Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 2/2023

Open Access 17-09-2022

Explaining the Association Between Fetal Growth and Childhood ADHD Symptoms: Cross-cohort Replication

Auteurs: Niamh Dooley, Colm Healy, Ross Brannigan, David Cotter, Mary Clarke, Mary Cannon

Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology | Uitgave 2/2023

Abstract

The association between restricted fetal growth and symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in childhood is well-replicated and robust. However, fetal growth is determined by many prenatal factors and associations with mental health may be confounded by familial and social context. In this study, we sought to quantify the relative contributions of prenatal factors and familial confounds to the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms. Two independent cohorts were analyzed, the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study (ABCD; United States) and the Growing Up in Ireland (GUI) study. ADHD symptoms were measured by the Child Behavior Checklist (ABCD) and the Strengths & Difficulties questionnaire (GUI) at age 9–10. Using sequential regression models, we assessed the change-in-association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms after controlling for sex, familial factors (socioeconomic/demographic factors & family psychiatric history) and prenatal factors (pregnancy complications & maternal substance-use during pregnancy). Converging findings from cohorts suggested that over a quarter of the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms is attributable to familial confounds. The degree to which the association was explained by prenatal factors differed by cohort—pregnancy complications explained a larger proportion of the effect in ABCD (7.9%) than GUI (2.7%), and maternal substance-use explained a larger proportion of the effect in GUI (22.7%) compared to ABCD (4.8%). Different explanations of the fetal growth-ADHD association across cohorts suggests cohort-specific, and potentially nationally-specific, risk factors for fetal growth and related neurodevelopmental outcomes. The evidence suggests early prevention of ADHD in Ireland should focus on minimizing maternal smoking during pregnancy. In the US, prevention and treatment of pregnancy complications are highlighted as viable targets for intervention.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-022-00971-9.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Well established risk factors for childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) include genetic/familial susceptibility to ADHD, male sex, and restricted fetal growth (Thapar et al., 2013). As a partially environmentally-determined characteristic, fetal growth may be the most amenable to intervention. However, it is not currently understood which fetal growth determinant best explains the association between restricted fetal growth and ADHD symptoms.
Birth weight is a crude approximation of the quality of fetal growth. If an infant’s weight is appropriate for their gestational age, we could assume that their fetal growth rate was typical. The association between lower birth weight and attention problems in childhood has been replicated many times, in many populations (meta-analyses: Franz et al., 2018; Momany et al., 2018). Lower birth weights, even among those born at full term, are linked with an elevated risk of ADHD and academic difficulties (Class et al., 2014; Groen-Blokhuis et al., 2011; Hultman et al., 2007; Momany et al., 2018). Within-twin and within-sibling studies suggest that the association may be independent from genetics and other familial factors (Class et al., 2014; Ficks et al., 2013; Groen-Blokhuis et al., 2011; Hultman et al., 2007; Lim et al., 2018; Pettersson et al., 2015). Experimentally-induced fetal growth restriction in animals has also been shown to influence neurodevelopment and behavior (Lauritz et al., 2012; Meyer et al., 2014). It has therefore been proposed that poor fetal growth is an independent causal factor in the development of ADHD. The replicable association between birth weight and ADHD symptoms in twins also shows that, despite experiencing the same prenatal exposures (e.g., maternal smoking), twin differences in fetal growth can occur and it is this that predicts differences in childhood ADHD symptoms. Large-scale studies of singleton births are needed to understand which prenatal factors contribute to restricted fetal growth, and which account for the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms on a population level.
The fetal origins hypothesis (and more recently the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease hypothesis) posits that conditions in utero can permanently program certain aspects of the child’s physiology, explaining why lower birth weights may be causally related to neurodevelopmental issues. However, fetal growth rate and birth weight are the products of many prenatal and pre-pregnancy factors such as maternal malnutrition, smoking, drug-use, certain medications, parental age and pregnancy/gestational complications (Budree et al., 2017; Heaman et al., 2013; McCowan & Horgan, 2009), several of which are also directly associated with subsequent mental health of the child (He et al., 2020; Smith et al., 2016). Proposed mechanisms to explain how prenatal risk factors such as maternal smoking and gestational infection directly impact neurodevelopment have included: lack of oxygen and blood to the fetus leading to altered gene expression (Smith et al., 2016), neurotransmitter function (Laplante et al., 2012), and activation of immune and inflammatory processes (Meyer et al., 2009; Mirza et al., 2015). It may be that birth weight is a convenient proxy for such direct effects of fetal adversity on neurodevelopment. In this study, we quantify the extent to which the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms are explained by prenatal determinants of fetal growth. We are particularly interested in the contribution of modifiable prenatal factors such as maternal substance-use in pregnancy. Understanding the extent to which the association is modifiable will help establish the viability of prenatal interventions.
Familial background is a major source of confounding in the theoretically causal pathway between prenatal adversity, lower birth weight and childhood mental health problems. For instance young maternal age is linked with both low birth weights (Aras, 2013) and increased ADHD disruptive behaviours in the child (Tearne, 2015). Household socioeconomics, often exemplified by income and education level, has also shown strong links with both reduced birth weight (Madden, 2014; Martinson & Reichman, 2016) and ADHD risk (Russell et al., 2016). That said, the association between birth weight and ADHD symptoms typically survives correction for familial socioeconomic factors (Abel et al., 2010; Class et al., 2014; Lærum et al., 2017; Pettersson et al., 2019). Low birth weight and ADHD are also not randomly distributed across racial, ethnic or migrant groups. Black women born in the US are more likely to have a low birth weight/preterm child than any other race/ethnicity even after controlling for income and educational attainment, though other unmeasured inequalities cannot be ruled out (Catov et al., 2015; Giscombé & Lobel, 2005). Family psychiatric history is also relevant to both low birth weight and ADHD via several plausible pathways. It captures genetic susceptibility to ADHD (Thapar et al., 2013), but psychiatric issues in the mother may also affect fetal growth via increased stress (Mongan et al., 2019; Wadhwa et al., 2011), and problematic behaviors and coping styles may also be socially learned. Demographic/socioeconomic factors and family psychiatric history should therefore be controlled for when assessing the effect of fetal growth on ADHD symptoms.
No study to our knowledge has assessed the extent to which the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms is explained by preceding prenatal factors. This is important information because fetal growth, estimated from weight and age at birth, is not in itself a modifiable factor. Understanding which prenatal factors and to what extent they contribute to the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms may help identify prenatal targets for preventative interventions. Similarly, but not equivalently, studies have used prenatal factors to directly predict ADHD diagnosis, where the goal is predictive accuracy rather than explanation (Huhdanpaa et al., 2021; Schwenke et al., 2018; Sciberras et al., 2011; Silva et al., 2014; Willoughby et al., 2020). Other research has assessed the effect of birth weight on ADHD symptoms after adjustment for prenatal factors, however the focus of those studies is generally on the final, fully adjusted, birth weight effect, and not on how it changes due to the addition of each type of explanatory factor (Murray et al., 2016; Silva et al., 2014; Wiles et al., 2006). This study attempts to disentangle potentially confounding factors such as familial income and psychiatric history, and prenatal factors, which temporally precede fetal growth and may explain its effects.
Several other aspects of our study design differentiate it from the existing literature. First, we use a continuous parent-reported scale as our outcome, rather than binary ADHD diagnosis, or a cut-off corresponding to clinical risk, which have typically been the outcomes of choice when assessing prenatal predictors. While diagnosis can be clinically informative, subjective symptom scales can offer greater sensitivity to smaller effects. This may be particularly important given that low birth weight has been associated with ADHD symptom shifts within the sub-clinical range (Milberger et al., 1997; Murray et al., 2016, 2021). Second, unlike studies which assess one risk factor in isolation (e.g., maternal smoking), our data includes an array of pregnancy complications and substances consumed by the mother, allowing us to assess independence of their effects. Finally, our study uses two independent population-based cohorts of children (both aged 9–10), one from the United States (Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study; ABCD; N = 8,358) and one from Ireland (Growing Up in Ireland study; GUI; N = 7,724) to test replicability of findings. Meta-analytic findings suggested the association between birth weight and ADHD symptoms was significantly moderated by geographic region suggesting that context may be important when considering this particular association (Momany et al., 2018). Parallel analysis in two nationally-representative samples may help disentangle generalizable from context-specific explanations for the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms.

Aims of the Study

1.
Quantify the extent to which (i) familial confounds and (ii) prenatal factors account for the association between birth weight and childhood ADHD symptoms
 
2.
Assess the cohort-generalizability of these findings.
 

Material and Methods

Participants

ABCD (United States)

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (abcdstudy.​org) is an ongoing longitudinal study of over 10,000 children born 2007–2009 and recruited from 22 sites across the United States. The ABCD sample was designed to mirror US population demographics by recruiting through geographically, demographically, and socioeconomically diverse school systems surrounding each of the research sites. A stratified probability sample of schools was selected for each site based on sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and urbanicity to minimize systematic sampling biases in recruitment at the school level (Garavan et al., 2018). We included baseline data only (release 3.0) which assesses the children aged 9–10 (referred to as 9 for simplicity). All data, including details about the pregnancy and birth, was provided retrospectively at this time point. In cases where 2 or more children from the same household were included, we included only the eldest to ensure independence of observations. Our final dataset from ABCD included 8,835 singleton-born children with outcome data (mean age = 119 months [9 years & 11 months], SD = 7 months). The University of California at San Diego (San Diego, CA, 208 USA) Institutional Review Board was responsible for the ethical oversight of the ABCD study. ABCD data can be accessed by application to the NIMH Data Archive (NDA) Repository (ndar.​nih.​gov). The data used in this report was drawn from Release 3.0 and will be available on the NDA (https://​doi.​org/​10.​15154/​1524734).

GUI (Ireland)

The Growing Up in Ireland (GUI) study (growingup.​ie) is an ongoing longitudinal study of over 10,000 children born between December 2007 and June 2008 inclusive. Children were sampled from the Child Benefit Register and stratified by marital status, county of residence, nationality and number of children in household to achieve national representativeness (Quail et al., 2011). Thornton et al. (2013) showed that the proportions of unemployed parents, non-nationals, specific maternal age groups, and parental education levels in the baseline GUI sample approximated those in the Irish population during those years. Data collection began at 9 months of age, with follow-ups at ages 3, 5, 7 and 9 years. We used the 9-month data for information surrounding the pregnancy/birth and socioeconomic factors in the household and the 9-year data to assess ADHD symptoms. For consistency with ABCD, socioeconomic factors such as income and parental education were extracted from the 9-year data. Our final dataset from GUI included 7,724 singleton-born children with outcome data (mean age = 113 months [9 years & 5 months], SD = 2 months). Ethical approval for GUI was granted by the research ethics committee of the Health Research Board in Ireland. Results in this report are based on analyses of data from the Research Microdata Files. This data is not publicly available but is available to researchers affiliated with an Irish institution via application to the Central Statistics Office (see growingup.​ie).
For both data sets, the respondent of interviews and questionnaires was most often the mother (ABCD: 85%; GUI: 97%).

Cohort Comparison

Cohorts differed in several ways making it inappropriate to merge them. First, ADHD symptoms were measured using two different tools (CBCL & SDQ). Second, information about the pregnancy and birth were recorded 9 months post-partum in GUI and 9 years post-partum in ABCD. Third, some variables had slightly different definitions across cohorts (e.g., family psychiatric history; Table S7).
Ethical approval for the secondary analysis of both datasets was granted by the Research Ethics Committee of the Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland.

Measures

ADHD Symptoms

The attention problems subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) was used in ABCD. This parent-rated questionnaire contains 119 items in total, 10 of which reflect attention problems. Items are rated on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = not at all true; 1 = somewhat true; 2 = very true). The attention problems scale reflects both inattention and hyperactivity symptoms and ranges from 0–20. Meta-analysis shows that this scale can discriminate ADHD cases and controls with moderate accuracy (AUC = 0.82; Chang et al., 2016).
In GUI, the hyperactivity scale from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire was used (SDQ; Goodman, 1997), which is also parent-rated with each item scored on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = not true; 1 = somewhat true; 3 = certainly true). The SDQ contains 25 items, 5 of which form a hyperactivity score. Despite the name, items probe inattention (2), hyperactivity (2) and impulsivity (1). A review of 8 studies showed this scale can discriminate ADHD cases and controls with high accuracy (AUC = 0.90, range = 0.76–0.97; Stone et al., 2010).
The parent-reported CBCL and SDQ attention/hyperactivity problem scales are highly correlated (r ~ 0.68; Koskelainen et al., 2000; Stone et al., 2010). Items used to form each scale are provided in Table S1. Raw scores were used rather than T-scores allowing greater precision as they are not truncated, an approach which is advocated by Achenbach and Rescorla (2001) for research in non-clinical settings.
Both scales were log-transformed for analysis due to positive skew.

Fetal Growth

Fetal growth was calculated as the difference between an individual’s birth weight (in kilograms) and the average birth weight for their gestational age (in weeks). It is therefore a measure of deviance from the average birth weight for a given gestational age. Fetal growth was used instead of raw birth weight as several studies suggest that the fetal growth rate is the aspect of birth weight most strongly linked with ADHD symptoms (Groen-Blokhuis et al., 2011; Momany et al., 2018; Pettersson et al., 2015).
In ABCD, gestational age was not recorded for births after gestational week 40. Therefore, fetal growth does not account for post-term growth in this cohort. Birth weight was originally reported in pounds and ounces in ABCD, which was converted kilograms. Where pounds were reported whilst ounces were not birth weight was calculated to the nearest kg using pounds alone. Values which were unlikely to reflect true birth weight (e.g. ounces only) were removed. Birth information was reported retrospectively at age 9–10 in ABCD, however agreement between medical records and retrospective maternal report of exact birth weight has been shown to be strong (Rice et al., 2007).

Familial Factors

Socioeconomics & Demographics

Five socioeconomic & demographic factors were included: race/ethnicity, maternal age, household income, parental education level and single parenthood. Race/ethnicity groups differed by cohort with 5 available in ABCD and 4 available in GUI (Table 1). “Other/mixed” race/ethnicity was not included in GUI analysis due to small group size (n = 29). Maternal age at birth was recorded retrospectively in ABCD while GUI asked the mother’s age at time of first assessment at 9 months postpartum. Household income in ABCD was a choice of 10 income brackets from < $5000 to $200,000 + and reflects income from all sources before deductions. In GUI, income reflected that from all sources, after tax and social insurance, divided by the number of people in the household, and split into deciles. Parental education refers to the education level of the parent with the highest level. Educational levels were binned into 5 levels for both cohorts reflecting (1) incomplete schooling (before completion of secondary/high school); (2) school complete (e.g. Leaving Certificate, high school degree, GED); (3) sub-Bachelor qualification (e.g. non-degree certificate, associate degree); (4) Bachelor or Professional degree; (5) Postgraduate degree, diploma or certificate. In ABCD, dual-parenthood was defined as the presence of a partner to the primary caregiver who was involved in 40% or more of the child’s daily activities. In GUI, dual-parenthood was defined by spouse/partner to the primary caregiver who was resident in the home. Income, education and single-parenthood were recorded at the time of ADHD symptom assessment (age 9).
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for all variables used in analysis from both cohorts
ABCD (United States; N = 8,835)
GUI (Ireland; N = 7,724)
Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Symptoms
CBCL “attention problems”
M = 3.15, SD = 3.58
(range: 0–19)
SDQ “hyperactivity difficulties”
M = 3.06, SD = 2.58
(range: 0–10)
Child age (months)
M = 119, SD = 7
(range: 108–131)
Child age (months)
M = 113, SD = 2
(range: 111–120)
Birth Data
Fetal Growth
M = 0.0, SD = 0.5
(range: -2.3—2.5)
Fetal Growth
M = 0.0, SD = 0.5
(range: -2.9—4.3)
Sex
53% male (n = 4,675)
Sex
50% male (n = 3,883)
Demographics & Socioeconomics
Race/Ethnicity of child
49% White (n = 4,334)
Race/Ethnicity of mother
85% White Irish (n = 6,517)
 
23% Hispanic (n = 1,985)
 
10% White non-Irish (n = 802)
 
15% Black (n = 1,348)
 
2% African or Black (n = 187)
 
3% Asian (n = 225)
 
2% Asian (n = 171)
 
11% Other (n = 932)
 
0.4% Other/Mixed (n = 29)
Maternal age (years)
M = 29, SD = 6 (range: 13–60)
Maternal age (years) a
M = 32, SD = 5 (range: 16–55)
Household income bracket
M = 7.1, SD = 2.5 (range: 1–10)
Household equivalized income deciles
M = 5.8, SD = 2.9 (range: 1–10)
Parent education level
M = 3.5, SD = 1.3 (range:1–5)
Parent education level
M = 3.3, SD = 1.2 (range: 1–5)
Single-parent
20% (n = 1,747)
Single-parent
9% (n = 732)
Family Psychiatric History b
Parental depression/anxiety
34% (n = 3295)
Parental depression/anxiety
34% (n = 2,589)
Parental conduct issues
14% (n = 1314)
Parental trouble with law
15% (n = 1,157)
Familial substance-use issues
20% (n = 1966)
Familial substance-use issues
2% (n = 153)
Pregnancy Complications
Total pregnancy complications
M = 0.5, SD = 0.8 (range: 0–7)
Total pregnancy complications
M = 0.6, SD = 0.9 (range: 0–6)
Severe/persistent nausea and vomiting c
14% (n = 1,163)
Persistent vomiting or nausea
16% (n = 1,279)
Urinary tract infection
8% (n = 651)
Urinary or kidney infection
14% (n = 1,071)
Pre-eclampsia, eclampsia or toxemia
6% (n = 486)
Pre-eclampsia
7% (n = 516)
High blood pressure
9% (n = 727)
High blood pressure
10% (n = 809)
Diabetes (gestational)
7% (n = 575)
Diabetes (gestational)
3% (n = 213)
Heavy bleeding requiring bed rest or special treatment
3% (n = 274)
Bleeding in second half
6% (n = 445)
Rhesus incompatibility
3% (n = 219)
Rhesus incompatibility
4% (n = 300)
Placental previa, abruptio or other issue
3% (n = 228)
Placenta previa or other placental disorder
3% (n = 232)
Maternal substance-use in pregnancy
Smoking
14% (n = 1,244)
Smoking
16% (n = 1,175)
Alcohol
28% (n = 2,293)
Alcohol
22% (n = 1,653)
Drugs d
8% (n = 700)
Drugs d
1% (n = 79)
aMaternal age in GUI refers to age at 9 months postpartum
bSee Table S7 for specific definition of family psychiatric history variables
cSpecifically defined as nausea and vomiting beyond 6th month or accompanied by weight loss
dABCD drug-use refers to any non-prescription drug; GUI drug-use refers specifically to use of marijuana/cannabis, heroin/methadone/crack/cocaine, or amphetamines/other stimulants

Family Psychiatric History

Parental depression/anxiety was defined as at least one parent having ever suffered from depression, nerves or nervous breakdown (ABCD), or at least one parent having ever been treated for clinical depression, anxiety, nerves or phobias (GUI). Parental conduct issues in ABCD were captured by either parent having trouble with the police or law, difficulty retaining employment, or getting into fights. In GUI, parental conduct issues were captured by either parent having been in trouble with the police for any offence other than a traffic offence. Familial substance-use problems in ABCD referred to any member of the immediate family (biological parents or full-siblings) ever having a history of drug/alcohol-related problems while in GUI was defined as the study child experiencing alcoholism or drug-use within the immediate family. Specific definitions of each aspect of psychiatric history are provided in Table S7. For GUI, family psychiatric data was aggregated across all available waves to better approximate “lifetime” parental histories (see Supplementary Material for further details).

Prenatal Factors

Pregnancy Complications

The eight pregnancy complications recorded in both cohorts were: persistent nausea/vomiting, pre-eclampsia, high blood-pressure, urinary tract (or kidney) infection, bleeding, placenta issues (e.g., previa), rhesus incompatibility and gestational diabetes (insulin-/diet-treated). Table 1 shows any cohort differences in wording of these conditions. Pregnancy complications were retrospectively reported 9–10 years post-partum in ABCD, however long-term maternal recall for obstetric complications has shown to be accurate (Ramos et al., 2020; Rice et al., 2007). As many of these complications may co-occur, a sum of all complications was used for the analysis.

Maternal Substance-use in Pregnancy

Maternal smoking, alcohol-use and drug-use referred to those events at any point during pregnancy and at any frequency. While frequency and timing of smoking in pregnancy may be relevant (Brannigan et al., 2020) these were not included in the analysis due to cohort-differences in how timing was captured (trimesters 1–3 [GUI] Vs before and after knowledge of the pregnancy [ABCD]; Table S8). In ABCD, drug-use during pregnancy was defined broadly by “any non-prescription drug use”. On the other hand, GUI mothers were asked specifically about use of the following drug groups during pregnancy: cannabis/marijuana, amphetamines or other stimulants, heroin/methadone/crack/cocaine.
These variables may overlap to some degree with family history of substance-use problems (above) however the former is distinct by being specific to the mother during the pregnancy and including any level of substance-use (not necessarily abuse).

Data Analysis

Data Cleaning

Coherence between the cohorts was maximized regarding choice of variables, manipulation of variables (e.g., centering, transforming) and statistical analysis. For instance, only those family psychiatric history variables and pregnancy complications that were available in both cohorts were included.
Dimensionality of the data was reduced by creating totals (total number of gestational complications) or overarching binary variables (e.g., any drug during pregnancy). This was preferred to data-driven reduction methods such as principal components analysis as it allowed easier comparison between cohorts and generalizability to other studies.
All binary variables were treatment-coded [0,1] except sex which was deviation-coded [-0.5, 0.5] such that the intercept referred to the average of males and females. Continuous predictor variables with non-zero means were centered (income, education, maternal age). Reference levels of categorical variables were the most common level.

Statistical Analysis

“Explanation” of the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms in this study refers to the statistical reduction of the association due to the step-by-step addition of other variables, as with a classic confounding model. A baseline association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms is established (represented by the beta coefficient) in each cohort while controlling for sex (Model 0; M0). In the next step (M1), 5 potentially confounding socioeconomic and demographic factors are consecutively added to the baseline model and the change in birth weight coefficient from baseline is noted. If the effect of fetal growth reduces in magnitude, there is an overlap or covariance between the contribution of socioeconomic/demographic factors and the contribution of fetal growth to ADHD symptoms. The same logic is applied to family psychiatric history (M2), pregnancy complications (M3) and maternal substance-use in pregnancy (M4). Any remaining effect of fetal growth reflects that which is not explained by variables in M1-4. Order of entry was based on the modifiability of factors with familial factors considered least modifiable and maternal behaviors during pregnancy most modifiable.
Models M0-M4 were standard linear regressions, performed using the stats package (v3.6.3) in R. As the outcome (ADHD symptoms) was skewed in both cohorts (Fig. S1), they were log-transformed. As such, B coefficients in these linear models refer to change in log(ADHD symptoms) for each unit increase in predictor, however we were more interested in the % change in estimate due to the addition of other factors (M1-4) rather than the absolute estimate.
To test whether results were robust to a different choice of model, an ordinal logistic model was performed with symptom groups as outcome (low, moderate, high or very high; Fig. S1), using the polr function from the MASS package (v7.3–51.4). Groups were based on percentiles (low =  < 50th percentile, moderate = 50th-79th percentile, high = 80th-89th percentile; very high = 90th-100th percentile). While the coefficients of a logistic model are expressed in log(odds of outcome) the % change in coefficients due to the addition of other factors are comparable to a linear model.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 provides descriptive statistics for all variables used in the analysis. The average score on ADHD symptom scales was approximately 3 points in both cohorts, despite use of different scales with different ranges (CBCL: M = 3.15 [SD = 3.58]; SDQ: M = 3.06 [SD = 2.58]). Mean fetal growth was zero by definition, with a SD of 0.5 kg in both cohorts. That is, most participants had a birth weight that was typical for their gestational age, but variation by half a kilogram was common. For both cohorts, fetal growth distributions were approximately normal (Fig. S5) and trajectories of fetal growth approximated international norms (Fig. S6).
Racial/ethnic variation was greater in ABCD compared to GUI, the latter of which was 85% White Irish (Table 1). The average maternal age at birth was 2–3 younger in ABCD than in GUI. Mean parental education level was similar across ABCD (M = 3.5, SD = 1.3) and GUI (M = 3.3, SD = 1.2) which was between a non-degree qualification (level 3) and a primary degree (level 4; e.g. Bachelor). Single-parent households were more common in ABCD (20%) compared to GUI (9%). Income levels could not be directly compared across cohorts as ABCD reported income bracket before tax and GUI reported income decile after tax.

Inferential Statistics

The baseline association between fetal growth and childhood ADHD symptoms was significant in both ABCD (B = 0.06, 95% CI = 0.03–0.10, t = 3.41, p = 0.001) and GUI (B = 0.075, 95% CI = 0.04–0.11, t = 4.39, p < 0.001). After controlling for socioeconomic and demographic factors (M1), family psychiatric history (M2), pregnancy complications (M3), and maternal substance-use (M4), the effect of fetal growth on ADHD symptoms was reduced to non-significance in ABCD (B = 0.04, 95% CI = -0.003 – 0.08, t = 1.78, p = 0.08) and to marginal significance in GUI (B = 0.04, 95% CI = 0.001–0.07, t = 1.98, p = 0.05). This corresponded to a 43% and 52% drop in the fetal growth effect estimate from baseline to fully-adjusted in ABCD and GUI respectively (Fig. 1). Note B statistics are in terms of log-transformed symptom scales.
Socioeconomic and demographic factors (household income, parental education, maternal age, single-parenthood, race/ethnicity) accounted for 23.4% of the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms on average (ABCD = 25.4% Vs GUI = 21.3%). Family psychiatric history (parent depression/anxiety, parent conduct issues, familial substance-abuse) accounted for a further 5% of the association (ABCD = 4.8% Vs GUI = 5.3%). After controlling for these two sets of confounds (M1 + M2), the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms remained significant at the 5% alpha level in both cohorts, though the association was more robust in GUI (p = 0.002) compared to ABCD (p = 0.027; Table 2). The proportion of the effect explained by M1 + M2 confounds was also higher in ABCD (30.2%) than GUI (26.6%) which is evident from Fig. 2.
Table 2
Change in the fetal growth effect on ADHD symptoms, after stepwise control for other factors
  
M0
M1
M2
M3
M4
 
  
Baseline model a
M0 + SES & demographics
M1 + Family psych history
M2 + Pregnancy complications
M3 + Maternal substance-use
Total % change
ABCD
Fetal growth B
0.063
0.047
0.044
0.039
0.036
 
 
95% CI
[0.03, 0.10]
[0.001, 0.09]
[0.005, 0.08]
[-0.0002, 0.08]
[-0.003, 0.08]
 
 
p
< .001
0.018
0.027
0.051
0.076
 
 
ηp2
0.001
0.001
0.001
0.001
< 0.001
 
 
B change
0.016
0.003
0.005
0.003
 
 
% of baseline
25.4%
4.8%
7.9%
4.8%
43%
GUI
Fetal growth B
0.075
0.059
0.055
0.053
0.036
 
 
95% CI
[0.04, 0.11]
[0.02, 0.09]
[0.02, 0.09]
[0.02, 0.09]
[0.0003, 0.07]
 
 
p
< .001
0.001
0.002
0.003
0.048
 
 
ηp2
0.003
0.002
0.001
0.001
0.001
 
 
B change
0.016
0.004
0.002
0.017
 
 
% of baseline
21.3%
5.3%
2.7%
22.7%
52%
Cohort Ave
% of baseline
 
23.4%
5.0%
5.3%
13.7%
47%
SES socioeconomic status
ADHD symptom scales were log-transformed
a Baseline model: fetal growth + sex
The next two steps of prenatal factors (pregnancy complications; maternal substance-use) explained an additional 12.7% of the fetal growth effect in ABCD but an additional 25.4% of the effect in GUI. This cohort-difference was predominantly driven by the higher proportion accounted for by maternal substance use in GUI (22.7%) compared to ABCD (4.8%; Table 2; Fig. 2).
The baseline model, including just fetal growth and sex accounted for ~ 3% of the variance in ADHD symptom scores (ABCD R2 = 2.9%; GUI R2 = 3.7%) while the fully-adjusted model (M4) accounted for 8–10% (ABCD R2 = 9.6%; GUI R2 = 8.3%). In ABCD, the most reliable independent predictors of ADHD symptoms were male sex (t = 15.19, ηp2 = 0.03), parental depression/anxiety (t = 10.84, ηp2 = 0.02) and the number of pregnancy complications experienced (t = 7.01, ηp2 = 0.01; Table S3). The most reliable independent predictors of ADHD symptoms in GUI were male sex (t = 15.58, ηp2 = 0.04), parental depression/anxiety (t = 7.45, ηp2 = 0.01) and younger maternal age (t = -7.54, ηp2 = 0.01; Table S5).
An ordinal logistic regression predicting groups of varying ADHD symptom severity (low, moderate, high, very high) was performed to test statistical robustness of linear models. Results were similar regardless of whether linear or ordinal models were used (Table S2; Fig. S4)— changes to the fetal growth effect estimate due to each step of control variables were similar (± 2%) to the linear model with one exception. In ABCD (US), the total proportion accounted for was greater when predicting symptom groups (ordinal 50% vs linear 43%). This was driven by the greater change-in-estimate due to control of socioeconomics/demographic factors in the ordinal model (31.3%) compared to the linear model (25.4%; Table S2).

Discussion

Results from two independent cohorts suggest the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms at age 9 can be explained in part by sociodemographic confounds and prenatal events. Across both cohorts, over 25% of the association was attributable to familial confounds (socioeconomics, demographics, family psychiatric history) and the significance of the association survived adjustment for familial confounds but not prenatal factors. However, results differed by cohort regarding the relative contributions of socioeconomic/demographic factors (ABCD > GUI) and prenatal factors (GUI > ABCD) to the effect of fetal growth on ADHD symptoms (Fig. 2; Fig. S4). Fetal growth, as estimated from weight and age at birth, was a relatively context-independent predictor of ADHD symptoms while the explanatory or driving factors of the association were somewhat context-dependent.
Averaging cohort results (Table 2), 23.4% of the association between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms was captured by socioeconomic and demographic factors such as household income, race/ethnicity, parental age, education and single parenthood. This is consistent with other studies which have found the fetal growth (or small-for-gestational-age) effect on ADHD outcomes attenuates after controlling for factors such as maternal age, household income, parental education or single-parenthood (Murray et al., 2016; Pettersson et al., 2019; Wiles et al., 2006). The decrease in fetal growth effect after control of socioeconomic and demographic factors was slightly greater in ABCD (25.4%) compared to GUI (21.3%), however this cohort-discrepancy widened in an ordinal sensitivity analysis (ABCD 31.3% Vs GUI 20.5%; Table S2). This disparity may be explained by national differences in the correlation between socioeconomic factors and fetal growth. Martinson and colleagues (2016) found that income-related inequalities in low birth weight rates were present in the US, the United Kingdom, Canada and Australia, but that the magnitude of this gradient was greatest in the US. The authors suggested that the more generous social support and healthcare systems of the UK, Canada and Australia may play a buffering role. Differences may also be explained by the greater racial and ethnic heterogeneity in ABCD (Table 1) and the greater correlation between certain race/ethnicities and fetal growth in ABCD compared to GUI (Figs. S2-S3).
Pregnancy complications accounted for 5.3% of the fetal growth effect on ADHD symptoms on average, with a larger proportion accounted for in ABCD (7.9%) than GUI (2.7%). Ordinal models approximated this cohort difference (9.9% vs 3.2%). The mean number of pregnancy complications in mothers across cohorts was similar across cohorts (Table 1) however the independent effect of pregnancy complications on ADHD symptoms was stronger in ABCD compared to GUI (Tables S3, S5). This suggests that, despite being similarly prevalent across cohorts, pregnancy complications have more adverse effects on both fetal growth and childhood behavior in ABCD compared to GUI. While we did not assess interactive effects between pregnancy complications and maternal age, the younger age of ABCD mothers compared to those in GUI may explain the difference in results (Table 1). For instance, the association between preeclampsia and small-for-gestational age is stronger for mothers under 25 compared to over 25 (Li et al., 2018). Alternatively, this result may be explained by unmeasured cohort differences such as in maternal weight status, stress access to prenatal care, or quality of care (Bronstein et al., 2018; Fuchs et al., 2022). Rates of obesity, for instance, are higher among women in the US compared to women in comparable countries, and this may interact multiplicatively with certain pregnancy complications to increase the risk of both restricted fetal growth and neurodevelopmental issues (Bronstein et al., 2018; Kong et al., 2020).
Perhaps the most striking aspect of Fig. 2 is the greater proportion of the effect accounted for by maternal substance-use in GUI (22.7%) compared to ABCD (4.8%). This is despite similar reported rates of smoking, and lower rates of alcohol and drug-use among GUI mothers (Table 1). GUI mothers smoked more cigarettes and more persistently than ABCD mothers, smoking ~ 9 cigarettes per day on average throughout pregnancy (Table S8). Maternal smoking was also a significant independent predictor of ADHD symptoms in the GUI, but not ABCD. By contrast, maternal alcohol-use and drug-use during pregnancy were significant independent predictors of symptoms in ABCD, but not in GUI (Tables S3, S5). Our findings are consistent with another analysis of the GUI data which showed strong links between maternal smoking and intrauterine growth restriction in Ireland (Madden, 2014), and support the need for improved smoking cessation programs in Irish maternal hospitals (Reynolds et al., 2017). There has been some evidence that the association between smoking in pregnancy and offspring ADHD is not causal and is fully accounted for by shared genetic factors between mother and child (Rice et al., 2018). Such logic could also be applied to other types of substance-use in pregnancy. However, there is evidence supporting the causal association between maternal smoking in pregnancy and birth weight (Rice et al., 2018). Given reliable associations between fetal growth and ADHD symptoms have been observed in human and animal studies (see introduction), it may be that maternal substance-use in pregnancy, such as smoking, impacts child neurodevelopment via fetal growth restriction (Brannigan et al., 2020).
Our results suggest such prenatal factors can capture up to a quarter of the birth weight effect on ADHD symptoms; however future studies will need to assess, in practice, whether reduction in prenatal risks have any tangible effect on childhood ADHD symptoms at a population level. Effect sizes were small– Table 2 indicated that fetal growth accounted for less than 0.5% of variance in ADHD symptoms across all models (all ηp2 < 0.005) and supplementary tables showed that fully-adjusted models explained just 8–10% of variance in outcomes (ABCD R2 = 9.6%; GUI R2 = 8.3%; Tables S3, S5). A meta-analysis suggested birth weight accounted for 2.25% in the variance in ADHD symptoms (r = -0.15; Momany et al., 2018). While the effect size observed in this study was smaller, this does not necessarily invalidate the clinical relevance of our findings. First, even well-established risk factors for ADHD such as male sex and parental psychopathology had small effect sizes (ηp2 < 0.05). This could be explained by our large sample sizes and inclusion of normative symptom scales rather than groups from each end of the spectrum (Kühberger et al., 2014). Second, we know from animal experimentation (Lauritz et al., 2012; Meyer et al., 2014), human meta-analysis (Momany et al., 2018) and multiple twin studies (Ficks et al., 2013; Groen-Blokhuis et al., 2011; Hultman et al., 2007; Pettersson et al., 2015) that fetal growth is a highly replicable predictor of neurodevelopmental problems, even when genetic and social confounds are controlled for. Third, other large cohort studies have showed birth weight accounts for < 1% of the variance in ADHD symptom dimensions (Ficks et al., 2013; van Mil et al., 2015). Finally, the importance of studying prenatal risks to neurodevelopment lies in their temporal precedence over all postnatal risk factors. Small deviations from typical neurodevelopment (reflected by small effect sizes) at an early stage of development have the potential to moderate the effects of all subsequent insults.
The primary strength of this study is its use of two large nationally-representative cohorts. The matched analysis on both cohorts helps determine which findings are generalizable and which may be cohort- or nation-specific. While Ireland and the US are both developed countries, they differ on demographic make-up, healthcare systems, policies and culture. As some of these largely unmeasured factors may confound the association between fetal growth restriction and childhood psychopathology, replicating the association across cohorts can be considered further support for a causal association (Murray et al., 2016). Other strengths include: thorough control of potential confounds, the use of alternative statistical modelling to probe the robustness of findings (ordinal regression), and the specificity of our target population to children aged 9–10 born between 2007 and 2009.
Several aspects of the study design limit interpretation of results. First, comparability of cohort results is limited by differences in outcome scales (CBCL vs SDQ), in definitions of sociodemographic and family psychiatric variables (see Supplementary Material) and in the gestational age range captured (capped at 40 weeks in ABCD). Future studies assessing the generalizability of prenatal contributions to fetal growth and ADHD symptoms should use samples with better matched data, merged into one analysis, to quantitively assess the significance of cohort differences. Second, in ABCD we rely on the retrospective report of gestational and birth events which may be influenced by recall bias (9 years on). In both cohorts, we rely on the mother to report to provide both exposure and outcome data, which may be biased. Third, the study was conducted in singleton-born children thus results are not applicable to twins. Finally, there may be unmeasured sources of confounding such as migrant status, neighborhood poverty, maternal health, weight and stress levels.

Conclusions

We found replicable evidence across Irish and US-based cohorts for the linear association between reduced fetal growth and increased ADHD symptoms by age 9. Background familial confounds (e.g. income, race/ethnicity, family psychiatric history) accounted for over a quarter of that association in both cohorts, however it remained statistically significant. Pregnancy complications and maternal substance-use accounted for different proportions of the fetal growth effect across cohorts, which may reflect societal and cultural differences.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethics Approval

The ABCD and GUI studies were performed in line with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The University of California at San Diego (San Diego, CA, USA) Institutional Review Board was responsible for the ethical oversight of the ABCD study. Ethical approval for GUI was granted by the research ethics committee of the Health Research Board in Ireland. The secondary analysis of both datasets was approved by the Research Ethics Committee for the Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland (REC1693).
Informed consent for children to participate in the GUI and ABCD studies was obtained from their legal guardians.
Informed consent was given to publish study findings from which the identities of children could not be ascertained.

Conflict of Interests

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment Burlington (p. 1617). University of Vermont. Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment Burlington (p. 1617). University of Vermont. Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.
go back to reference Aras, R. Y. (2013). Is maternal age risk factor for low birth weight? Archives of Medicine and Health Sciences, 1(1), 33.CrossRef Aras, R. Y. (2013). Is maternal age risk factor for low birth weight? Archives of Medicine and Health Sciences, 1(1), 33.CrossRef
go back to reference Brannigan, R., Healy, C., Cannon, M., Leacy, F. P., & Clarke, M. C. (2020). Prenatal tobacco exposure and psychiatric outcomes in adolescence: is the effect mediated through birth weight? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 142(4), 284–293. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13210 Brannigan, R., Healy, C., Cannon, M., Leacy, F. P., & Clarke, M. C. (2020). Prenatal tobacco exposure and psychiatric outcomes in adolescence: is the effect mediated through birth weight? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 142(4), 284–293. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​acps.​13210
go back to reference Budree, S., Stein, D. J., Brittain, K., Goddard, E., Koen, N., Barnett, W., Myer, L., & Zar, H. J. (2017). Maternal and infant factors had a significant impact on birthweight and longitudinal growth in a South African birth cohort. Acta Paediatrica, 106(11), 1793–1801. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.14015 Budree, S., Stein, D. J., Brittain, K., Goddard, E., Koen, N., Barnett, W., Myer, L., & Zar, H. J. (2017). Maternal and infant factors had a significant impact on birthweight and longitudinal growth in a South African birth cohort. Acta Paediatrica, 106(11), 1793–1801. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​apa.​14015
go back to reference Ficks, C. A., Lahey, B. B., & Waldman, I. D. (2013). Does low birth weight share common genetic or environmental risk with childhood disruptive disorders? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 842.CrossRef Ficks, C. A., Lahey, B. B., & Waldman, I. D. (2013). Does low birth weight share common genetic or environmental risk with childhood disruptive disorders? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 842.CrossRef
go back to reference Franz, A. P., Bolat, G. U., Bolat, H., Matijasevich, A., Santos, I. S., Silveira, R. C., Procianoy, R. S., Rohde, L. A., & Moreira-Maia, C. R. (2018). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and very preterm/very low birth weight: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 141(1). Franz, A. P., Bolat, G. U., Bolat, H., Matijasevich, A., Santos, I. S., Silveira, R. C., Procianoy, R. S., Rohde, L. A., & Moreira-Maia, C. R. (2018). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and very preterm/very low birth weight: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 141(1).
go back to reference Fuchs, A., Resch, F., Kaess, M., & Moehler, E. (2022). Early parenting stress links obstetric complications and child psychopathology in middle childhood in an at-risk sample. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 43(1), e48–e55.CrossRef Fuchs, A., Resch, F., Kaess, M., & Moehler, E. (2022). Early parenting stress links obstetric complications and child psychopathology in middle childhood in an at-risk sample. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 43(1), e48–e55.CrossRef
go back to reference Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R., Heeringa, S., Jernigan, T., Potter, A., Thompson, W., & Zahs, D. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 16–22.CrossRef Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R., Heeringa, S., Jernigan, T., Potter, A., Thompson, W., & Zahs, D. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 16–22.CrossRef
go back to reference Giscombé, C. L., & Lobel, M. (2005). Explaining disproportionately high rates of adverse birth outcomes among African Americans: The impact of stress, racism, and related factors in pregnancy. Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 662.CrossRef Giscombé, C. L., & Lobel, M. (2005). Explaining disproportionately high rates of adverse birth outcomes among African Americans: The impact of stress, racism, and related factors in pregnancy. Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 662.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.CrossRef Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.CrossRef
go back to reference Groen-Blokhuis, M. M., Middeldorp, C. M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2011). Evidence for a causal association of low birth weight and attention problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(12), 1247–1254 e1242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.09.007 Groen-Blokhuis, M. M., Middeldorp, C. M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2011). Evidence for a causal association of low birth weight and attention problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(12), 1247–1254 e1242. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​jaac.​2011.​09.​007
go back to reference He, Y., Chen, J., Zhu, L.-H., Hua, L.-L., & Ke, F.-F. (2020). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD: Results from a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24(12), 1637–1647.CrossRef He, Y., Chen, J., Zhu, L.-H., Hua, L.-L., & Ke, F.-F. (2020). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD: Results from a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24(12), 1637–1647.CrossRef
go back to reference Heaman, M., Kingston, D., Chalmers, B., Sauve, R., Lee, L., & Young, D. (2013). Risk Factors for Preterm Birth and Small-for-gestational-age Births among Canadian Women. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 27(1), 54–61.CrossRef Heaman, M., Kingston, D., Chalmers, B., Sauve, R., Lee, L., & Young, D. (2013). Risk Factors for Preterm Birth and Small-for-gestational-age Births among Canadian Women. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 27(1), 54–61.CrossRef
go back to reference Huhdanpaa, H., Morales-Munoz, I., Aronen, E. T., Polkki, P., Saarenpaa-Heikkila, O., Kylliainen, A., & Paavonen, E. J. (2021). Prenatal and Postnatal Predictive Factors for Children's Inattentive and Hyperactive Symptoms at 5 Years of Age: The Role of Early Family-related Factors. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 52(5), 783–799. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-020-01057-7 Huhdanpaa, H., Morales-Munoz, I., Aronen, E. T., Polkki, P., Saarenpaa-Heikkila, O., Kylliainen, A., & Paavonen, E. J. (2021). Prenatal and Postnatal Predictive Factors for Children's Inattentive and Hyperactive Symptoms at 5 Years of Age: The Role of Early Family-related Factors. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 52(5), 783–799. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10578-020-01057-7
go back to reference Hultman, C. M., Torrang, A., Tuvblad, C., Cnattingius, S., Larsson, J. O., & Lichtenstein, P. (2007). Birth weight and attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptoms in childhood and early adolescence: a prospective Swedish twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(3), 370–377. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000246059.62706.22 Hultman, C. M., Torrang, A., Tuvblad, C., Cnattingius, S., Larsson, J. O., & Lichtenstein, P. (2007). Birth weight and attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptoms in childhood and early adolescence: a prospective Swedish twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(3), 370–377. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1097/​01.​chi.​0000246059.​62706.​22
go back to reference Kong, L., Chen, X., Gissler, M., & Lavebratt, C. (2020). Relationship of prenatal maternal obesity and diabetes to offspring neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders: A narrative review. International Journal of Obesity, 44(10), 1981–2000.CrossRef Kong, L., Chen, X., Gissler, M., & Lavebratt, C. (2020). Relationship of prenatal maternal obesity and diabetes to offspring neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders: A narrative review. International Journal of Obesity, 44(10), 1981–2000.CrossRef
go back to reference Lærum, A. M., Reitan, S. K., Evensen, K. A. I., Lydersen, S., Brubakk, A.-M., Skranes, J., & Indredavik, M. S. (2017). Psychiatric disorders and general functioning in low birth weight adults: a longitudinal study. Pediatrics, e20162135. Lærum, A. M., Reitan, S. K., Evensen, K. A. I., Lydersen, S., Brubakk, A.-M., Skranes, J., & Indredavik, M. S. (2017). Psychiatric disorders and general functioning in low birth weight adults: a longitudinal study. Pediatrics, e20162135.
go back to reference Laplante, F., Brake, W. G., Chehab, S. L., & Sullivan, R. M. (2012). Sex differences in the effects of perinatal anoxia on dopamine function in rats. Neuroscience Letters, 506(1), 89–93.CrossRef Laplante, F., Brake, W. G., Chehab, S. L., & Sullivan, R. M. (2012). Sex differences in the effects of perinatal anoxia on dopamine function in rats. Neuroscience Letters, 506(1), 89–93.CrossRef
go back to reference Lauritz, B., Siebel, A., Guille, V., Jefferies, A., & Wlodek, M. (2012). Growth restriction alters adult spatial memory and sensorimotor gating in a sex-specific manner. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, 3(1), 59–68.CrossRef Lauritz, B., Siebel, A., Guille, V., Jefferies, A., & Wlodek, M. (2012). Growth restriction alters adult spatial memory and sensorimotor gating in a sex-specific manner. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, 3(1), 59–68.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, X., Zhang, W., Lin, J., Liu, H., Yang, Z., Teng, Y., Duan, S., Li, Y., Xie, Y., Lin, X., Xie, L., Peng, Q., Huang, J., Chen, J., Duan, W., Luo, J., & Zhang, J. (2018). Preterm birth, low birthweight, and small for gestational age among women with preeclampsia: Does maternal age matter? Pregnancy Hypertension, 13, 260–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.preghy.2018.07.004 Li, X., Zhang, W., Lin, J., Liu, H., Yang, Z., Teng, Y., Duan, S., Li, Y., Xie, Y., Lin, X., Xie, L., Peng, Q., Huang, J., Chen, J., Duan, W., Luo, J., & Zhang, J. (2018). Preterm birth, low birthweight, and small for gestational age among women with preeclampsia: Does maternal age matter? Pregnancy Hypertension, 13, 260–266. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​preghy.​2018.​07.​004
go back to reference Lim, K. X., Liu, C. Y., Schoeler, T., Cecil, C. A., Barker, E. D., Viding, E., Greven, C. U., & Pingault, J. B. (2018). The role of birth weight on the causal pathway to child and adolescent ADHD symptomatology: A population-based twin differences longitudinal design. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(10), 1036–1043.CrossRef Lim, K. X., Liu, C. Y., Schoeler, T., Cecil, C. A., Barker, E. D., Viding, E., Greven, C. U., & Pingault, J. B. (2018). The role of birth weight on the causal pathway to child and adolescent ADHD symptomatology: A population-based twin differences longitudinal design. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(10), 1036–1043.CrossRef
go back to reference McCowan, L., & Horgan, R. P. (2009). Risk factors for small for gestational age infants. Best Practice & Research: Clinical Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 23(6), 779–793. McCowan, L., & Horgan, R. P. (2009). Risk factors for small for gestational age infants. Best Practice & Research: Clinical Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 23(6), 779–793.
go back to reference Milberger, S., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Guite, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1997). Pregnancy, delivery and infancy complications and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Issues of gene-environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 41(1), 65–75.CrossRef Milberger, S., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Guite, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1997). Pregnancy, delivery and infancy complications and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Issues of gene-environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 41(1), 65–75.CrossRef
go back to reference Mongan, D., Lynch, J., Hanna, D., Shannon, C., Hamilton, S., Potter, C., Gorman, C., McCambridge, O., Morrow, R., & Mulholland, C. (2019). Prevalence of self-reported mental disorders in pregnancy and associations with adverse neonatal outcomes: A population-based cross-sectional study. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 19(1), 1–10.CrossRef Mongan, D., Lynch, J., Hanna, D., Shannon, C., Hamilton, S., Potter, C., Gorman, C., McCambridge, O., Morrow, R., & Mulholland, C. (2019). Prevalence of self-reported mental disorders in pregnancy and associations with adverse neonatal outcomes: A population-based cross-sectional study. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 19(1), 1–10.CrossRef
go back to reference Murray, E., Pearson, R., Fernandes, M., Santos, I. S., Barros, F. C., Victora, C. G., Stein, A., & Matijasevich, A. (2016). Are fetal growth impairment and preterm birth causally related to child attention problems and ADHD? Evidence from a comparison between high-income and middle-income cohorts. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 70(7), 704–709. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2015-206222 Murray, E., Pearson, R., Fernandes, M., Santos, I. S., Barros, F. C., Victora, C. G., Stein, A., & Matijasevich, A. (2016). Are fetal growth impairment and preterm birth causally related to child attention problems and ADHD? Evidence from a comparison between high-income and middle-income cohorts. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 70(7), 704–709. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1136/​jech-2015-206222
go back to reference Pettersson, E., Sjölander, A., Almqvist, C., Anckarsäter, H., D’Onofrio, B. M., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, H. (2015). Birth weight as an independent predictor of ADHD symptoms: A within-twin pair analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(4), 453–459. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12299CrossRef Pettersson, E., Sjölander, A., Almqvist, C., Anckarsäter, H., D’Onofrio, B. M., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, H. (2015). Birth weight as an independent predictor of ADHD symptoms: A within-twin pair analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(4), 453–459. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​12299CrossRef
go back to reference Quail, A., Williams, J., McCrory, C., Murray, A., & Thornton, M. (2011). A summary guide to wave 1 of the infant cohort (at 9 months) of Growing Up in Ireland. Quail, A., Williams, J., McCrory, C., Murray, A., & Thornton, M. (2011). A summary guide to wave 1 of the infant cohort (at 9 months) of Growing Up in Ireland.
go back to reference Reynolds, C. M. E., Egan, B., Cawley, S., Kennedy, R., Sheehan, S., & Turner, M. J. (2017). A national audit of smoking cessation services in Irish maternity units. Irish Medical Journal, 110(6). Reynolds, C. M. E., Egan, B., Cawley, S., Kennedy, R., Sheehan, S., & Turner, M. J. (2017). A national audit of smoking cessation services in Irish maternity units. Irish Medical Journal, 110(6).
go back to reference Rice, F., Langley, K., Woodford, C., Davey Smith, G., & Thapar, A. (2018). Identifying the contribution of prenatal risk factors to offspring development and psychopathology: What designs to use and a critique of literature on maternal smoking and stress in pregnancy. Development and Psychopathology, 30(3), 1107–1128. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000421 Rice, F., Langley, K., Woodford, C., Davey Smith, G., & Thapar, A. (2018). Identifying the contribution of prenatal risk factors to offspring development and psychopathology: What designs to use and a critique of literature on maternal smoking and stress in pregnancy. Development and Psychopathology, 30(3), 1107–1128. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S095457941800042​1
go back to reference Rice, F., Lewis, A., Harold, G., van den Bree, M., Boivin, J., Hay, D. F., Owen, M. J., & Thapar, A. (2007). Agreement between maternal report and antenatal records for a range of pre and peri-natal factors: The influence of maternal and child characteristics. Early Human Development, 83(8), 497–504.CrossRef Rice, F., Lewis, A., Harold, G., van den Bree, M., Boivin, J., Hay, D. F., Owen, M. J., & Thapar, A. (2007). Agreement between maternal report and antenatal records for a range of pre and peri-natal factors: The influence of maternal and child characteristics. Early Human Development, 83(8), 497–504.CrossRef
go back to reference Russell, A. E., Ford, T., Williams, R., & Russell, G. (2016). The association between socioeconomic disadvantage and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A systematic review. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 47(3), 440–458.CrossRef Russell, A. E., Ford, T., Williams, R., & Russell, G. (2016). The association between socioeconomic disadvantage and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A systematic review. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 47(3), 440–458.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwenke, E., Fasching, P. A., Faschingbauer, F., Pretscher, J., Kehl, S., Peretz, R., Keller, A., Haberle, L., Eichler, A., Irlbauer-Muller, V., Dammer, U., Beckmann, M. W., & Schneider, M. (2018). Predicting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using pregnancy and birth characteristics. Archives of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 298(5), 889–895. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00404-018-4888-0 Schwenke, E., Fasching, P. A., Faschingbauer, F., Pretscher, J., Kehl, S., Peretz, R., Keller, A., Haberle, L., Eichler, A., Irlbauer-Muller, V., Dammer, U., Beckmann, M. W., & Schneider, M. (2018). Predicting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using pregnancy and birth characteristics. Archives of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 298(5), 889–895. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00404-018-4888-0
go back to reference Sciberras, E., Ukoumunne, O. C., & Efron, D. (2011, Oct). Predictors of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children aged 6–7 years: a national longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(7), 1025–1034. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9504-8 Sciberras, E., Ukoumunne, O. C., & Efron, D. (2011, Oct). Predictors of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children aged 6–7 years: a national longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(7), 1025–1034. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-011-9504-8
go back to reference Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C. M. E., Vermulst, A. A., & Janssens, J. M. A. M. (2010). Psychometric Properties of the Parent and Teacher Versions of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire for 4- to 12-Year-Olds: A Review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13(3), 254–274. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2 Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C. M. E., Vermulst, A. A., & Janssens, J. M. A. M. (2010). Psychometric Properties of the Parent and Teacher Versions of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire for 4- to 12-Year-Olds: A Review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13(3), 254–274. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10567-010-0071-2
go back to reference Thornton, M., Williams, J., McCrory, C., Murray, A., & Quail, A. (2013). Growing up in Ireland. Design, instrumentation and procedures for the infant cohort at wave one (9 months). Department of Children and Youth Affairs. Thornton, M., Williams, J., McCrory, C., Murray, A., & Quail, A. (2013). Growing up in Ireland. Design, instrumentation and procedures for the infant cohort at wave one (9 months). Department of Children and Youth Affairs.
go back to reference van Mil, N. H., Steegers-Theunissen, R. P., Motazedi, E., Jansen, P. W., Jaddoe, V. W., Steegers, E. A., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2015). Low and high birth weight and the risk of child attention problems. The Journal of pediatrics, 166(4), 862–869 e861–863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2014.12.075 van Mil, N. H., Steegers-Theunissen, R. P., Motazedi, E., Jansen, P. W., Jaddoe, V. W., Steegers, E. A., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2015). Low and high birth weight and the risk of child attention problems. The Journal of pediatrics, 166(4), 862–869 e861–863. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​jpeds.​2014.​12.​075
go back to reference Wadhwa, P. D., Entringer, S., Buss, C., & Lu, M. C. (2011). The contribution of maternal stress to preterm birth: Issues and considerations. Clinics in Perinatology, 38(3), 351–384.CrossRef Wadhwa, P. D., Entringer, S., Buss, C., & Lu, M. C. (2011). The contribution of maternal stress to preterm birth: Issues and considerations. Clinics in Perinatology, 38(3), 351–384.CrossRef
go back to reference Wiles, N. J., Peters, T. J., Heron, J., Gunnell, D., Emond, A., & Lewis, G. (2006). Fetal growth and childhood behavioral problems: results from the ALSPAC cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology, 163(9), 829–837. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwj108 Wiles, N. J., Peters, T. J., Heron, J., Gunnell, D., Emond, A., & Lewis, G. (2006). Fetal growth and childhood behavioral problems: results from the ALSPAC cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology, 163(9), 829–837. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​aje/​kwj108
go back to reference Willoughby, M., Williams, J., Mills-Koonce, W., & Blair, C. (2020). Early life predictors of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptomatology profiles from early through middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 32(3), 791–802. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001135 Willoughby, M., Williams, J., Mills-Koonce, W., & Blair, C. (2020). Early life predictors of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptomatology profiles from early through middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 32(3), 791–802. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S095457941900113​5
Metagegevens
Titel
Explaining the Association Between Fetal Growth and Childhood ADHD Symptoms: Cross-cohort Replication
Auteurs
Niamh Dooley
Colm Healy
Ross Brannigan
David Cotter
Mary Clarke
Mary Cannon
Publicatiedatum
17-09-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology / Uitgave 2/2023
Print ISSN: 2730-7166
Elektronisch ISSN: 2730-7174
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-022-00971-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2023

Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 2/2023 Naar de uitgave