Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2006

01-07-2006 | Original Article

Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting?

Auteurs: Ronald Hübner, Michel D. Druey

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Repetition effects are often helpful in revealing information about mental structures and processes. Usually, positive effects have been observed when the stimuli or responses are repeated. However, in task shift studies it has also been found that response repetitions can produce negative effects if the task shifts. Although several mechanisms have been proposed to account for this interaction between task shifting and response repetition, many details remain open. Therefore, a series of four experiments was conducted to answer two questions. First, are motor responses necessary to produce response-related repetition effects, or is response activation sufficient? Second, does the risk of an accidental re-execution of the last response affect the repetition costs? The results show that response activation alone can produce repetition effects. Furthermore, the risk of accidental response re-execution largely modulates these effects.
Voetnoten
1
Our proportion of single stimulus trials (50%) is rather small compared with other change task studies, where they were usually within a range of 75 to 90% (Logan, 1994). Nevertheless, we used this smaller proportion in order to keep the experiment within reasonable temporal limits.
 
2
Compared with the previous change task studies, we used a somewhat different terminology here. Instead of “signal inhibit trials”, we denote the trials in which participants did not respond to S1 but only responded to S2 “successful change trials.” Furthermore, instead of “signal respond trials” for trials in which participants erroneously responded to S1 and S2, we used “accidental dual task trials” here. The reasons for this adaptation are that (a) the latter terms are only descriptive and do not already imply theoretical connotations that may interfere with the concepts relevant to the purposes of this study, and (b) are more explicit regarding the cases they designate.
 
3
Notice that the number of observations per condition were unequal due to the fact that the participants responded on average only to about 17% of the S1 at the SOA of 50 ms, but to about 53% at the SOA of 350 ms. Consequently, the mean RTs for each participant and condition are based on unequal numbers of trials. However, since all conditions comprised at least eight valid trials, which were considered sufficient to estimate the respective mean response times, we did not apply special statistical procedures.
 
4
We did not analyze the R1 data in accidental dual task trials, because they are considered as erroneous responses here.
 
5
Compared with the previous change task studies, we used somewhat different terminology here. Instead of “signal inhibit trials,” we denote the trials in which participants did not respond to S1 but only responded to S2 “successful change trials.” Furthermore, instead of “signal respond trials” for trials in which participants erroneously responded to S1 and S2, we used “accidental dual task trials” here. The reasons for this adaptation are that (a) the latter terms are only descriptive and do not already imply theoretical connotations that may interfere with the concepts relevant to the purposes of this study, and (b) are more explicit regarding the cases they designate.
 
6
We could speculate that index/middle finger or inner/outer keys could also have served as response categories (Logan & Schulkind, 2000). However, with these categories the mappings between stimulus categories and response categories are not spatially consistent across hands. Since spatial consistency between mappings plays an important role (Duncan, 1979), we assumed that the participants would choose spatial response categories. This assumption was confirmed by our data.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bertelson, P. (1963). S-R relationships and RT to new vs. repeated signals in a send task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 478–484. Bertelson, P. (1963). S-R relationships and RT to new vs. repeated signals in a send task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 478–484.
go back to reference Bertelson, P. (1965). Serial choice reaction-time as a function of response versus signal-and-response repetition. Nature, 206, 217–218. Bertelson, P. (1965). Serial choice reaction-time as a function of response versus signal-and-response repetition. Nature, 206, 217–218.
go back to reference Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron, 39, 713–726. Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron, 39, 713–726.
go back to reference Campbell, K. C., & Proctor, R. W. (1993). Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1345–1362. Campbell, K. C., & Proctor, R. W. (1993). Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1345–1362.
go back to reference Duncan, J. (1979). Divided attention: The whole is more than the sum of its parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 216–228. Duncan, J. (1979). Divided attention: The whole is more than the sum of its parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 216–228.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998a). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384. Hommel, B. (1998a). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998b). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216. Hommel, B. (1998b). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.
go back to reference Hübner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 640–653. Hübner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 640–653.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T. (1999). Response repetition benefits and costs. Acta Psychologica, 103, 295–310. Kleinsorge, T. (1999). Response repetition benefits and costs. Acta Psychologica, 103, 295–310.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., & Gajewski, P. D. (2004). Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 302–306. Kleinsorge, T., & Gajewski, P. D. (2004). Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 302–306.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62, 300–312. Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62, 300–312.
go back to reference Lien, M. C., Schweickert, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 692–712. Lien, M. C., Schweickert, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 692–712.
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1985). On the ability to inhibit simple thoughts and actions. II. Stop-signal studies of repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 675–691. Logan, G. D. (1985). On the ability to inhibit simple thoughts and actions. II. Stop-signal studies of repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 675–691.
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 188–239). San Diego: Academic Press. Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 188–239). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Burkell, J. (1986). Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 549–563. Logan, G. D., & Burkell, J. (1986). Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 549–563.
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations. I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1072–1090. Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations. I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1072–1090.
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000a). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249. Meiran, N. (2000a). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000b). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 377–399). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Meiran, N. (2000b). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 377–399). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2005). Task rule congruency and Simon-like effects in switching between spatial tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 58A, 1023–1041. Meiran, N. (2005). Task rule congruency and Simon-like effects in switching between spatial tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 58A, 1023–1041.
go back to reference Meiran, N., & Gotler, A. (2001). Modelling cognitive control in task switching and ageing. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 165–186. Meiran, N., & Gotler, A. (2001). Modelling cognitive control in task switching and ageing. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 165–186.
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377. Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377.
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning. II. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 33–48. Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning. II. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 33–48.
go back to reference Peeke, S. C., & Stone, G. C. (1972). Sequential effects in two- and four-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 111–116. Peeke, S. C., & Stone, G. C. (1972). Sequential effects in two- and four-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 111–116.
go back to reference Quinlan, P. T. (1999). Sequential effects in auditory choice reaction time tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 297–303. Quinlan, P. T. (1999). Sequential effects in auditory choice reaction time tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 297–303.
go back to reference Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1968). Repetition effects and signal classification strategies in serial choice-response tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20A, 232–240. Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1968). Repetition effects and signal classification strategies in serial choice-response tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20A, 232–240.
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105. Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105.
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582. Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582.
go back to reference Smith, M. C. (1968). Repetition effect and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 435–439. Smith, M. C. (1968). Repetition effect and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 435–439.
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic S-R bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413. Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic S-R bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1952). The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high-speed performance—a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19. Welford, A. T. (1952). The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high-speed performance—a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19.
Metagegevens
Titel
Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting?
Auteurs
Ronald Hübner
Michel D. Druey
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0219-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2006

Psychological Research 4/2006 Naar de uitgave