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. Journal of Vision, 7(14), 1–16.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mulckhuyse, M., van Zoest, W., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Capture of the eyes by relevant and irrelevant onsets. Experimental Brain Research, 186(2), 225–235.CrossRef Mulckhuyse, M., van Zoest, W., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Capture of the eyes by relevant and irrelevant onsets. Experimental Brain Research, 186(2), 225–235.CrossRef
go back to reference Munoz, D. P., & Istvan, P. J. (1998). Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(3), 1193–1209.PubMed Munoz, D. P., & Istvan, P. J. (1998). Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(3), 1193–1209.PubMed
go back to reference Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy–Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13.PubMedCrossRef Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy–Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Peirce, J. W. (2008). Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in neuroinformatics, 2, 10.PubMedCrossRef Peirce, J. W. (2008). Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in neuroinformatics, 2, 10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reingold, E. M., & Stampe, D. M. (2002). Saccadic inhibition in voluntary and reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 371–388.PubMedCrossRef Reingold, E. M., & Stampe, D. M. (2002). Saccadic inhibition in voluntary and reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 371–388.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umilta, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umilta, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Trappenberg, T. P., Dorris, M. C., Munoz, D. P., & Klein, R. M. (2001). A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2), 256–271.PubMedCrossRef Trappenberg, T. P., Dorris, M. C., Munoz, D. P., & Klein, R. M. (2001). A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2), 256–271.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Walker, R., Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X., & Findlay, J. M. (1997). Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: Evidence for an extended fixation zone. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78(2), 1108–1119.PubMed Walker, R., Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X., & Findlay, J. M. (1997). Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: Evidence for an extended fixation zone. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78(2), 1108–1119.PubMed
go back to reference Wurtz, R. H. (2008). Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability. Vision Research, 48(20), 2070–2089.PubMedCrossRef Wurtz, R. H. (2008). Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability. Vision Research, 48(20), 2070–2089.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Bottom-up effects modulate saccadic latencies in well-known eye movement paradigm
Auteurs
Saskia van Stockum
Michael R. MacAskill
Tim J. Anderson
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0305-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2011

Psychological Research 4/2011 Naar de uitgave