Introduction
Cognitive-behavioral (CB) interventions are generally found to be mildly effective in preventing the onset of depression among high-risk youth (see the meta-analyses by Rasing et al.,
2017 & Werner-Seidler et al.,
2017). Although informative, the establishment of the effectiveness of psychological interventions is primarily concerned with comparing aggregated pre- to post-intervention outcomes across groups (i.e., intervention versus control). As such, effectiveness research implicitly assumes that change is linear and steady across time (Hayes et al.,
2007) and that group-derived estimates can be generalized back down to the individual participant (Fisher et al.,
2018). Those assumptions and research practices, however, are worrisome because the aggregation of data across individuals may obscure heterogeneity in symptom change trajectories. Several studies have shown that individuals may follow extremely variable time courses of progress (e.g., Barkham et al.,
2006; Krause et al.,
1998) and that different change trajectories may relate to different (long-term) outcomes (e.g., Hayes et al.,
2007). Consequently, interindividual differences in trajectories of change are hypothesized to reflect differential processes and mechanisms of change (Kazdin,
2007; Vittengl et al.,
2013). Individual time course data are henceforth increasingly recognized for their ability to study
how rather than
whether change occurs (Hayes et al.,
2007). An enhanced understanding of how change unfolds during indicated preventive interventions for adolescent depression may enable (school) clinicians to more accurately predict individuals’ prognoses and tailor intervention strategies as needed (cf. Borntrager & Lyon,
2015; Lambert,
2007; Lutz et al.,
2002). Unfortunately, research on patterns of change in a depression prevention context among adolescents is surprisingly scarce. The current study aimed to address this gap by studying profiles of change in a school-based depression prevention trial for adolescent girls with elevated depressive symptoms.
The most widely adopted pattern of change is the
log-linear trajectory (i.e., strong improvement at the beginning of therapy, followed by slower remission thenceforth), which is built upon the theoretical origins of the dosage model of psychotherapy (Howard et al.,
1986). This model suggests that there is a decelerating, log-linear relationship between the number of sessions (dose) and the probability of improvement (effect). Quicker improvements early in therapy have been ascribed to a remoralizing response (i.e., the restoration of hope), whereas slower symptom remission later in therapy may represent learning and practicing skills (Howard et al.,
1993). Although the log-linear model of change has been demonstrated among various diagnoses, treatments, and outcome-measures within clinical settings (Lutz et al.,
2002), research suggests a high degree of individual variation in trajectories of change (e.g., Hayes et al.,
2007). Moreover, research on distinct shapes of change of youth in
preventive contexts is scarce. This is surprising, because one might expect a non-help seeking sample – who may have minimal motivation to change – to have very different change trajectories compared to adolescents in a clinical sample who sought out treatment (Prochaska & Norcross,
2001). The identification of distinct trajectories of symptom change in a preventive sample may be crucial to tailor preventive interventions to the diverse needs of high risk adolescents (Scott et al.,
2019).
A modeling technique that may allow for the identification of distinct trajectories of change is the Growth Mixture Modeling (GMM) approach (Kaplan & Muthén,
2004). This analytical strategy belongs to the person-centered approaches and aims to categorize individuals into subgroups based on intra-individual response trajectories, such that individuals within a group are more similar than individuals between groups (Jung & Wickrama,
2008). Previous studies have used GMM to detect responders and non-responders to antidepressant medication in clinical trials among adults (Gueorguieva et al.,
2011) and elderly patients (Zilcha-Mano et al.,
2017) and to identify distinct patterns of change among adults receiving psychotherapy across a range of different outpatient settings (e.g., Saunders et al.,
2019; Stulz et al.,
2007).
Earlier work has also adopted the GMM approach to investigate differential trajectories of change in clinical and subclinical adolescent samples. For example, a re-analysis of the Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study comparing CB therapy, fluoxetine, a combined condition, and placebo revealed three distinct subgroups with unique trajectories of change: high severity-early improvement, high severity-limited improvement, and moderate severity-late improvement (Scott et al.,
2019). Analyses showed that adolescents were less likely to populate the high severity-early improvement class when receiving CB therapy compared to the placebo condition. Further, the inclusion of covariates revealed several baseline factors that predicted trajectory class membership. Specifically, adolescents in the high severity-limited improvement class were found to be older, had higher depression severity, higher levels of hopelessness, and reported more severe cognitive distortions compared to late improvers. Distinct trajectories of change were also identified among an indicated prevention trial for adolescent depression, comparing a CB group, CB bibliotherapy and brochure control condition (Brière et al.,
2016). Depressive symptoms were not assessed
during the intervention period but at pre- and post-test and 6-, 12-, and 24-months follow-up. Latent Class Growth Analysis (a special case of GMM; Jung & Wickrama,
2008) revealed four distinct profiles of symptom change: a low-declining (58% of the sample), high-declining (26%), high-persistent (10%) and resurging (i.e., high initial severity followed by a decline and then increase; 6%) pattern. Adolescents in either CB condition were significantly less likely to follow the high-persistent trajectory relative to the brochure control condition. Interestingly, motivation to reduce depression was higher in all three high initial depression trajectories relative to the low-declining trajectory and a negative cognitive style was more prevalent in the high-declining and high-persistent class trajectories compared to the resurging and low-declining trajectories. These studies both contribute to the evidence that non-specific factors – i.e., factors that are shared across most forms of therapy such as motivation to change, self-efficacy (Bandura et al.,
1999) or expectancies (Laska et al.,
2014) – may play an important role in predicting one’s intervention response (Wampold,
2016). Next, the finding that cognitive vulnerabilities predicted trajectory membership in both trials is not surprising, given that these vulnerabilities are considered the core intervention target in CB therapy (Beck et al.,
1979). More research is needed to confirm whether non-specific factors and cognitive coping strategies may serve as useful predictors of the shapes of change in an adolescent depression prevention context.
Next, although previous studies have demonstrated empirically typical symptom change trajectories and found factors that predict differential intervention response, no studies have yet investigated potential distinct trajectories of change during indicated depression prevention programs for adolescents. Studying shapes of change in a preventive context may be of crucial importance since trajectories may inform theories on how adolescents change (Owen et al.,
2015). Moreover, knowledge about different profiles of change may inform expectations about the magnitude and timing of change during preventive efforts. Such knowledge may have important implications for clinical practice, since a better understanding of how and when change comes about may allow one to predict an individual’s intervention response and timely adjust and tailor intervention strategies as needed (Lutz,
2002). For example, if the systematic monitoring of adolescents’ intervention response indicates that some girls are following a chronic course, these girls could be detected during the intervention and timely referred to more intensive intervention options. Traditional approaches often rely on pre-post measurements and hence can only detect non-responders
in hindsight of interventions, whereas continuous monitoring offers opportunities to detect and timely refer at-risk girls
during the intervention period. The systematic monitoring of children’s mental health symptoms and feedback of these ongoing assessments to (school) clinicians may be crucial to improve school-based mental health service delivery (Borntrager & Lyon,
2015).
Discussion
Although research on the effectiveness of depression prevention programs for adolescents has gained substantial attention in the literature, little is known about how change during prevention efforts comes about. Given that the field predominantly relies on comparing aggregated pre- to post intervention outcomes, insight into how symptoms change over time is lost and heterogeneity in symptom change trajectories may be obscured. By using a person-centered approach, the current study contributes to the understanding of profiles of change during indicated prevention programs among adolescent girls with elevated depressive symptoms. As expected, several subgroups with unique trajectories of symptom change could be identified: Moderate-Declining, High-Persistent, and Deteriorating-Declining trajectories. Unexpectedly, exposure to one of the CB interventions appeared to be unrelated to the trajectories of change as similar rates of trajectory membership were found in all conditions (including the monitoring control condition). Further findings indicated that trajectory membership was partially related to outcomes at 12-months follow-up. Girls within the High-Persistent trajectory had higher levels of depressive symptoms at follow-up relative to the girls in the other trajectories. Finally, several baseline factors (i.e., depression severity at screening, age, acceptance, rumination, catastrophizing, and self-efficacy) predicted trajectory membership.
The distinct trajectories identified in the current study are partially consistent with previous studies. Specifically, the Moderate-Declining trajectory – i.e., moderate symptom levels at baseline that gradually declined through post-test – is comparable with the subgroups “Low-Declining” (Brière et al.,
2016), “Later-improvement” (Scott et al.,
2019), and “Slow-remission” (Maalouf et al.,
2012) that were detected among adolescents with depressive symptoms in previous preventive as well as clinical trials. The High-Persistent class – i.e., elevated symptoms at baseline that remained elevated through post-test – also aligns with previously identified trajectories, such as the “High-Persistent” (Brière et al.,
2016), “Limited improvement” (Scott et al.,
2019) and “No-remission” (Maalouf et al.,
2012) subgroups. Girls within the High-Persistent trajectory varied closely around the clinical cut-off score of the RADS-2 of 76 (p. 231, Hilsenroth & Segal,
2003) and had worse outcomes at 12 months follow-up relative to the other girls. Thus, these girls showed chronic symptom trajectories and probably needed more intensive intervention. Some of these girls may have met the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders criteria for persistent depressive disorder (5
th ed.; American Psychiatric Association,
2013), given the chronic nature of their symptoms. The current results provide support for the idea of a stepped care model in which symptoms are systematically monitored and more intensive intervention options (e.g., individual therapy delivered by a school clinician or treatment within the clinical setting) are provided for those who do not respond to low-intensity interventions (Hermens et al.,
2014).
In contrast to the trajectories described thus far, the Deteriorating-Declining trajectory, which was characterized by a period of worsening at the beginning of the intervention period, followed by a strong reduction in symptoms to post-test, has not been demonstrated previously in studies using a GMM approach. The current results show that those who experience initial worsening can still greatly decline in depressive symptoms during the course of an intervention or merely monitoring period. Perhaps, girls within this trajectory experienced a so-called “depression spike”, which refers to a transient period of apparent worsening that actually opens up a system towards more healthy patterns of functioning and may catalyze transformational change (i.e., a steep decrease in depressive symptoms; Hayes et al.,
2007).
The distinct trajectories that were found in the current study raises the question of what processes and mechanisms may have produced differential change (Kazdin,
2007). For instance, one might speculate that the gradual changes in the Moderate-Declining trajectory may reflect gradual learning and practice of skills (Vittengl et al.,
2013) or that the depression spikes of girls within the Deteriorating-Declining effect were brought about by certain intervention effects or techniques. However, unexpectedly, it was found that intervention condition was unrelated to the trajectories of symptom change. The beneficial symptom trajectories (i.e., Moderate-Declining and Deteriorating-Declining) comprised the majority of participants in the indicated CB interventions, but also in the monitoring condition. This finding renders it impossible to draw any conclusions as to whether the identified (beneficial) change trajectories might be reflective of naturally occurring features of depression course (i.e., spontaneous remission) or actual intervention effects. The design of this study and lack of power to explore profiles within the separate conditions does not lend itself to parse between either conclusion. The present results are consistent with previous analyses on the initial RCT that also failed to find a prophylactic effect of the CB interventions compared to the monitoring control group (Poppelaars et al.,
2016). Nevertheless, the person-centered approach of this study adds that the general sample could be divided into unique subpopulations with similar symptom trajectories and showed that the prevalence of these subpopulations did not differ between conditions. These results suggest that close attention should be paid to the personal characteristics of the adolescent that may explain one’s intervention response (Bernaras et al.,
2019).
In the current study, there were a few baseline characteristics that significantly predicted trajectory class membership. With regard to the High-Persistent group, girls within this trajectory had higher levels of depressive symptomatology at screening relative to girls within the Moderate-Declining trajectory, which points towards the chronic symptom course of girls in the High-Persistent group and opens up the possibility for early detection. Additionally, girls within the High-Persistent trajectory were found to be generally older than girls in the Deteriorating-Declining trajectory. This finding is consistent previous research showing that older adolescents were less likely to respond to CB therapy (in a sample that ranged between 10 and 17 years old; Jayson et al.,
1998). The authors reasoned that that depressive symptoms may be more firmly established in older adolescents and they may therefore be more resistant to intervention efforts (Jayson et al.,
1998). It should be noted, however, that the mean age difference between the two trajectories in the current study was small (see Table
4), so future research is needed to replicate this finding and improve understanding of the effect of age on intervention response. Last, girls in the High-Persistent trajectory reported lower levels of self-efficacy compared to girls in the Moderate-Declining trajectory. Social cognitive theory suggests that a low sense of perceived self-efficacy may lead to a discrepancy between personal aspirations and the perceived capacity to attain those (Bandura et al.,
1999). This discrepancy may give rise to negative self-evaluation and depression, which may explain the higher levels of depressive symptoms among adolescents who reported a low sense of self-efficacy.
Further reporting higher levels of acceptance and/or lower levels of rumination predicted membership of the Deteriorating-Declining class relative to the High-Persistent class. Previous scholars have suggested that maladaptive ruminative responses may contribute to higher levels of depressive symptoms over time, because rumination may prolong and increase the negative thinking characteristic of a dysphoric mood (Nolen-Hoeksema,
2000). In contrast, acceptance is considered an adaptive coping response and has been associated with less depressive symptoms in youth (Schäfer et al.,
2017). It has been proposed that the acceptance of emotions may enable one to discard dysfunctional strategies (e.g., suppressing or judging negative feelings; Werner & Gross,
2010). Those explanations may clarify why girls with high levels of acceptance and low levels of rumination showed a beneficial symptom course, whereas girls low in acceptance and high in rumination followed a chronic course.
The final predictor of trajectory class was catastrophizing, which was higher in the Deteriorating-Declining trajectory relatively to the Moderate-Declining trajectory. Cognitive models of depression suggest that catastrophizing is a cognitive distortion that may increase vulnerability to depression (Dozois & Beck,
2008), which may partially explain why girls in the Deteriorating-Declining trajectory reported more depressive symptoms at pre-test. Based on this baseline characteristic it is not possible, however, draw any conclusions as to why these girls showed a period of worsening before a steep decrease to post-test. It should also be noted that changes in baseline factors changed during the intervention period were not examined, which would be expected, in particular, for girls that were exposed to one of the CB interventions. Future research is needed to study whether any of those baseline factors might serve as potential mediators of change in depressive symptomatology.
Strengths, Limitations and Future Directions
The present study has a number of important strengths. Trajectories of depressive symptom change were studied in a relatively large sample of sub-clinically depressed adolescent girls with a high response rate. The weekly assessments of adolescents’ symptoms provided insight in their unique profiles of change. Moreover, the current study extends previous analyses on the initial RCT (Poppelaars et al.,
2016), since the person-centered approach of this study enabled for the identification of unique subgroups and for investigating whether their distinct trajectories were associated with the intervention conditions, long term course and different baseline factors.
Despite the strengths of the present study, several limitations should be acknowledged. First, the prevalence of the Deteriorating-Declining trajectory was relatively low (
n = 13), limiting the statistical power of the analyses involving this group. Additionally, after identifying the best fitting model, trajectory class membership was fixed in further analyses so these did not take into account uncertainties in the assignment of individuals to the distinct trajectory classes (i.e., the class-membership posterior probabilities; Proust-Lima et al.,
2017). Another limitation was that the current sample may not accurately reflect the diverse population of adolescent girls in the Netherlands as most girls followed higher education and 95% were of Dutch descent. Last, analyses comprised all intervention conditions due to the small sample sizes of the separate conditions, thereby limiting the identification of trajectories within intervention condition.
Although the present study provides insight in distinct trajectories of symptom change in an indicated depression prevention trial among adolescents, future research is needed to enhance understanding of the distinct trajectories. An important and challenging issue to address in future research is to identify the mechanisms of change (Kazdin,
2007) that may facilitate improvement during indicated CB interventions. To this aim, researchers are encouraged to study whether distinct symptom-change trajectories can be linked with therapy process variables (e.g., therapeutic alliance or in-game play behaviors), intervention specific mediators of change (e.g., changes in cognitions), and other time-varying covariates such as life-events or social-interpersonal functioning. Such measurements may also allow researchers to uncover processes that are associated with the worsening state early in the Deteriorating-Declining trajectory. Future research is also needed to replicate the distinct trajectories that were found in the present study, given the exploratory nature of this study. One subgroup was quite small, which could limit the generalizability of the present findings. Next it would be interesting to study whether shapes of change might differ among a male adolescent sample. Besides looking at individual change trajectories based on standardized symptom measures, future researchers are also encouraged to look at idiographic symptom (clusters) to gain insight in the problems of greatest concern to adolescents. Additionally, in order to tease apart girls within the High-Persistent versus Deteriorating-Declining group, future studies are needed to identify indicators that may pinpoint which individual belongs to which group. Last, future studies are encouraged to adopt a qualitative approach, in which adolescents from each subgroup are interviewed to enhance understanding of how they experienced the interventions, how they relate to changes in their daily life and what might be operative for those who do not improve (Kazdin,
2007).
Implications
Despite the aforementioned limitations, the present study has important implications for clinical practice. First and foremost, results imply that the identification of girls within the High-Persistent trajectory is crucial as these girls showed a chronic symptom course and probably needed more intensive intervention. The systematic monitoring of individuals’ symptoms and feedback of this information to school clinicians and prevention workers may open up the possibility to detect these girls from an early stage and prevent the development of a clinical depression among highest risk adolescent girls. In order to identify girls within the high-persistent trajectory it may be also important to ask girls about the duration of their complaints at screening, given the chronic nature of the girls’ symptoms within this group. Moreover, the detection of the Deteriorating-Declining trajectory suggests that, when initial worsening has been observed, (school) clinicians and prevention workers should still consider to continue intervention efforts. Further, findings also imply that a large proportion of adolescent girls selected for indicated prevention may not need a formal intervention since the majority of participants in the monitoring control condition followed a beneficial symptom course trajectory, suggesting that rates of spontaneous recovery are high. Finally, the present study’s finding that the distinct trajectories were not related to intervention conditions hampers the ability to inform decision-making related to which type of preventive intervention (OVK, SPARX, OVK & SPARX or merely monitoring) should be delivered. Nevertheless, the distinct trajectories may in the future be used to inform decisions related to predictions about individual intervention responses, teasing apart girls that may be at risk for intervention failure, and when to refer those girls (e.g., the change trajectories identified by the current data suggest that if girls haven’t improved around session 5 it might be worthwhile to consider more intensive intervention options since chances of improvement are low).
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.