Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2023

26-05-2022 | Original Article

Emotions in motion: affective valence can influence compatibility effects with graspable objects

Auteurs: Elisa Scerrati, Sandro Rubichi, Roberto Nicoletti, Cristina Iani

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2023

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Previous studies showed that affective valence (positive, negative) influences Stimulus–Response Compatibility (SRC) effects elicited by both relevant and irrelevant spatial dimensions. We tested whether valence influences SRC effects when the irrelevant spatial dimension rather than being conveyed by the entire stimulus location is conveyed by the location of the stimulus’ graspable part, i.e., the Handle–Response (H–R) compatibility effect. Participants saw objects with either a flower, a spider or nothing on their handle and categorized them as kitchen utensils or garage tools through button presses. In Experiment 1, a random presentation of valenced stimuli was used, whereas in Experiment 2 differently valenced stimuli were arranged in different blocks. Furthermore, participants in Experiment 2 could be spider-fearful or not. In Experiment 1, an H–R compatibility effect occurred for response latencies, regardless of whether stimuli presented a negative, positive or no element on their handle. In Experiment 2 the effect occurred only when a positive element was shown on the object's handle. In addition, spider-fearful individuals showed significantly slower responses when the element appearing on the object's handle had a negative valence. These results suggest that the SRC effect observed with pictures of graspable objects may be sensitive to the affective characteristics of stimuli and that approach/avoidance response tendencies may also depend on individual differences (being spider-fearful or not).
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Since we did not use any validated questionnaire, the information concerning spider-fear in Experiment 1 is not used in the analysis as no reliable measure of spider-fear was obtained.
 
2
Since several studies showed the importance of manual dominance in S–R compatibility effects (e.g., Song et al., 2017; Fagard & Ittyerah, 2017; Freddi et al., 2016) and given that both experiments involved a fair amount of ambidextrous and left-handed participants (21% and 12% in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively) we tested whether Handedness had an effect on response times by performing a LMM separately for each experiment where Handedness (righ-handed, left-handed, ambidextrous) was added as a fixed effect. Neither experiment showed a main effect of Handedness (p = 0.895 and p = 0.557 in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively), nor an interaction between Handedness and H–R compatibility (p = 0.452 and p = 0.284 in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively), indicating that Handedness did not influence response times. In order to test the robustness of the results observed with the full samples of participants, further analyses were conducted for each experiment separately on the sample of right-handed participants alone. Analyses from both experiments confirmed the results obtained with the full samples. Specifically, Experiment 1 (N = 43 right-handed) showed a main effect of H–R compatibility, p < 0.05, with slightly faster response latencies for H–R compatible (M: 626 ms; SE: 14.9) than incompatible (M: 630 ms; SE: 14.9) trials, whereas Experiment 2 (N = 47 right-handed) showed a significant interaction between Affective Valence and H–R compatibility (p = 0.008) with significantly faster H–R compatible (M: 609 ms; SD: 74.87) than incompatible trials (M: 619 ms; SD: 83.20) only when the element appearing on the graspable handle was positive (i.e., flower), t(46) = 2.25, p < 0.05, and a significant interaction between Affective Valence and Group (p < 0.001) with slower mean response times for objects featuring a negative element (M: 633 ms; SD: 111.41) on the graspable handle than objects featuring a positive (M: 620 ms; SD: 98.05) and no element (M: 620 ms; SD: 105.29) on the handle for spider-fearful participants only, t(22) = 2.34, p < 0.05 and t(22) = 2.09, p < 0.05, respectively.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2012). Grasping the pain: Motor resonance with dangerous affordances. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1627–1639.PubMedCrossRef Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2012). Grasping the pain: Motor resonance with dangerous affordances. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1627–1639.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Anelli, F., Nicoletti, R., Bolzani, R., & Borghi, A. M. (2013). Keep away from danger: Dangerous objects in dynamic and static situations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 344.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Anelli, F., Nicoletti, R., Bolzani, R., & Borghi, A. M. (2013). Keep away from danger: Dangerous objects in dynamic and static situations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 344.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Anwyl-Irvine, A. L., Massonnié, J., Flitton, A., Kirkham, N., & Evershed, J. K. (2020). Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods, 52(1), 388–407.PubMedCrossRef Anwyl-Irvine, A. L., Massonnié, J., Flitton, A., Kirkham, N., & Evershed, J. K. (2020). Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods, 52(1), 388–407.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.CrossRef Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.CrossRef
go back to reference Basanovic, J., Dean, L., Riskind, J. H., & MacLeod, C. (2019). High spider-fearful and low spider-fearful individuals differentially perceive the speed of approaching, but not receding, spider stimuli. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(2), 514–521.CrossRef Basanovic, J., Dean, L., Riskind, J. H., & MacLeod, C. (2019). High spider-fearful and low spider-fearful individuals differentially perceive the speed of approaching, but not receding, spider stimuli. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(2), 514–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298.PubMedCrossRef Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: Behavior, feeling, and physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 242–276). Oxford University Press. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: Behavior, feeling, and physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 242–276). Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Buccino, G., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Rodà, F., & Riggio, L. (2009). Broken affordances, broken objects: A TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3074–3078.PubMedCrossRef Buccino, G., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Rodà, F., & Riggio, L. (2009). Broken affordances, broken objects: A TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3074–3078.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224.CrossRef Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224.CrossRef
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 853–861.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 853–861.PubMed
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 737.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 737.PubMed
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: Evidence for a basis in spatial coding. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 77(5), 618–636.PubMedCrossRef Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: Evidence for a basis in spatial coding. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 77(5), 618–636.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conde, E. F. Q., Jazenko, F., Fraga Filho, R. S., da Costa, D. H., Torro-Alves, N., Cavallet, M., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2011). Stimulus affective valence reverses spatial compatibility effect. Psychology & Neuroscience, 4, 81–87.CrossRef Conde, E. F. Q., Jazenko, F., Fraga Filho, R. S., da Costa, D. H., Torro-Alves, N., Cavallet, M., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2011). Stimulus affective valence reverses spatial compatibility effect. Psychology & Neuroscience, 4, 81–87.CrossRef
go back to reference Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1998). Visuomotor priming. Visual Cognition, 5, 109–125.CrossRef Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1998). Visuomotor priming. Visual Cognition, 5, 109–125.CrossRef
go back to reference Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of consciousness. Vintage Books. Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of consciousness. Vintage Books.
go back to reference De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 189–206.CrossRef De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 189–206.CrossRef
go back to reference Eder, A. B., & Hommel, B. (2013). Anticipatory control of approach and avoidance: An ideomotor approach. Emotion Review, 5, 275–279.CrossRef Eder, A. B., & Hommel, B. (2013). Anticipatory control of approach and avoidance: An ideomotor approach. Emotion Review, 5, 275–279.CrossRef
go back to reference Ellis, R. (2018). Bodies and other objects: The sensorimotor foundations of cognition (p. I–II). Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Ellis, R. (2018). Bodies and other objects: The sensorimotor foundations of cognition (p. I–II). Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fini, C., Fischer, M., Bardi, L., Brass, M., & Moors, A. (2020). Support from a TMS/MEP study for a direct link between positive/negative stimuli and approach/avoidance tendencies. Neuropsychologia, 143, 107496.PubMedCrossRef Fini, C., Fischer, M., Bardi, L., Brass, M., & Moors, A. (2020). Support from a TMS/MEP study for a direct link between positive/negative stimuli and approach/avoidance tendencies. Neuropsychologia, 143, 107496.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Dahl, C. D. (2007). The time course of visuo-motor affordances. Experimental Brain Research, 176(3), 519–524.PubMedCrossRef Fischer, M. H., & Dahl, C. D. (2007). The time course of visuo-motor affordances. Experimental Brain Research, 176(3), 519–524.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, M., Fini, C., Brass, M., & Moors, A. (2020). Early approach and avoidance tendencies can be goal-directed: Support from a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20, 648–657.CrossRef Fischer, M., Fini, C., Brass, M., & Moors, A. (2020). Early approach and avoidance tendencies can be goal-directed: Support from a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20, 648–657.CrossRef
go back to reference Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMedCrossRef Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Freddi, S., Brouillet, T., Cretenet, J., Heurley, L. P., & Dru, V. (2016). A continuous mapping between space and valence with left-and right-handers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(3), 865–870.CrossRef Freddi, S., Brouillet, T., Cretenet, J., Heurley, L. P., & Dru, V. (2016). A continuous mapping between space and valence with left-and right-handers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(3), 865–870.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Goslin, J., Dixon, T., Fischer, M. H., Cangelosi, A., & Ellis, R. (2012). Electrophysiological examination of embodiment in vision and action. Psychological Science, 23(2), 152–157.PubMedCrossRef Goslin, J., Dixon, T., Fischer, M. H., Cangelosi, A., & Ellis, R. (2012). Electrophysiological examination of embodiment in vision and action. Psychological Science, 23(2), 152–157.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.PubMedCrossRef Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hajcak, G., Moser, J. S., & Simons, R. F. (2006). Attending to affect: Appraisal strategies modulate the electrocortical response to arousing pictures. Emotion, 6(3), 517.PubMedCrossRef Hajcak, G., Moser, J. S., & Simons, R. F. (2006). Attending to affect: Appraisal strategies modulate the electrocortical response to arousing pictures. Emotion, 6(3), 517.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2003). Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 53(6), 494–501.PubMedCrossRef Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2003). Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 53(6), 494–501.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Lippelt, D. P., Gurbuz, E., & Pfister, R. (2017). Contributions of expected sensory and affective action effects to action selection and performance: Evidence from forced-and free-choice tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(3), 821–827.CrossRef Hommel, B., Lippelt, D. P., Gurbuz, E., & Pfister, R. (2017). Contributions of expected sensory and affective action effects to action selection and performance: Evidence from forced-and free-choice tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(3), 821–827.CrossRef
go back to reference Iani, C., Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., & Nicoletti, R. (2011). On the relationship between affordance and Simon effects: Are the effects really independent? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 121–131.CrossRef Iani, C., Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., & Nicoletti, R. (2011). On the relationship between affordance and Simon effects: Are the effects really independent? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 121–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Keightley, M. L., Winocur, G., Graham, S. J., Mayberg, H. S., Hevenor, S. J., & Grady, C. L. (2003). An fMRI study investigating cognitive modulation of brain regions associated with emotional processing of visual stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(5), 585–596.PubMedCrossRef Keightley, M. L., Winocur, G., Graham, S. J., Mayberg, H. S., Hevenor, S. J., & Grady, C. L. (2003). An fMRI study investigating cognitive modulation of brain regions associated with emotional processing of visual stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(5), 585–596.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kozlik, J., Neumann, R., & Lozo, L. (2015). Contrasting motivational orientation and evaluative coding accounts: On the need to differentiate the effectors of approach/avoidance responses. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 563.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kozlik, J., Neumann, R., & Lozo, L. (2015). Contrasting motivational orientation and evaluative coding accounts: On the need to differentiate the effectors of approach/avoidance responses. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 563.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Laham, S. M., Kashima, Y., Dix, J., & Wheeler, M. (2015). A meta-analysis of the facilitation of arm flexion and extension movements as a function of stimulus valence. Cognition and Emotion, 29(6), 1069–1090.PubMedCrossRef Laham, S. M., Kashima, Y., Dix, J., & Wheeler, M. (2015). A meta-analysis of the facilitation of arm flexion and extension movements as a function of stimulus valence. Cognition and Emotion, 29(6), 1069–1090.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lavender, T., & Hommel, B. (2007). Affect and action: Towards an event-coding account. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 1270–1296.CrossRef Lavender, T., & Hommel, B. (2007). Affect and action: Towards an event-coding account. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 1270–1296.CrossRef
go back to reference LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Touchstone. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Touchstone.
go back to reference Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 476–490.CrossRef Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 476–490.CrossRef
go back to reference Markman, A. B., & Brendl, C. M. (2005). Constraining theories of embodied cognition. Psychological Science, 16, 6–10.PubMedCrossRef Markman, A. B., & Brendl, C. M. (2005). Constraining theories of embodied cognition. Psychological Science, 16, 6–10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Michalland, A. H., Thébault, G., Briglia, J., Fraisse, P., & Brouillet, D. (2019). Grasping a Chestnut Burr. Experimental Psychology, 66(4), 310–317.PubMedCrossRef Michalland, A. H., Thébault, G., Briglia, J., Fraisse, P., & Brouillet, D. (2019). Grasping a Chestnut Burr. Experimental Psychology, 66(4), 310–317.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Michalowski, J. M., Melzig, C. A., Weike, A. I., Stockburger, J., Schupp, H. T., & Hamm, A. O. (2009). Brain dynamics in spider-phobic individuals exposed to phobia-relevant and other emotional stimuli. Emotion, 9(3), 306.PubMedCrossRef Michalowski, J. M., Melzig, C. A., Weike, A. I., Stockburger, J., Schupp, H. T., & Hamm, A. O. (2009). Brain dynamics in spider-phobic individuals exposed to phobia-relevant and other emotional stimuli. Emotion, 9(3), 306.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moors, A., Fini, C., Everaert, T., Bardi, L., Bossuyt, E., Kuppens, P., & Brass, M. (2019). The role of stimulus-driven versus goal-directed processes in fight and flight tendencies measured with motor evoked potentials induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS One, 14(5), e0217266.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Moors, A., Fini, C., Everaert, T., Bardi, L., Bossuyt, E., Kuppens, P., & Brass, M. (2019). The role of stimulus-driven versus goal-directed processes in fight and flight tendencies measured with motor evoked potentials induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS One, 14(5), e0217266.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1996). A comparison of two spider fear questionnaires. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27(3), 241–244.PubMedCrossRef Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1996). A comparison of two spider fear questionnaires. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27(3), 241–244.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mustile, M., Giocondo, F., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., & Kourtis, D. (2021). Motor inhibition to dangerous objects: Electrophysiological evidence for task-dependent aversive affordances. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 33(5), 826-839. Mustile, M., Giocondo, F., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., & Kourtis, D. (2021). Motor inhibition to dangerous objects: Electrophysiological evidence for task-dependent aversive affordances. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 33(5), 826-839.
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.PubMedCrossRef Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Osgood, C. (1952). The nature and measurement of meaning. Psychological Bulletin, 49, 172–237.CrossRef Osgood, C. (1952). The nature and measurement of meaning. Psychological Bulletin, 49, 172–237.CrossRef
go back to reference Pellicano, A., & Binkofski, F. (2021). The prominent role of perceptual salience in object discrimination: Overt discrimination of graspable side does not activate grasping affordances. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(3), 1234–1247.PubMedCrossRef Pellicano, A., & Binkofski, F. (2021). The prominent role of perceptual salience in object discrimination: Overt discrimination of graspable side does not activate grasping affordances. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(3), 1234–1247.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pereira, M. G., Volchan, E., de Souza, G. G. L., Oliveira, L., Campagnoli, R. R., Pinheiro, W. M., & Pessoa, L. (2006). Sustained and transient modulation of performance induced by emotional picture viewing. Emotion, 6(4), 622–634. Pereira, M. G., Volchan, E., de Souza, G. G. L., Oliveira, L., Campagnoli, R. R., Pinheiro, W. M., & Pessoa, L. (2006). Sustained and transient modulation of performance induced by emotional picture viewing. Emotion, 6(4), 622–634.
go back to reference Phaf, R. H., Mohr, S. E., Rotteveel, M., & Wicherts, J. M. (2014). Approach, avoidance, and affect: A meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–16.CrossRef Phaf, R. H., Mohr, S. E., Rotteveel, M., & Wicherts, J. M. (2014). Approach, avoidance, and affect: A meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Saccone, E. J., Churches, O., & Nicholls, M. E. (2016). Explicit spatial compatibility is not critical to the object handle effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(10), 1643–1653.PubMed Saccone, E. J., Churches, O., & Nicholls, M. E. (2016). Explicit spatial compatibility is not critical to the object handle effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(10), 1643–1653.PubMed
go back to reference Scerrati, E., & D’Ascenzo, S. (2018). Effetti di corrispondenza con oggetti a funzione implicita versus esplicita. Giornale Italiano Di Psicologia, 45(3), 669–676. Scerrati, E., & D’Ascenzo, S. (2018). Effetti di corrispondenza con oggetti a funzione implicita versus esplicita. Giornale Italiano Di Psicologia, 45(3), 669–676.
go back to reference Scerrati, E., Iani, C., Lugli, L., & Rubichi, S. (2019). C’è un effetto di potenziamento dell’azione con oggetti bimanuali? Giornale Italiano Di Psicologia, 46(4), 987–996. Scerrati, E., Iani, C., Lugli, L., & Rubichi, S. (2019). C’è un effetto di potenziamento dell’azione con oggetti bimanuali? Giornale Italiano Di Psicologia, 46(4), 987–996.
go back to reference Scerrati, E., Iani, C., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Rubichi, S. (2020a). Do my hands prime your hands? The hand-to-response correspondence effect. Acta Psychologica, 203, 1–12. CrossRef Scerrati, E., Iani, C., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Rubichi, S. (2020a). Do my hands prime your hands? The hand-to-response correspondence effect. Acta Psychologica, 203, 1–12. CrossRef
go back to reference Scerrati, E., Iani, C., & Rubichi, S. (2021). Does the activation of motor information affect semantic processing? In L. Bechberger, K.-U. Kühnberger & M. Liu (Eds.), Concepts in Action (pp. 153–166). Springer, Cham. Scerrati, E., Iani, C., & Rubichi, S. (2021). Does the activation of motor information affect semantic processing? In L. Bechberger, K.-U. Kühnberger & M. Liu (Eds.), Concepts in Action (pp. 153–166). Springer, Cham.
go back to reference Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11.CrossRef Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11.CrossRef
go back to reference Scerrati, E., Marzola, G., Villani, C., Lugli, L., & D’Ascenzo, S. (2021). Nuovi scenari per gli esperimenti in psicologia: la modalità online. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia (In press). Scerrati, E., Marzola, G., Villani, C., Lugli, L., & D’Ascenzo, S. (2021). Nuovi scenari per gli esperimenti in psicologia: la modalità online. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia (In press).
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). North-Holland. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). North-Holland.
go back to reference Solarz, A. K. (1960). Latency on instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 239–245.PubMedCrossRef Solarz, A. K. (1960). Latency on instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 239–245.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Song, X., Chen, J., & Proctor, R. W. (2017). Role of hand dominance in mapping preferences for emotional-valence words to keypress responses. Acta Psychologica, 180, 33–39.PubMedCrossRef Song, X., Chen, J., & Proctor, R. W. (2017). Role of hand dominance in mapping preferences for emotional-valence words to keypress responses. Acta Psychologica, 180, 33–39.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Szymanski, J., & O’Donohue, W. (1995). Fear of spiders questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26(1), 31–34.PubMedCrossRef Szymanski, J., & O’Donohue, W. (1995). Fear of spiders questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26(1), 31–34.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 493–498. Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 493–498.
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830–846.PubMed Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830–846.PubMed
go back to reference Vainio, L., Ala-Salomäki, H., Huovilainen, T., Nikkinen, H., Salo, M., Väliaho, J., & Paavilainen, P. (2014). Mug handle affordance and automatic response inhibition: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(9), 1697–1719.PubMedCrossRef Vainio, L., Ala-Salomäki, H., Huovilainen, T., Nikkinen, H., Salo, M., Väliaho, J., & Paavilainen, P. (2014). Mug handle affordance and automatic response inhibition: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(9), 1697–1719.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vainio, L., & Ellis, R. (2020). Action inhibition and affordances associated with a non-target object: An integrative review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 112, 487–502.CrossRef Vainio, L., & Ellis, R. (2020). Action inhibition and affordances associated with a non-target object: An integrative review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 112, 487–502.CrossRef
go back to reference Vainio, L., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2007). The role of visual attention in action priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 241–261.PubMedCrossRef Vainio, L., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2007). The role of visual attention in action priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 241–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Approach and avoidance as action effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1298–1306.CrossRef Van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Approach and avoidance as action effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1298–1306.CrossRef
go back to reference Yamaguchi, M., & Chen, J. (2019). Affective influences without approach/avoidance actions: On the congruence between valence and stimulus-response mappings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 545–551.CrossRef Yamaguchi, M., & Chen, J. (2019). Affective influences without approach/avoidance actions: On the congruence between valence and stimulus-response mappings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 545–551.CrossRef
go back to reference Yamaguchi, M., Chen, J., Mishler, S., & Proctor, R. W. (2018). Flowers and spiders in spatial stimulus-response compatibility: Does affective valence influence selection of task-sets or selection of responses? Cognition and Emotion, 32(5), 1003–1017.PubMedCrossRef Yamaguchi, M., Chen, J., Mishler, S., & Proctor, R. W. (2018). Flowers and spiders in spatial stimulus-response compatibility: Does affective valence influence selection of task-sets or selection of responses? Cognition and Emotion, 32(5), 1003–1017.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRef Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Emotions in motion: affective valence can influence compatibility effects with graspable objects
Auteurs
Elisa Scerrati
Sandro Rubichi
Roberto Nicoletti
Cristina Iani
Publicatiedatum
26-05-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2023
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01688-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2023

Psychological Research 3/2023 Naar de uitgave