Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2021

02-04-2020 | Original Article

Effort avoidance is not simply error avoidance

Auteurs: Iman Feghhi, David A. Rosenbaum

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Little is known about how effort is represented for different kinds of tasks. Recently, we suggested that it would help to establish empirical benchmarks for this problem. Accordingly, Feghhi and Rosenbaum (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45:983–994, 2019) estimated how many additional digits to be memorized corresponded to navigating through a narrow gap versus a wide gap. The estimates were based on a study in which participants chose between walking paths with associated memory demands. We found that participants were equally willing to choose to walk through a narrow gap as to walk through a wide gap when the narrow-gap walk required memorization of 0.55 fewer digits on average than the wide-gap walk. In the present experiment, we sought to replicate and extend this previous finding in two ways: (1) by presenting the memory digits in auditory rather than visual form to test the hypothesis that participants used phonological recoding of the visually presented digits; and (2) by providing a new metric of the relative difficulty of navigation errors compared to recall errors. We provided 36 university students with two action/memorization options per trial and asked them to choose the easier option. Each option had varying degrees of physical demand (walking through a wide or narrow gap) and mental demand (memorizing 6, 7, or 8 digits). We expected performance to be comparable to what we observed earlier with visually presented digits to be memorized, and this prediction was confirmed. We also used a new metric to show that navigation errors were implicitly judged to be 17% more costly than recall errors. The fact that this percentage was not 0 indicates that reducing percent error was not the only basis for reducing effort.
Literatuur
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The Psychology of Memory. New York: Basic Books. Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The Psychology of Memory. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Bhatt, T., Subramaniam, S., & Varghese, R. (2016). Examining interference of different cognitive tasks on voluntary balance control in aging and stroke. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 2575–2584.CrossRef Bhatt, T., Subramaniam, S., & Varghese, R. (2016). Examining interference of different cognitive tasks on voluntary balance control in aging and stroke. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 2575–2584.CrossRef
go back to reference Boureau, Y. L., Sokol-Hessner, P., & Daw, N. D. (2015). Deciding how to decide: Self-control and meta-decision making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 700–710.CrossRef Boureau, Y. L., Sokol-Hessner, P., & Daw, N. D. (2015). Deciding how to decide: Self-control and meta-decision making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 700–710.CrossRef
go back to reference Cavanagh, J. P. (1972). Relation between immediate memory span and memory search rate. Psychological Review, 79, 525–530.CrossRef Cavanagh, J. P. (1972). Relation between immediate memory span and memory search rate. Psychological Review, 79, 525–530.CrossRef
go back to reference Cao, L., & Händel, B. (2019). Walking enhances peripheral visual processing in humans. PLoS Biology, 17(10), e3000511.CrossRef Cao, L., & Händel, B. (2019). Walking enhances peripheral visual processing in humans. PLoS Biology, 17(10), e3000511.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2013). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Routledge.CrossRef Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2013). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Routledge.CrossRef
go back to reference Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429–432.CrossRef Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429–432.CrossRef
go back to reference Craig, A. D. (2013). An interoceptive neuroanatomical perspective on feelings, energy, and effort. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 685–686.CrossRef Craig, A. D. (2013). An interoceptive neuroanatomical perspective on feelings, energy, and effort. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 685–686.CrossRef
go back to reference Duenas, M., Salazar, A., Ojeda, B., Arana, R., & Failde, I. (2016). Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) to handle missing data and time-dependent variables in longitudinal studies: an application to assess the evolution of Health Related Quality of Life in coronary patients. Epidemiologia e Prevenzione, 40, 116–123.PubMed Duenas, M., Salazar, A., Ojeda, B., Arana, R., & Failde, I. (2016). Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) to handle missing data and time-dependent variables in longitudinal studies: an application to assess the evolution of Health Related Quality of Life in coronary patients. Epidemiologia e Prevenzione, 40, 116–123.PubMed
go back to reference Dunn, T. L., Lutes, D. J., & Risko, E. F. (2016). Metacognitive evaluation in the avoidance of demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1372–1387.PubMed Dunn, T. L., Lutes, D. J., & Risko, E. F. (2016). Metacognitive evaluation in the avoidance of demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1372–1387.PubMed
go back to reference Dunn, T. L., Inzlicht, M., & Risko, E. F. (2019). Anticipating cognitive effort: roles of perceived error-likelihood and time demands. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83, 1033–1056.CrossRef Dunn, T. L., Inzlicht, M., & Risko, E. F. (2019). Anticipating cognitive effort: roles of perceived error-likelihood and time demands. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83, 1033–1056.CrossRef
go back to reference Feghhi, I., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2019). Judging the subjective difficulty of different kinds of tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45, 983–994.PubMed Feghhi, I., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2019). Judging the subjective difficulty of different kinds of tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45, 983–994.PubMed
go back to reference Gray, W. D., Sims, C. R., Fu, W. T., & Schoelles, M. J. (2006). The soft constraints hypothesis: A rational analysis approach to resource allocation for interactive behavior. Psychological Review, 113, 461–482.CrossRef Gray, W. D., Sims, C. R., Fu, W. T., & Schoelles, M. J. (2006). The soft constraints hypothesis: A rational analysis approach to resource allocation for interactive behavior. Psychological Review, 113, 461–482.CrossRef
go back to reference Hosmer, D. W., Hosmer, T., Le Cessie, S., & Lemeshow, S. (1997). A comparison of goodness-of-fit tests for the logistic regression model. Statistics in Medicine, 16, 965–980.CrossRef Hosmer, D. W., Hosmer, T., Le Cessie, S., & Lemeshow, S. (1997). A comparison of goodness-of-fit tests for the logistic regression model. Statistics in Medicine, 16, 965–980.CrossRef
go back to reference Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion—Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21, 1686–1693.CrossRef Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion—Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21, 1686–1693.CrossRef
go back to reference Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General., 139, 665–682.CrossRef Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General., 139, 665–682.CrossRef
go back to reference Koriat, A. (2008). Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition. Memory and Cognition, 36, 416–428.CrossRef Koriat, A. (2008). Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition. Memory and Cognition, 36, 416–428.CrossRef
go back to reference Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661–679.CrossRef Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661–679.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2011). Computational Modeling in Cognition: Principles and Practice. Los Angeles: Sage.CrossRef Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2011). Computational Modeling in Cognition: Principles and Practice. Los Angeles: Sage.CrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–224.CrossRef Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–224.CrossRef
go back to reference Pitts, B., Riggs, S. L., & Sarter, N. (2016). Cross-modal matching: A critical but neglected step in multimodal research. IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems, 46, 445–450.CrossRef Pitts, B., Riggs, S. L., & Sarter, N. (2016). Cross-modal matching: A critical but neglected step in multimodal research. IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems, 46, 445–450.CrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392–409.CrossRef Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392–409.CrossRef
go back to reference Potts, C. A., Pastel, S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018). How are cognitive and physical difficulty compared? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 500–511.CrossRef Potts, C. A., Pastel, S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018). How are cognitive and physical difficulty compared? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 500–511.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A. (2017). Knowing Hands—The Cognitive Psychology of Manual Control. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A. (2017). Knowing Hands—The Cognitive Psychology of Manual Control. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., & Gregory, R. W. (2002). Development of a method for measuring movement-related effort. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 365–373.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A., & Gregory, R. W. (2002). Development of a method for measuring movement-related effort. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 365–373.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., & Gaydos, M. J. (2008). A method for obtaining psychophysical estimates of movement costs. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40, 11–17.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A., & Gaydos, M. J. (2008). A method for obtaining psychophysical estimates of movement costs. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40, 11–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Stevens, J. C., & Marks, L. E. (1965). Cross-modality matching of brightness and loudness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 54, 407–411.CrossRef Stevens, J. C., & Marks, L. E. (1965). Cross-modality matching of brightness and loudness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 54, 407–411.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652–654.CrossRef Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652–654.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, Amsterdam, 30, 276–315.CrossRef Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, Amsterdam, 30, 276–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Yarkoni, T., & Westfall, J. (2017). Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: Lessons from machine learning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 1100–1122.CrossRef Yarkoni, T., & Westfall, J. (2017). Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: Lessons from machine learning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 1100–1122.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Effort avoidance is not simply error avoidance
Auteurs
Iman Feghhi
David A. Rosenbaum
Publicatiedatum
02-04-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01331-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2021

Psychological Research 4/2021 Naar de uitgave