Introduction
Heart failure is a debilitating clinical syndrome with a poor prognosis despite considerable improvements in treatment options over the last decade (Hunt
2005). Dyspnoea, oedema, fluid retention, pulmonary congestion and fatigue comprise common symptoms of heart failure, and although not necessarily present at the same time within an individual patient they all have a detrimental influence on functional status and quality of life (Hunt
2005; Juenger et al.
2002; Masoudi et al.
2004).
Quality of life has gained increased recognition as an important patient-centered outcome (Krumholz et al.
2005) and has been incorporated as a performance measure for evaluating the quality of clinical care in the Improving Continuous Cardiac Care (IC3) registry (
http://www.improvingcardiaccare.org/Documents/IC3v1_ProgramSummary.pdf), a recent initiative by the American College of Cardiology Foundation in the United States. Some studies indicate that heart failure patients may prefer better quality of life over prolonged survival (Spertus
2008). Poor quality of life, as assessed by patient self-report, has also been shown to predict prognosis, including mortality and rehospitalization in heart failure patients (Mommersteeg et al.
2009). Hence, for the management of patients in clinical practice, knowledge of the patient’s quality of life and in particular the clinical and psychological profile of those patients who experience poor quality of life is important for secondary prevention. Such information may help identify patients who may need more aggressive medical treatment or adjunctive psychological intervention (Spertus
2008).
Previously, mood states, such as anxiety and depression, have been identified as important determinants of quality of life both in heart failure patients and in patients with coronary artery disease (Pedersen et al.
2006; Rector et al.
1987; Schiffer et al.
2008a,
b). However, little attention has been paid to the role of personality factors in heart failure, with only two studies focusing on the impact of the distressed (Type D) personality (i.e., increased tendency to experience increased negative emotions paired with emotional non-expression due to fear of rejection by others) on quality of life in heart failure (Schiffer et al.
2008a,
b,
2005). These studies were conducted in outpatients with heart failure rather than primary care heart failure patients, with one of the studies using a cross-sectional study design (Schiffer et al.
2005). Both studies showed that Type D was associated with poorer quality of life and health status, adjusting for demographic and clinical characteristics, including left ventricular dysfunction and New York Heart Association (NYHA) functional class (Schiffer et al.
2008a,
b,
2005). In heart failure, Type D has also been related to increased levels of pro-inflammatory cytokines (Denollet et al.
2009a,
b), which are markers of poor prognosis in heart failure (Dunlay et al.
2008), and with late (>6 months) mortality (Schiffer et al.
2009). Given that heart failure patients seen by their primary care physician may have a milder disease spectrum, it is important to investigate whether findings from outpatients seen by their cardiologist or heart failure nurse generalize to this patient group. In the general cardiovascular literature, there is accumulating evidence that Type D is a risk marker for adverse health outcomes across different forms of cardiovascular disease (Pedersen and Denollet
2006). Type D personality has also been shown to predict adverse health outcomes in 482 older primary care patients above and beyond personality traits encompassed in the Five Factor Model of personality, indicating that Type D provides unique risk that is not captured by standard personality traits (Chapman et al.
2007).
In the current study, we examined (1) the influence of Type D personality on quality of life at baseline and 9-month follow-up in primary care heart failure patients, and (2) the extent to which the influence of personality on quality of life is confounded by NYHA class and depressive symptoms.
Results
Baseline characteristics of the sample
The prevalence of Type D personality was 31.9%. Baseline characteristics for the total sample and stratified by Type D personality are shown in Table
1. Heart failure patients with a Type D personality disposition were slightly younger and experienced more depressive symptoms compared to non-Type D patients. No other statistically significant differences were found on baseline characteristics between the two groups.
Table 1
Baseline characteristics for the total sample and stratified by Type D personality
Age, mean (SD) |
71.9 (9.4)
|
70.0 (9.8)
|
72.8 (9.0)
|
.03
|
Male gender | 47.4% | 48.8% | 46.8% | .88 |
Living with a partner | 69.6% | 65.0% | 71.8% | .35 |
Low educationa
| 73.6% | 72.5% | 74.1% | .91 |
Employed | 10.0% | 11.8% | 9.6% | .77 |
NYHA class II–IV | 42.6% | 41.3% | 43.3% | .87 |
Depressive symptoms |
21.5%
|
43.8%
|
11.1%
|
<.001
|
Extent of independence between the constructs Type D personality and quality of life
In order to rule out that Type D would predict quality of life outcomes solely due to there being too large an overlap between the two constructs, we first investigated the relationship between Type D and quality of life using correlational analyses. Pearson’s correlations showed that the Type D Scale negative affectivity component correlated significantly with emotional (
r = .53;
P < .001) and physical quality of life (
r = .40;
P < .001) at baseline and also with emotional (
r = .50;
P < .001) and physical quality of life (
r = .38;
P < .001) at 9 months. Similarly, the Type D Scale social inhibition component correlated significantly with emotional (
r = .27;
P < .001) and physical quality of life (
r = .18;
P = .004) at baseline and also with emotional (
r = .27;
P < .001) and physical quality of life (
r = .23;
P < .001) at 9 months. Given that a high score on the Minnesota Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire reflects a poorer quality of life, higher scores on the Type D Scale negative affectivity and social inhibition components were associated with poorer emotional and physical quality of life. Nevertheless, the shared variance between the subcomponents of the Type D Scale and the quality of life scores ranged from only 3 to 28%, with none of the correlations exceeding the cut-off >.85 (Munro
2001). This indicates that there is not a problem with collinearity nor that there is a question of the argument being circular if Type D is related to quality of life in further analyses.
These results were confirmed when relating Type D caseness to quality of life scores, with the shared variance between Type D personality and quality of life being lower than when using continuous scores for the Type D subcomponents, although Type D was still significantly correlated with baseline emotional (ρ = .28; P < .001) and physical quality of life (ρ = .17; P = .009) and with 9-month emotional (ρ = .22; P < .001) and physical quality of life (ρ = .15; P < .02).
The influence of Type D personality on quality of life at baseline and 9 months (unadjusted)
Analysis of variance with repeated measures showed that Type D patients experienced poorer emotional quality of life both at baseline and 9 months compared to non-Type D patients (
F(1,249) = 24.784;
P < .001; partial eta-squared = .091) (Table
2). There was no significant change in emotional quality of life between baseline and 9 months (
F(1,249) = .076;
P = .78; partial eta-squared = .000), nor was there a differential change in emotional quality of life over time between Type D and non-Type D patients, as indicated by the non-significant interaction effect for time
by Type D (
F(1,249) = 1.043;
P = .31; partial eta-squared = .004). Hence, Type D exerted a stable effect on quality of life over time.
Table 2
Quality of life at baseline and 9 months, stratified by Type D personality, presented as means (SDs)
Emotional quality of life |
Baseline | 5.96 (5.92) | 2.89 (3.75) |
9 months | 5.78 (5.72) | 3.20 (3.92) |
Physical quality of life |
Baseline | 16.33 (10.08) | 12.99 (10.27) |
9 months | 16.44 (10.51) | 13.22 (10.48) |
Similar results were found for the physical dimension of quality of life, with Type D patients experiencing poorer physical quality of life at both time points compared to non-Type D patients (
F(1,249) = 6.243;
P = .01; partial eta-squared = .024) (Table
2). There was also no change in physical quality of life over time (
F(1,249) = .115;
P = .74; partial eta-squared = .000), nor was the interaction effect for time
by Type D significant (
F(1,249) = .014;
P = .91; partial eta-squared = .000), indicating that Type D also exerted a stable effect on physical quality of life over time.
The influence of Type D personality on quality of life at baseline and 9 months (adjusted)
Type D was still a significant independent determinant of poor emotional quality of life (F(1,227) = 4.808; P = .03; partial eta-squared = .021), adjusting for gender, age, marital status, education, employment status, NYHA class, and depressive symptoms, using analysis of covariance with repeated measures. Younger age (F(1,227) = 4.947; P = .03; partial eta-squared = .021), NYHA class II-IV (F(1,227) = 27.550; P < .001; partial eta-squared = .108), and depressive symptoms (F(1,227) = 56.181; P < .001; partial eta-squared = .198) were also significant determinants of poor emotional quality of life. In adjusted analysis, the within-subjects effect for time was still not significant (F(1,227) = .810; P = .37; partial eta-squared = .004), indicating that there was no change in emotional quality of life over time. None of the other interaction effects for time by covariates were significant (P’s > .05), except for NYHA class by time, with a plot of the estimated marginal means indicating that patients in NYHA class II-IV experienced a deterioration in emotional quality of life over time.
In adjusted analysis, Type D was no longer an independent determinant of poor physical quality of life (F(1,227) = 1.115; P = .29; partial eta-squared = .005). The only covariates significantly associated with poor physical quality of life were female gender (F(1,227) = 6.560; P = .01; partial eta-squared = .028), NYHA class II-IV (F(1,227) = 149.759; P < .001; partial eta-squared = .397), and depressive symptoms (F(1,227) = 25.085; P < .001; partial eta-squared = .100). In adjusted analysis, there was also no change in physical quality of life over time (F(1,227) = .005; P = .94; partial eta-squared = .000), nor were any of the within-subjects effects for time by covariates significant (P’s > .05).
Discussion
In the current prospective study, primary care heart failure patients with a Type D personality experienced poorer emotional quality of life compared to non-Type D patients. The influence of personality on emotional quality of life in adjusted analysis could not be explained by demographic characteristics, NYHA class, and depressive symptoms. Although Type D personality was also associated with worse physical quality of life in unadjusted analysis, this relationship was no longer significant when controlling for demographic and clinical characteristics and depressive symptoms. NYHA class and depressive symptoms were also important, independent determinants of both poor emotional and physical quality of life.
Previous studies have shown that mood states, such as anxiety and depression, are important determinants of poor quality of life in heart failure patients, but these studies were either cross-sectional (Brenes
2007; Muller-Tasch et al.
2007) or conducted in outpatients with heart failure (Evangelista et al.
2008; Heo et al.
2008). The current study extends those findings by showing that personality factors add to the level of prediction of poor quality of life above and beyond depressive symptoms. This finding is consistent with a recent Dutch study of heart failure outpatients, showing that Type D patients report poorer emotional but not physical quality of life, as assessed by means of both disease-specific and generic measures (Schiffer et al.
2008a,
b).
In the current study, we also found that higher NYHA class was associated with poorer quality of life. In previous studies, a worsening of functional status in heart failure outpatients predicted a decline in quality of life (Masoudi et al.
2004), and in a cross-sectional study of primary care heart failure NYHA functional class was associated with poor quality of life on several dimensions of the Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) (Khunti et al.
2007). NYHA class III or IV has also been found to predict impairment in quality of life after open heart surgery (Pelle et al.
2009) and poor prognosis in heart failure (Andersson et al.
2008; Muntwyler et al.
2002). Despite the predictive value of NYHA class and its wide usage as a measure of functional status in heart failure patients, its objectivity and reproducibility are the subject of some debate (Raphael et al.
2007). There may also be some confounding between NYHA class and specific psychological measures, as we previously showed that Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale scores, but not Type D personality, increased with higher NYHA class (Scherer et al.
2006).
The results of the current study have some implications for research and clinical practice. First, our results emphasize the importance of looking at the dimensions of quality of life (i.e., emotional versus physical) separately, as we only found a relationship between Type D personality and emotional quality of life, but not physical quality of life. This indicates that using a component or total score of quality of life may mask differences between subgroups on different aspects of quality of life. Second, previous studies in outpatients with heart failure and coronary artery disease have shown that Type D personality is associated with adverse health outcomes, including mortality, morbidity, and poor quality of life, independent of disease severity (Schiffer et al.
2008a,
b,
2005; Pedersen and Denollet
2006; Denollet and Pedersen
2008). This was also confirmed in the current study, with patients with a Type D personality experiencing poorer emotional quality of life. To date, no randomized controlled trial targeting Type D personality has been conducted, but preliminary evidence suggests that these patients derive benefits from cardiac rehabilitation in terms of improved quality of life (Karlsson et al.
2007; Pelle et al.
2008). Hence, identification of this high-risk subgroup of patients seems worthwhile from a secondary prevention perspective, with respect to examining whether their heart failure is optimally managed or whether other treatment initiatives should be implemented. By analogy, we still do not know how to treat depression in cardiac patients, at least to the extent that such treatment leads to a concomitant improvement in survival (Berkman et al.
2003). However, this does not mean that we should not screen cardiac patients for depression nor that depression should be left untreated, as this would be unethical due to the known influence of depression on compliance, lifestyle modifications, participation in cardiac rehabilitation, mortality, and morbidity (Whooley
2009). Third, there are several advantages of using an instrument, such as the Type D scale, to identify patients with a general vulnerability to experience adverse health outcomes, over measures of anxiety, depression, and quality of life. These include the confounding of the latter measures with indices of somatic disease, such as NYHA class and left ventricular dysfunction. In a previous study, using the same patient sample as in the current study, we showed that a total score on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale was confounded by NYHA class, whereas this was not the case for the Type D scale (Scherer et al.
2006). Similar confounding of the Beck Depression inventory, but not the Type D Scale, with indices of somatic disease has been found in patients with coronary artery disease (Martens et al.
2007). Moreover, evidence from patients with coronary artery disease indicates that not all distressed patients can be captured by standard measures of depression, and that despite some overlap Type D provides unique information above and beyond measures of depression. Of 1,205 myocardial infarction patients, 206 (17%) patients had a clinical diagnosis of depression, 224 (19%) patients a Type D personality, whereas only 90 (7%) had both forms of distress (Denollet et al.
2009a,
b). This may also be the case for quality of life, when used as a screening instrument rather than an outcome measure. Others have also indicated that measures that are sensitive as outcome measures may not necessarily be optimal screening instruments (Hevey et al.
2004). Furthermore, a recent study of patients with ischemic heart disease or heart failure indicated that despite some overlap between the Type D Scale and measures frequently used in cardiovascular research, including the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, the Beck Depression Inventory, and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, the Type D Scale was the only examined measure that remained intact in its originally proposed form using factor analysis on item level and second-order level (Pelle et al.
2009).
The results of the current study have some limitations. First, given that the study was designed to examine predictors of patient-centered outcomes available in primary care practices, we had little information on traditional risk factors, such as co morbidity, hypercholesterolemia, left ventricular dysfunction, etc. Although we cannot rule out that left ventricular dysfunction may have confounded the results, evidence for an impact of left ventricular dysfunction on patient-centered outcomes, such as quality of life, anxiety and depressive symptoms, is mixed, with some (Bjelland et al.
2002; de Rivas et al.
2008) but not all studies (Lewis et al.
2007; Schiffer et al.
2008a,
b,
2005) supporting such a relationship. Second, depressive symptoms were assessed by means of self-report rather than a clinical diagnostic interview. However, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale has good sensitivity and specificity compared to the gold standard of a clinical diagnostic interview (Bjelland et al.
2002), and has also been shown to predict mortality in several cardiac patient groups (Herrmann et al.
2000; Junger et al.
2005). Third, patients excluded from analyses were more likely to be older and have a higher NYHA class compared to patients included in analyses, indicating that included patients had a less severe spectrum of disease, which might have led to a bias. Fourth, the results may primarily be generalizable to heart failure patients in NYHA class I and II, given that patients in NYHA class III and IV were under sampled. This necessitated the somewhat arbitrary cut-off of NYHA class I versus NYHA class II–IV for the statistical analyses. Fifth, several measures are available with which to assess quality of life of both a generic and disease-specific nature and a narrow or broad focus in terms of the number of domains of quality of life assessed. Although we chose to use the Minnesota Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire, which is a well accepted and frequently used disease-specific measure in heart failure, it only represents a limited number of quality of life domains. Sixth, it could be argued that the potential overlap between psychological factors and between psychological factors and quality of life may threaten the value of the results of the current study, as Type D might solely predict quality of life outcomes due to there being too large an overlap between the constructs. However, the shared variance between the subcomponents of the Type D Scale and the quality of life scores ranged from only 3 to 28%, with none of the correlations exceeding the cut-off >.85, indicating that there was not a problem with collinearity (Munro
2001). Similarly, a recent study of patients with ischemic heart disease or heart failure examining the overlap between measures frequently used in cardiovascular research, including the Type D Scale and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, showed that these measures are distinct despite some overlap (Pelle et al.
2009). In the latter study, the Type D Scale was the only of the examined measures for which the original proposed structure remained intact using factor analysis on item level and second-order level.
Despite these limitations this study also has several strengths, including its prospective study design with the assessment of quality of life at two time points, the use of a disease-specific measure of quality of life, and the inclusion of personality factors, which is a novel approach in heart failure research. In addition, the study was conducted in a relatively large group of patients selected from the whole patient population of various practices by means of a pre-defined algorithm, and is therefore largely representative of the elderly heart failure patient seen in general practice. The relatively large number of exclusions can be traced back to this algorithm, making it unlikely that our sample was subject to an unintentional selection bias.
Further research is warranted to examine the added value of the Type D personality construct in primary care heart failure patients, particularly with respect to whether Type D affects mortality and rehospitalization in this patient group. Such research should aim to include more detailed information on the clinical characteristics of the patients than was available in the current study, in order to draw more firm conclusions. It would also be worthwhile to include other personality traits, such as comprised within the Five Factor Model of personality, to establish that Type D has added value above and beyond these traits as a predictor of health outcomes, as shown in older primary care patients (Chapman et al.
2007). Finally, further research on Type D should investigate whether the construct can best be viewed as a taxonomy, or whether patients in the grey zone around the cut-offs for determining Type D personality are also at risk for adverse health outcomes.
In conclusion, primary care heart failure patients with a Type D personality experienced poorer emotional but not physical quality of life compared to non-Type D patients. The influence of Type D personality on emotional quality of life could not be attributed to demographic characteristics, NYHA class, nor depressive symptoms. Patients with this personality profile should be identified in primary care, as both Type D and poor quality of life have been associated with mortality in this patient group. This could be done with the brief, validated Type D Scale (DS14), which comprises little burden to patients and to clinical practice. An international committee providing recommendations for the assessment of psychosocial risk factor in clinical cardiology practice has also recommended the use of the Type D Scale as one of the measures to identify high-risk patients (Albus et al.
2004).