Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2022

13-04-2021 | Original Article

Two processing stages of the SNARC effect

Auteurs: Weizhi Nan, Lizhu Yan, Guochun Yang, Xun Liu, Shimin Fu

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2022

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The spatial–numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect showed that small/large numbers represented in the left/right space facilitated left/right responses, respectively. However, the processing stage (semantic representation or response selection) of the SNARC effect is still controversial. To investigate this issue, we adopted a modified magnitude comparison task in which the effects of SNARC, Stroop (semantic-representation stage), and Simon (response-selection stage) could be simultaneously induced. The processing stages of the SNARC effect were investigated by examining the interactions among these effects. According to the additive factor logic, if two effects are interactive, then they occur in the same stage; if two effects are additive, then they occur in different stages. Across two experiments, the SNARC effect interacted with the Stroop effect and with the Simon effect. These results suggested that the SNARC effect occurred in both the semantic-representation and response-selection stages and provided insight into that the SNARC effect might have two originating sources.
Literatuur
go back to reference Abad, E. V., Scheffers, M. K., Coles, M. G. H., Fournier, L., & Adamson, A. (1997). The dimensionality of the flanker compatibility effect: a psychophysiological analysis. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 60(3), 144–155.PubMed Abad, E. V., Scheffers, M. K., Coles, M. G. H., Fournier, L., & Adamson, A. (1997). The dimensionality of the flanker compatibility effect: a psychophysiological analysis. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 60(3), 144–155.PubMed
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(2), 365.PubMed Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(2), 365.PubMed
go back to reference Basso Moro, S., Dell’Acqua, R., & Cutini, S. (2018). The SNARC effect is not a unitary phenomenon. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(2), 688–695. Basso Moro, S., Dell’Acqua, R., & Cutini, S. (2018). The SNARC effect is not a unitary phenomenon. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(2), 688–695.
go back to reference Chen, A., Bailey, K., Tiernan, B. N., & West, R. (2011). Neural correlates of stimulus and response interference in a 2–1 mapping stroop task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80, 129–138.PubMed Chen, A., Bailey, K., Tiernan, B. N., & West, R. (2011). Neural correlates of stimulus and response interference in a 2–1 mapping stroop task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80, 129–138.PubMed
go back to reference Cutini, S., Scarpa, F., Scatturin, P., DellAcqua, R., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Number-space interactions in the human parietal cortex: enlightening the SNARC effect with functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Cerebral Cortex, 24(2), 444–451.PubMed Cutini, S., Scarpa, F., Scatturin, P., DellAcqua, R., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Number-space interactions in the human parietal cortex: enlightening the SNARC effect with functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Cerebral Cortex, 24(2), 444–451.PubMed
go back to reference Daar, M., & Pratt, J. (2008). Digits affect actions: the SNARC effect and response selection. Cortex, 44(4), 400–405.PubMed Daar, M., & Pratt, J. (2008). Digits affect actions: the SNARC effect and response selection. Cortex, 44(4), 400–405.PubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731–750.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731–750.PubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396.
go back to reference Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(10), 374–380.PubMed Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(10), 374–380.PubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition–from single digits to arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461–1483. Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition–from single digits to arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461–1483.
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.PubMed Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.PubMed
go back to reference Fitousi, D., & Algom, D. (2020). A model for two-digit number processing based on a joint Garner and system factorial technology analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 676–700. Fitousi, D., & Algom, D. (2020). A model for two-digit number processing based on a joint Garner and system factorial technology analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 676–700.
go back to reference Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 659–673. Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 659–673.
go back to reference Hock, H. S., & Egeth, H. (1970). Verbal interference with encoding in a perceptual classification task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(2), 299–303.PubMed Hock, H. S., & Egeth, H. (1970). Verbal interference with encoding in a perceptual classification task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(2), 299–303.PubMed
go back to reference Houwer, J. (2003). On the role of stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus compatibility in the stroop effect. Memory & Cognition, 31(3), 353–359. Houwer, J. (2003). On the role of stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus compatibility in the stroop effect. Memory & Cognition, 31(3), 353–359.
go back to reference Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response-selection stage. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 48–56.PubMed Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response-selection stage. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 48–56.PubMed
go back to reference Keus, I. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: evidence for a response-related origin. Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 681–695. Keus, I. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: evidence for a response-related origin. Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 681–695.
go back to reference Kornblum, H., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMed Kornblum, H., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S. (1994). The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: the case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56(3), 130–135.PubMed Kornblum, S. (1994). The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: the case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56(3), 130–135.PubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus–response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855–875.PubMed Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus–response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855–875.PubMed
go back to reference Leuthold, H. (2011). The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: a short review. Acta Psychologica, 136(2), 203–211.PubMed Leuthold, H. (2011). The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: a short review. Acta Psychologica, 136(2), 203–211.PubMed
go back to reference Li, Q., Nan, W., Wang, K., & Liu, X. (2014). Independent processing of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response conflicts. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e89249.PubMedPubMedCentral Li, Q., Nan, W., Wang, K., & Liu, X. (2014). Independent processing of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response conflicts. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e89249.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Liu, X., Park, Y., Gu, X., & Fan, J. (2010). Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus–response compatibility effects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(6), 1710–1720. Liu, X., Park, Y., Gu, X., & Fan, J. (2010). Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus–response compatibility effects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(6), 1710–1720.
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203.PubMed MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203.PubMed
go back to reference Mapelli, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. (2003). The SNARC effect: an instance of the Simon effect? Cognition, 88(3), B1–B10.PubMed Mapelli, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. (2003). The SNARC effect: an instance of the Simon effect? Cognition, 88(3), B1–B10.PubMed
go back to reference Melara, R. D., Wang, H., Vu, K. P., & Proctor, R. W. (2008). Attentional origins of the Simon effect: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Brain Research, 1215, 147–159.PubMed Melara, R. D., Wang, H., Vu, K. P., & Proctor, R. W. (2008). Attentional origins of the Simon effect: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Brain Research, 1215, 147–159.PubMed
go back to reference Nuerk, H. C., Kaufmann, L., Zoppoth, S., & Willmes, K. (2004). On the development of the mental number line: more, less, or never holistic with increasing age? Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1199–1211.PubMed Nuerk, H. C., Kaufmann, L., Zoppoth, S., & Willmes, K. (2004). On the development of the mental number line: more, less, or never holistic with increasing age? Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1199–1211.PubMed
go back to reference Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82, B25–B33.PubMed Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82, B25–B33.PubMed
go back to reference Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2002). A unit-decade compatibility effect in German number words. Current Psychology Letterss: Behaviour, Brain and Cognition, 2, 19–38. Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2002). A unit-decade compatibility effect in German number words. Current Psychology Letterss: Behaviour, Brain and Cognition, 2, 19–38.
go back to reference Rusconi, E., Turatto, M., & Umilta, C. (2007). Two orienting mechanisms in posterior parietal lobule: an rTMS study of the Simon and SNARC effects. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(4), 373–392.PubMed Rusconi, E., Turatto, M., & Umilta, C. (2007). Two orienting mechanisms in posterior parietal lobule: an rTMS study of the Simon and SNARC effects. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(4), 373–392.PubMed
go back to reference Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umilta, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 17815.PubMedPubMedCentral Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umilta, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 17815.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.PubMed Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.PubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the ‘Stroop effect’ vs the ‘Simon effect.’ Acta Psychologica, 73(2), 159–170.PubMed Simon, J. R., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the ‘Stroop effect’ vs the ‘Simon effect.’ Acta Psychologica, 73(2), 159–170.PubMed
go back to reference Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315. Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.
go back to reference Tlauka, M. (2002). The processing of numbers in choice-reaction tasks. Australian Psychological Society, 54(2), 94–98. Tlauka, M. (2002). The processing of numbers in choice-reaction tasks. Australian Psychological Society, 54(2), 94–98.
go back to reference Treccani, B., Milanese, N., & Umilta, C. (2010). Influence on Simon and SNARC effects of a nonspatial stimulus–response mapping: between-task logical recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1239–1254.PubMed Treccani, B., Milanese, N., & Umilta, C. (2010). Influence on Simon and SNARC effects of a nonspatial stimulus–response mapping: between-task logical recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1239–1254.PubMed
go back to reference van Dijck, J. P., & Doricchi, F. (2019). Multiple left-to-right spatial representations of number magnitudes? Evidence from left spatial neglect. Experimental Brain Research, 237(4), 1031–1043.PubMed van Dijck, J. P., & Doricchi, F. (2019). Multiple left-to-right spatial representations of number magnitudes? Evidence from left spatial neglect. Experimental Brain Research, 237(4), 1031–1043.PubMed
go back to reference van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114–119.PubMed van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114–119.PubMed
go back to reference van Veen, V., Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. NeuroImage, 14(6), 1302–1308.PubMed van Veen, V., Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. NeuroImage, 14(6), 1302–1308.PubMed
go back to reference Wang, K., Li, Q., Zheng, Y., Wang, H., & Liu, X. (2014). Temporal and spectral profiles of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict processing. NeuroImage, 89, 280–288.PubMed Wang, K., Li, Q., Zheng, Y., Wang, H., & Liu, X. (2014). Temporal and spectral profiles of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict processing. NeuroImage, 89, 280–288.PubMed
go back to reference Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 453–464.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 453–464.PubMed
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007a). Response coding in the Simon task. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(4), 401–410.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007a). Response coding in the Simon task. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(4), 401–410.PubMed
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007b). The Simon effect for vertical S-R relations: changing the mechanism by randomly varying the S–R mapping rule? Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(2), 219–233.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007b). The Simon effect for vertical S-R relations: changing the mechanism by randomly varying the S–R mapping rule? Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(2), 219–233.PubMed
go back to reference Yang, G., Nan, W., Li, Q., & Liu, X. (2015). Behavioral and electrophysiological profiles reveal domain-specific conflict processing. Advances in Computational Psychophysiology, 350(6256), 33–34. Yang, G., Nan, W., Li, Q., & Liu, X. (2015). Behavioral and electrophysiological profiles reveal domain-specific conflict processing. Advances in Computational Psychophysiology, 350(6256), 33–34.
go back to reference Yang, G., Nan, W., Zheng, Y., Wu, H., Li, Q., & Liu, X. (2017). Distinct cognitive control mechanisms as revealed by modality-specific conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(4), 807–818.PubMed Yang, G., Nan, W., Zheng, Y., Wu, H., Li, Q., & Liu, X. (2017). Distinct cognitive control mechanisms as revealed by modality-specific conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(4), 807–818.PubMed
go back to reference Zhang, H. H., Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1999). A parallel distributed processing model of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response compatibility. Cognitive Psychology, 38(3), 386–432.PubMed Zhang, H. H., Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1999). A parallel distributed processing model of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response compatibility. Cognitive Psychology, 38(3), 386–432.PubMed
go back to reference Zhou, X., Chen, C., Chen, L., & Dong, Q. (2008). Holistic or compositional representation of two-digit numbers? Evidence from the distance, magnitude, and SNARC effects in a number-matching task. Cognition, 106(3), 1525–1536.PubMed Zhou, X., Chen, C., Chen, L., & Dong, Q. (2008). Holistic or compositional representation of two-digit numbers? Evidence from the distance, magnitude, and SNARC effects in a number-matching task. Cognition, 106(3), 1525–1536.PubMed
go back to reference Zohar-Shai, B., Tzelgov, J., Karni, A., & Rubinsten, O. (2017). It does exist! A left-to-right spatial–numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect among native Hebrew speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(4), 719–728.PubMed Zohar-Shai, B., Tzelgov, J., Karni, A., & Rubinsten, O. (2017). It does exist! A left-to-right spatial–numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect among native Hebrew speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(4), 719–728.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Two processing stages of the SNARC effect
Auteurs
Weizhi Nan
Lizhu Yan
Guochun Yang
Xun Liu
Shimin Fu
Publicatiedatum
13-04-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2022
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01506-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2022

Psychological Research 2/2022 Naar de uitgave