Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Youth and Adolescence 9/2017

22-05-2017 | Empirical Research

Joint Effects of Peer Presence and Fatigue on Risk and Reward Processing in Late Adolescence

Auteurs: Karol Silva, Jamie Patrianakos, Jason Chein, Laurence Steinberg

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Youth and Adolescence | Uitgave 9/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Peers are thought to increase adolescents’ risk-taking behavior, at least in part, by heightening their sensitivity to rewards. In this study, we investigate whether the effect of peers on late adolescent males is exacerbated when youth are cognitively fatigued, a state characterized by weakened cognitive control and heightened orientation toward rewards, and well established as a factor that compromises decision making. We hypothesized that fatigued adolescents’ top-down regulation of reward-related impulses may be compromised, thereby potentially amplifying the effect of peers on reward- and risk-seeking behavior. Late adolescent males between 18 and 22 years old (mean age = 19.64, SD = 1.22; 61% Caucasian) completed a decision-making battery either alone or in the presence of 3 same-sex peers, and were either cognitively fatigued or non-fatigued. We compared behavior between four experimental groups—fatigued adolescents in a peer group, non-fatigued adolescents in a peer group, fatigued adolescents by themselves, and non-fatigued adolescents by themselves. The findings showed that cognitive fatigue and peer presence evinced independent effects on risk taking and sensitivity to rewards, but that these factors do not influence adolescent decision-making in an additive or synergistic fashion. To our surprise, being fatigued reduces (but does not eliminate) the effect of peers of risk taking. Moreover, the impact of peers on adolescent males’ ability to learn from negative consequences is not compromised when adolescents are in a state of mental fatigue. Our results suggest that mental fatigue increases late adolescent males’ reward sensitivity to the same extent as peer presence, but does not amplify the peer effect on risk-taking behavior. In this regard, grouping adolescents when they are fatigued may be less dangerous than when they are rested. Similarly, the added presence of peers does not further exacerbate the effect of fatigue on adolescent’s reward- and risk-seeking inclinations. In fact, given peers’ unique effect on adolescents’ ability to learn from costly decisions, our findings suggest that seeking the presence of peers—which is often a rewarding experience in and of itself—may be an adaptive response to mitigate the impact of fatigue on decision making.
Literatuur
go back to reference Ackerman, P. L., & Kanter, R. (2009). Test length and cognitive fatigue: An empirical examination of effects on performance and test-taker reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 163–181. doi:10.1037/a0015719.PubMed Ackerman, P. L., & Kanter, R. (2009). Test length and cognitive fatigue: An empirical examination of effects on performance and test-taker reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 163–181. doi:10.​1037/​a0015719.PubMed
go back to reference Barnes, J. C., & Meldrum, R. C. (2015). The impact of sleep duration on adolescent development: A generically informed analysis of identical twin pairs. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 489–150.CrossRefPubMed Barnes, J. C., & Meldrum, R. C. (2015). The impact of sleep duration on adolescent development: A generically informed analysis of identical twin pairs. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 489–150.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107–116.CrossRefPubMed Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107–116.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brown, B. B., Clasen, D. R., & Eicher, S. A. (1986). Perception of peer pressure, peer conformity dispositions, and self-reported behavior among adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 22, 521–530. Brown, B. B., Clasen, D. R., & Eicher, S. A. (1986). Perception of peer pressure, peer conformity dispositions, and self-reported behavior among adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 22, 521–530.
go back to reference Brown, B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships in adolescence. In R. Lerner, L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. 3rd edn. (pp. 74–103). New York, NY: Wiley. Vol. 2. Brown, B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships in adolescence. In R. Lerner, L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. 3rd edn. (pp. 74–103). New York, NY: Wiley. Vol. 2.
go back to reference Cascio, C. N., Carp, J., O'Donnell, M. B., Tinney, Jr, F. J., Bingham, C. R., Shope, J. T., & Falk, E. B. (2014). Buffering social influence: Neural correlates of response inhibition predict driving safety in the presence of a peer. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(1), 83–95.CrossRef Cascio, C. N., Carp, J., O'Donnell, M. B., Tinney, Jr, F. J., Bingham, C. R., Shope, J. T., & Falk, E. B. (2014). Buffering social influence: Neural correlates of response inhibition predict driving safety in the presence of a peer. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(1), 83–95.CrossRef
go back to reference Cauffman, E., Shulman, E. P., Steinberg, L., Claus, E., Banich, M. T., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2010). Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Developmental Psychology, 46, 193–207. doi:10.1037/a0016128.CrossRefPubMed Cauffman, E., Shulman, E. P., Steinberg, L., Claus, E., Banich, M. T., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2010). Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Developmental Psychology, 46, 193–207. doi:10.​1037/​a0016128.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Centifanti, L. C. M., Modecki, K. L., MacLellan, S., & Gowling, H. (2016). Driving under the influence of risky peers: An experimental study of adolescent risk taking. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(1), 207–222. doi:10.1111/jora.12187.CrossRef Centifanti, L. C. M., Modecki, K. L., MacLellan, S., & Gowling, H. (2016). Driving under the influence of risky peers: An experimental study of adolescent risk taking. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(1), 207–222. doi:10.​1111/​jora.​12187.CrossRef
go back to reference Chassin, L., Hussong, A., Barrera, Jr., M., Molina, B., Trim, R., & Ritter, J. (2004). Adolescent substance use. In R. Lerner, L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. 2nd edn. (pp. 665–696). New York, NY: Wiley. 2004. Chassin, L., Hussong, A., Barrera, Jr., M., Molina, B., Trim, R., & Ritter, J. (2004). Adolescent substance use. In R. Lerner, L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. 2nd edn. (pp. 665–696). New York, NY: Wiley. 2004.
go back to reference Derrick, J. L. (2012). Energized by television: Familiar fictional worlds restore self-control. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(3), 299–307.CrossRef Derrick, J. L. (2012). Energized by television: Familiar fictional worlds restore self-control. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(3), 299–307.CrossRef
go back to reference Dobryakova, E., DeLuca, J., Genova, H. M., & Wylie, G. R. (2013). Neural correlates of cognitive fatigue: Cortico-striatal circuitry and effort-reward imbalance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 849–853.CrossRefPubMed Dobryakova, E., DeLuca, J., Genova, H. M., & Wylie, G. R. (2013). Neural correlates of cognitive fatigue: Cortico-striatal circuitry and effort-reward imbalance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 849–853.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gradisar, M., Gardner, G., & Dohnt, H. (2011). Recent worldwide sleep patterns and problems during adolescence: A review and meta-analysis of age, region, and sleep. Sleep Medicine, 12, 110–118.CrossRefPubMed Gradisar, M., Gardner, G., & Dohnt, H. (2011). Recent worldwide sleep patterns and problems during adolescence: A review and meta-analysis of age, region, and sleep. Sleep Medicine, 12, 110–118.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Green, L., Myerson, J., & Macaux, E. (2005). Temporal discounting when the choice is between two delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 1121–1133.PubMed Green, L., Myerson, J., & Macaux, E. (2005). Temporal discounting when the choice is between two delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 1121–1133.PubMed
go back to reference Gujar, N., Yoo, S.-S., Hu, P., & Walker, M. P. (2011). Sleep deprivation amplifies reactivity of brain reward networks, biasing appraisal of positive emotional experiences. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(12), 4466–4474.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gujar, N., Yoo, S.-S., Hu, P., & Walker, M. P. (2011). Sleep deprivation amplifies reactivity of brain reward networks, biasing appraisal of positive emotional experiences. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(12), 4466–4474.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2014). It is premature to regard the ego-depletion effect as ‘too incredible’. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 298 doi: 0.3389/fpsyg.2014.00298.PubMedPubMedCentral Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2014). It is premature to regard the ego-depletion effect as ‘too incredible’. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 298 doi: 0.3389/fpsyg.2014.00298.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A., & Zwienenberg, M. (2015). A multi-lab pre-registered replication of the ego- depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546–573.CrossRef Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A., & Zwienenberg, M. (2015). A multi-lab pre-registered replication of the ego- depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546–573.CrossRef
go back to reference Hockey, G. R. J., Maule, A. J., Clough, P. J., & Bdzola, L. (2000). Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition and Emotion, 14(6), 823–855.CrossRef Hockey, G. R. J., Maule, A. J., Clough, P. J., & Bdzola, L. (2000). Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition and Emotion, 14(6), 823–855.CrossRef
go back to reference Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2016). Beyond limited resources: Self-control failure as a product of shifting priorities. In K. Vohs, R. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation. 3rd edn. (pp. 165–181). New York, NY: Guilford Press. 2016. Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2016). Beyond limited resources: Self-control failure as a product of shifting priorities. In K. Vohs, R. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation. 3rd edn. (pp. 165–181). New York, NY: Guilford Press. 2016.
go back to reference Kim-Spoon, J., Kahn, R., Deater-Deckard, K., Chiu, P., Steinberg, L., & King-Casas, B. (2016). Risky decision making in a laboratory driving task is associated with health risk behaviors during late adolescence but not adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40, 58–63.CrossRefPubMed Kim-Spoon, J., Kahn, R., Deater-Deckard, K., Chiu, P., Steinberg, L., & King-Casas, B. (2016). Risky decision making in a laboratory driving task is associated with health risk behaviors during late adolescence but not adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40, 58–63.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logue, S., Chein, J., Gould, T., Holliday, E., & Steinberg, L. (2014). Adolescent mice, unlike adults, consume more alcohol in the presence of peers than alone. Developmental Science, 17(1), 79–85.CrossRefPubMed Logue, S., Chein, J., Gould, T., Holliday, E., & Steinberg, L. (2014). Adolescent mice, unlike adults, consume more alcohol in the presence of peers than alone. Developmental Science, 17(1), 79–85.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., & Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology, 106, 857–864.CrossRefPubMed Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., & Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology, 106, 857–864.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Masicampo, E. J., Martin, S. R., & Anderson, R. A. (2014). Understanding and overcoming self-control depletion. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(11), 638–649. doi:10.1111/spc3.12139.CrossRef Masicampo, E. J., Martin, S. R., & Anderson, R. A. (2014). Understanding and overcoming self-control depletion. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(11), 638–649. doi:10.​1111/​spc3.​12139.CrossRef
go back to reference Meldrum, R. C., & Restivo, E. (2014). The behavioral and health consequences of sleep deprivation among U.S. high school students: Relative deprivation matters. Preventive Medicine, 36, 24–28.CrossRef Meldrum, R. C., & Restivo, E. (2014). The behavioral and health consequences of sleep deprivation among U.S. high school students: Relative deprivation matters. Preventive Medicine, 36, 24–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Menz, M. M., Buchel, C., & Peters, J. (2012). Sleep deprivation is associated with attenuated parametric valuation and control signals in the midbrain during value-based decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(20), 6937–6946.CrossRefPubMed Menz, M. M., Buchel, C., & Peters, J. (2012). Sleep deprivation is associated with attenuated parametric valuation and control signals in the midbrain during value-based decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(20), 6937–6946.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Molden, D. C., Hui, C. M., Scholer, Aa, Meier, B. P., Noreen, E. E., D’Agostino, P. R., & Martin, V. (2012). Motivational vs. metabolic effects of carbohydrates on self-control. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1137–1144. doi:10.1177/0956797612439069.CrossRefPubMed Molden, D. C., Hui, C. M., Scholer, Aa, Meier, B. P., Noreen, E. E., D’Agostino, P. R., & Martin, V. (2012). Motivational vs. metabolic effects of carbohydrates on self-control. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1137–1144. doi:10.​1177/​0956797612439069​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., & Nienhaus, K. (2002). Self-control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of the self-control strength model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 113–120.CrossRefPubMed Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., & Nienhaus, K. (2002). Self-control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of the self-control strength model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 113–120.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user’s guide. 7th edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user’s guide. 7th edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
go back to reference Ohmura, Y., Takahashi, T., Kitamura, N., & Wehr, P. (2006). Three-month stability of delay and probability discounting measures. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14, 318–328.CrossRefPubMed Ohmura, Y., Takahashi, T., Kitamura, N., & Wehr, P. (2006). Three-month stability of delay and probability discounting measures. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14, 318–328.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Osgood, D. W., Wilson, J. K., Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (1996). Routine activities and individual deviant behavior. American Sociological Review, 61, 635–655.CrossRef Osgood, D. W., Wilson, J. K., Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (1996). Routine activities and individual deviant behavior. American Sociological Review, 61, 635–655.CrossRef
go back to reference Ouimet, M. C., Simons-Morton, B. G., Zador, P. L., Lerner, N. D., Freedman, M., Duncan, G. D., & Wang, J. (2010). Using the U.S. National household travel survey to estimate the impact of passenger characteristics on young drivers’ relative risk of fatal crash involvement. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42, 689–694.CrossRef Ouimet, M. C., Simons-Morton, B. G., Zador, P. L., Lerner, N. D., Freedman, M., Duncan, G. D., & Wang, J. (2010). Using the U.S. National household travel survey to estimate the impact of passenger characteristics on young drivers’ relative risk of fatal crash involvement. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42, 689–694.CrossRef
go back to reference Peach, H. D., & Gaultney, J. F. (2013). Sleep, impulse control, and sensation-seeking predict delinquent behavior in adolescents, emerging adults, and adults. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53, 293–299.CrossRefPubMed Peach, H. D., & Gaultney, J. F. (2013). Sleep, impulse control, and sensation-seeking predict delinquent behavior in adolescents, emerging adults, and adults. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53, 293–299.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pilcher, J. J., & Walters, A. S. (2010). How sleep deprivation affects psychological variables related to college students’ cognitive performance. Journal of American College Health, 46(3), 121–126.CrossRef Pilcher, J. J., & Walters, A. S. (2010). How sleep deprivation affects psychological variables related to college students’ cognitive performance. Journal of American College Health, 46(3), 121–126.CrossRef
go back to reference Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2017). Vigour and fatigue: How variation in affect underlies effective self-control. In T. Braver (Ed.), Motivation and cognitive control. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis/Routledge. Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2017). Vigour and fatigue: How variation in affect underlies effective self-control. In T. Braver (Ed.), Motivation and cognitive control. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis/Routledge.
go back to reference Schmeichel, B. J., Harmon-Jones, C., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). Exercising self-control increases approach motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 162–173. doi:10.1037/a0019797.CrossRefPubMed Schmeichel, B. J., Harmon-Jones, C., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). Exercising self-control increases approach motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 162–173. doi:10.​1037/​a0019797.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. (2009). Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 770–782.CrossRefPubMed Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. (2009). Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 770–782.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L., Willoughby, T., Reker, D. L., Campbell, K., Chalmers, H., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2012). Adolescent peer interaction and trait surgency weaken medial prefrontal cortex responses to failure. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 115–124. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq090.CrossRefPubMed Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L., Willoughby, T., Reker, D. L., Campbell, K., Chalmers, H., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2012). Adolescent peer interaction and trait surgency weaken medial prefrontal cortex responses to failure. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 115–124. doi:10.​1093/​scan/​nsq090.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Silva, K., Shulman, E. P., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2015). Peers increase adolescents’ exploratory behavior and sensitivity to positive and negative feedback. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26, 696–705.CrossRefPubMed Silva, K., Shulman, E. P., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2015). Peers increase adolescents’ exploratory behavior and sensitivity to positive and negative feedback. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26, 696–705.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, G. (2014). Adolescent cognitive control unaffected y presence of peers. Paper presented at The International Congress for Integrative Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience meeting. Los Angeles, CA. Rosenbaum, G. (2014). Adolescent cognitive control unaffected y presence of peers. Paper presented at The International Congress for Integrative Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience meeting. Los Angeles, CA.
go back to reference Shulman, E., Monahan, K., & Steinberg, L. (2017). Severe violence during adolescence and early adulthood and its relation to anticipated rewards and costs. Child Development, 88, 16–26.CrossRefPubMed Shulman, E., Monahan, K., & Steinberg, L. (2017). Severe violence during adolescence and early adulthood and its relation to anticipated rewards and costs. Child Development, 88, 16–26.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spear, L. P. (2009). The behavioral neuroscience of adolescence. New York, NY: Norton. Spear, L. P. (2009). The behavioral neuroscience of adolescence. New York, NY: Norton.
go back to reference Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(3), 379–384. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.007.CrossRef Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(3), 379–384. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jesp.​2006.​05.​007.CrossRef
go back to reference van der Linden, D., Frese, M., & Meijman, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychologica, 113, 45–65.CrossRefPubMed van der Linden, D., Frese, M., & Meijman, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychologica, 113, 45–65.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van Hoorn, J., Crone, E. A., & van Leijenhorst, L. (2016). Hanging out with the right crowd: peer influence on risk-taking behavior in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(1), 189–200. doi:10.1111/jora.12265.CrossRefPubMed van Hoorn, J., Crone, E. A., & van Leijenhorst, L. (2016). Hanging out with the right crowd: peer influence on risk-taking behavior in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(1), 189–200. doi:10.​1111/​jora.​12265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weigard, A., Chein, J., Albert, D., Smith, A., & Steinberg, A. (2014). Effects of anonymous peer observation on adolescents’ preference for immediate rewards. Developmental Science, 17, 71–78. doi:10.1111/desc.12099.CrossRefPubMed Weigard, A., Chein, J., Albert, D., Smith, A., & Steinberg, A. (2014). Effects of anonymous peer observation on adolescents’ preference for immediate rewards. Developmental Science, 17, 71–78. doi:10.​1111/​desc.​12099.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Womack, S. D., Hook, J. N., Reyna, S. H., & Romos, M. (2013). Sleep loss and risk-taking behavior: A review of the literature. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 11, 343–359.CrossRefPubMed Womack, S. D., Hook, J. N., Reyna, S. H., & Romos, M. (2013). Sleep loss and risk-taking behavior: A review of the literature. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 11, 343–359.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yusainy, C., & Lawrence, C. (2015). Brief mindfulness induction could reduce aggression after depletion. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 125–134.CrossRefPubMed Yusainy, C., & Lawrence, C. (2015). Brief mindfulness induction could reduce aggression after depletion. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 125–134.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Joint Effects of Peer Presence and Fatigue on Risk and Reward Processing in Late Adolescence
Auteurs
Karol Silva
Jamie Patrianakos
Jason Chein
Laurence Steinberg
Publicatiedatum
22-05-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Youth and Adolescence / Uitgave 9/2017
Print ISSN: 0047-2891
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-6601
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0690-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 9/2017

Journal of Youth and Adolescence 9/2017 Naar de uitgave