Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2006

01-07-2006 | Original Article

The control of visual attention and its influence on prioritized processing in a location negative priming paradigm

Auteurs: Rico Fischer, Herbert Hagendorf

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In a location-based negative priming paradigm, the possibility of a disengagement option of the underlying inhibitory mechanism was tested. Whereas in previous studies disengagement was observed when providing utility information about the probe trial structure, in the present study the allocation of visual attention to the stimuli was manipulated. In the first step an automatic deployment of visual attention was implemented by presenting all stimuli as abrupt onsets (Experiment 1), which demonstrated commonly observed negative priming effects. In further conditions of non-automatic allocation of visual attention in which target and distractor were presented as no-onset stimuli, negative priming effects were eliminated (Experiments 2 and 3). The preferred interpretation is that in conditions of automatic control of attention, target and distractor compete for control of action. A non-automatic control of visual attention, on the other hand, leads to a top-down modulated selection, which results in prioritized target encoding and a loss of distractor impact on the selection process. Alternative accounts and the role of no-onset distractor processing were investigated in Experiment 4.
Voetnoten
1
Note, even though no-onset targets are assumed to be processed efficiently in a no-onset condition with distractor presence, such a claim refers to the selection in a no-onset condition only. Hereby, the assumption of efficient no-onset target processing is not at odds with the observation that general RTs in no-onset trials are slowed compared with onset trials as seen in Experiments 1 and 2. A no-onset presentation can have several implications, which may account for overall slowed RTs. For example, no-onset trials can generally be experienced as more difficult. Also, the mask itself could be responsible for prolonged RTs in Experiment 2 (e.g., longer stimulus identification times)
 
2
We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting this possibility.
 
3
We thank an anonymous reviewer for offering this suggestion.
 
4
We thank an anonymous reviewer for bringing up this point.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Awh, E., Matsukura, M., & Serences, J. T. (2003). Top-down control over biased competition during covert spatial orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 52–63. Awh, E., Matsukura, M., & Serences, J. T. (2003). Top-down control over biased competition during covert spatial orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 52–63.
go back to reference Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.PubMed Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.PubMed
go back to reference Behringer, J. (2000). Experimental run system. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: BeriSoft. Behringer, J. (2000). Experimental run system. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: BeriSoft.
go back to reference Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon. Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon.
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., O’Donnell, C., & Pratt, J. (2002a). Disengaging the negative priming mechanism in locations tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14,207–225.CrossRef Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., O’Donnell, C., & Pratt, J. (2002a). Disengaging the negative priming mechanism in locations tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14,207–225.CrossRef
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., & Khan, M. (2002b). The influence of probe-trial election requirements on the location negative priming effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 273–282. Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., & Khan, M. (2002b). The influence of probe-trial election requirements on the location negative priming effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 273–282.
go back to reference Cave, K. R., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modelling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 225–271.CrossRefPubMed Cave, K. R., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modelling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 225–271.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cepeda, N. J., Cave, K. R., Bichot, N. P., & Kim, M. S. (1998). Spatial selection via feature-driven inhibition of distractor location. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 727–746.PubMed Cepeda, N. J., Cave, K. R., Bichot, N. P., & Kim, M. S. (1998). Spatial selection via feature-driven inhibition of distractor location. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 727–746.PubMed
go back to reference Christie, J., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Negative priming for spatial location? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 24–38.PubMed Christie, J., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Negative priming for spatial location? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 24–38.PubMed
go back to reference Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.
go back to reference Di Lollo, V. (1980). Temporal integration in vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 75–97.CrossRef Di Lollo, V. (1980). Temporal integration in vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 75–97.CrossRef
go back to reference Donk, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2001). Visual marking beside the mark: Prioritizing selection by abrupt onsets. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 891–900.PubMed Donk, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2001). Visual marking beside the mark: Prioritizing selection by abrupt onsets. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 891–900.PubMed
go back to reference Donk, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2003). Prioritizing selection of new elements: Bottom-up versus top-down control. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 1231–1242.PubMed Donk, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2003). Prioritizing selection of new elements: Bottom-up versus top-down control. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 1231–1242.PubMed
go back to reference Downing, P. E. (2000). Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11, 467–473. Downing, P. E. (2000). Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11, 467–473.
go back to reference Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (1999). Can new objects override attentional control settings? Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 727–739.PubMed Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (1999). Can new objects override attentional control settings? Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 727–739.PubMed
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.CrossRef Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.CrossRef
go back to reference Fox, E. (1994). Interference and negative priming from ignored distractors: The role of selection difficulty. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 565–574.PubMed Fox, E. (1994). Interference and negative priming from ignored distractors: The role of selection difficulty. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 565–574.PubMed
go back to reference Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 145–173. Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 145–173.
go back to reference Gibson, B. S. (1996). Visual quality and attentional capture: A challenge to the special role of abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1496–1504. Gibson, B. S. (1996). Visual quality and attentional capture: A challenge to the special role of abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1496–1504.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2000). Intentional control of automatic stimulus-response translation. In Y. Rossetti & A. Revonsuo (Eds.), Beyond dissociation: Interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing (pp. 221–242). Amsterdam: Benjamins. Hommel, B. (2000). Intentional control of automatic stimulus-response translation. In Y. Rossetti & A. Revonsuo (Eds.), Beyond dissociation: Interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing (pp. 221–242). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
go back to reference Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory mechanisms in attention, memory and language (pp. 53–113). Orlando, FL: Academic. Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory mechanisms in attention, memory and language (pp. 53–113). Orlando, FL: Academic.
go back to reference Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary vs. automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary vs. automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception and Psychophysics, 43, 346–354.PubMed Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception and Psychophysics, 43, 346–354.PubMed
go back to reference Kane, M. J., May, C. P., Hasher, L., Rahal, T., & Stolzfus, E. R. (1997). Dual mechanisms of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 637–650. Kane, M. J., May, C. P., Hasher, L., Rahal, T., & Stolzfus, E. R. (1997). Dual mechanisms of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 637–650.
go back to reference MacDonald, P. A., Joordens, S., Seergobin, K. N. (1999). Negative priming effects that are bigger than a breadbox: Attention to distractor does not eliminate negative priming, it enhances it. Memory and Cognition, 27, 197–207. MacDonald, P. A., Joordens, S., Seergobin, K. N. (1999). Negative priming effects that are bigger than a breadbox: Attention to distractor does not eliminate negative priming, it enhances it. Memory and Cognition, 27, 197–207.
go back to reference Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442. Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.
go back to reference Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M., & Seiffert, A. E. (1998). Selective attention: A reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105, 203–229.CrossRefPubMed Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M., & Seiffert, A. E. (1998). Selective attention: A reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105, 203–229.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Milliken, B., Tipper, S., Houghton, G., & Lupianez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception and Psychophysics, 62, 1280–1296.PubMed Milliken, B., Tipper, S., Houghton, G., & Lupianez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception and Psychophysics, 62, 1280–1296.PubMed
go back to reference Müller, H., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330.CrossRef Müller, H., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330.CrossRef
go back to reference Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 565–576. Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 565–576.
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming. II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 993–1000. Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming. II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 993–1000.
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Terry, K. M., & Valdes, L. A. (1994). Negative priming without probe selection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 119–121. Neill, W. T., Terry, K. M., & Valdes, L. A. (1994). Negative priming without probe selection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 119–121.
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In F. N. Dempster, & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 207–261). New York: Academic. Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In F. N. Dempster, & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 207–261). New York: Academic.
go back to reference Pratt, J., & Hirshhorn, M. (2003). Examining the time course of facilitation and inhibition with simultaneous onset and offset cues. Psychological Research, 67, 261–265.CrossRefPubMed Pratt, J., & Hirshhorn, M. (2003). Examining the time course of facilitation and inhibition with simultaneous onset and offset cues. Psychological Research, 67, 261–265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pratt, J., & Hommel, B. (2003). Symbolic control of visual attention: The role of working memory and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 835–845. Pratt, J., & Hommel, B. (2003). Symbolic control of visual attention: The role of working memory and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 835–845.
go back to reference Pratt J., & McAuliffe J. (2001) The effects of onsets and offsets on visual attention. Psychological Research,65, 185–191.CrossRefPubMed Pratt J., & McAuliffe J. (2001) The effects of onsets and offsets on visual attention. Psychological Research,65, 185–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Reder, L. M., Weber, K., Shang, J., & Vanyukov, P. M. (2003). The adaptive character of the attentional system: Statistical sensitivity in a target localization task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 631–649. Reder, L. M., Weber, K., Shang, J., & Vanyukov, P. M. (2003). The adaptive character of the attentional system: Statistical sensitivity in a target localization task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 631–649.
go back to reference Richards, A. (1999). The effects of cuing target location and response mode on interference and negative priming using a visual selection paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 449–463.CrossRefPubMed Richards, A. (1999). The effects of cuing target location and response mode on interference and negative priming using a visual selection paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 449–463.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Samuel, A. G., & Weiner, S. K. (2001). Attentional consequences of object appearance and disappearance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1433–1451. Samuel, A. G., & Weiner, S. K. (2001). Attentional consequences of object appearance and disappearance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1433–1451.
go back to reference Sohn, M.-H., & Carlson, R. A. (2000). Effects of repetition and foreknowledge in task-set reconfiguration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1445–1460. Sohn, M.-H., & Carlson, R. A. (2000). Effects of repetition and foreknowledge in task-set reconfiguration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1445–1460.
go back to reference Stablum, F., Ricci, R., Pavgese, A., & Umiltá, C. (2001). Global/local processing and negative priming: The influence of selection difficulty and stimulus exposure. Psychological Research, 65, 81–97.CrossRefPubMed Stablum, F., Ricci, R., Pavgese, A., & Umiltá, C. (2001). Global/local processing and negative priming: The influence of selection difficulty and stimulus exposure. Psychological Research, 65, 81–97.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and shape. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.PubMed Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and shape. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.PubMed
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Godijn, R., & Pratt, J. (2004). A new estimation of the duration of attentional dwell time. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 60–64.PubMed Theeuwes, J., Godijn, R., & Pratt, J. (2004). A new estimation of the duration of attentional dwell time. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 60–64.PubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590. Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 321–343.CrossRefPubMed Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 321–343.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 591–611.PubMed Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 591–611.PubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J. C., & Bastedo J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: Time course and disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 681–692. Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J. C., & Bastedo J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: Time course and disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 681–692.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., & Houghton, G. (1994). Behavioral goals determine inhibitory mechanisms of selective attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 809–840. Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., & Houghton, G. (1994). Behavioral goals determine inhibitory mechanisms of selective attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 809–840.
go back to reference Todd, J. T., & Van Gelder, P. (1979). Implications of a transient-sustained dichotomy for the measurement of human performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 625–636. Todd, J. T., & Van Gelder, P. (1979). Implications of a transient-sustained dichotomy for the measurement of human performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 625–636.
go back to reference Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature, 400, 867–869.CrossRefPubMed Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature, 400, 867–869.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S. (1998). Control of visual attention. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 223–256). Hove, UK: Psychology. Yantis, S. (1998). Control of visual attention. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 223–256). Hove, UK: Psychology.
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 95–107.CrossRef Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 95–107.CrossRef
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Johnson, D. N. (1990). Mechanisms of attentional priority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 812–825.CrossRef Yantis, S., & Johnson, D. N. (1990). Mechanisms of attentional priority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 812–825.CrossRef
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jones, E. (1991). Mechanisms of attentional selection: Temporally modulated priority tags. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 166–178.PubMed Yantis, S., & Jones, E. (1991). Mechanisms of attentional selection: Temporally modulated priority tags. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 166–178.PubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.CrossRef Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.CrossRef
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 121–134. Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 121–134.
go back to reference Yee, P. L., Santoro, K. E., Grey, A. L., & Woog, V. (2000). Deeper processing at target selection increases the magnitude of negative priming. Memory and Cognition, 28, 1429–1436. Yee, P. L., Santoro, K. E., Grey, A. L., & Woog, V. (2000). Deeper processing at target selection increases the magnitude of negative priming. Memory and Cognition, 28, 1429–1436.
Metagegevens
Titel
The control of visual attention and its influence on prioritized processing in a location negative priming paradigm
Auteurs
Rico Fischer
Herbert Hagendorf
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0220-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2006

Psychological Research 4/2006 Naar de uitgave