Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2011

01-11-2011 | Original Article

Talking and driving: applications of crossmodal action reveal a special role for spatial language

Auteurs: Paul Atchley, Jeff Dressel, Todd C. Jones, Rebecca A. Burson, David Marshall

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Talking reduces attention resulting in real-world crash risks to drivers that talk on a phone and drive. Driving is a behavior that is very demanding on spatial attention, suggesting potentially large interference by spatial codes in language. The current study investigated how different types of verbal codes influence visual attention during dual-task performance. In two experiments, participants performed a spatial or non-spatial verbal task while simultaneously performing a visual attention task. The results showed a larger decrement to visual attention performance when participants were concurrently engaged in a spatial verbal task. The results of the second experiment isolated this effect to the right cerebral hemisphere, consistent with a role for shared right parietal resources. These results are consistent with the idea that processing codes are an important component of coordinating talking and driving but generally inconsistent with a broad class of bottleneck approaches that describes dual-task decrements but treats component tasks as cognitively equivalent.
Literatuur
go back to reference Almor, A. (2008). Does language interfere with vision-based tasks? Experimental Psychology, 55, 260–268.PubMedCrossRef Almor, A. (2008). Does language interfere with vision-based tasks? Experimental Psychology, 55, 260–268.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Atchley, P., & Dressel, J. (2004). Conversation limits the functional field of view. Human Factors, 46, 664–673.PubMedCrossRef Atchley, P., & Dressel, J. (2004). Conversation limits the functional field of view. Human Factors, 46, 664–673.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Atchley, P., Atwood, S., & Boulton, A. (2011). The choice to text and drive in younger drivers: behavior may shape attitude. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43, 134–142.PubMedCrossRef Atchley, P., Atwood, S., & Boulton, A. (2011). The choice to text and drive in younger drivers: behavior may shape attitude. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43, 134–142.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1983). Working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 302B, 311–324.CrossRef Baddeley, A. D. (1983). Working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 302B, 311–324.CrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human memory: theory and practice. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human memory: theory and practice. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
go back to reference Ball, K. K., & Owsley, C. (1991). Identifying correlates of accident involvement for the older driver. Human Factors, 33, 583–595.PubMed Ball, K. K., & Owsley, C. (1991). Identifying correlates of accident involvement for the older driver. Human Factors, 33, 583–595.PubMed
go back to reference Ball, K., Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., & Bruni, J. R. (1993). Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older adults. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 34, 3110–3123.PubMed Ball, K., Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., & Bruni, J. R. (1993). Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older adults. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 34, 3110–3123.PubMed
go back to reference Ball, K. K., Owsley, C., Stalvey, B. T., Roenker, D. L., Sloane, M. E., & Graves, M. (1998). Driving avoidance and functional impairment in older drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 30, 313–322.PubMedCrossRef Ball, K. K., Owsley, C., Stalvey, B. T., Roenker, D. L., Sloane, M. E., & Graves, M. (1998). Driving avoidance and functional impairment in older drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 30, 313–322.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., et al. (2007). The english lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459.PubMedCrossRef Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., et al. (2007). The english lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brown, I. D., Tickner, A. H., & Simmonds, D. C. V. (1969). Interference between concurrent tasks of driving and telephoning. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 419–424.PubMedCrossRef Brown, I. D., Tickner, A. H., & Simmonds, D. C. V. (1969). Interference between concurrent tasks of driving and telephoning. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 419–424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: a critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–990.PubMedCrossRef Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: a critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–990.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dressel, J., & Atchley, P. (2005). Conversation limits attention: the impact of conversation complexity. Journal of Vision, 5, 398a.CrossRef Dressel, J., & Atchley, P. (2005). Conversation limits attention: the impact of conversation complexity. Journal of Vision, 5, 398a.CrossRef
go back to reference Drews, F. A., Pasupathi, M., & Strayer, D. L. (2008). Passenger and cell phone conversations in simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 392–400.PubMedCrossRef Drews, F. A., Pasupathi, M., & Strayer, D. L. (2008). Passenger and cell phone conversations in simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 392–400.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dux, P. E., Ivanhoff, J., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron, 52, 1109–1120.PubMedCrossRef Dux, P. E., Ivanhoff, J., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron, 52, 1109–1120.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grimshaw, G. M., Séguin, J. A., & Godfrey, H. K. (2009). Once more with feeling: the effects of emotional prosody on hemispheric specialisation for linguistic processing. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 313–326.CrossRef Grimshaw, G. M., Séguin, J. A., & Godfrey, H. K. (2009). Once more with feeling: the effects of emotional prosody on hemispheric specialisation for linguistic processing. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 313–326.CrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., & Wifall, T. (2011) Searching working memory for the source of dual-task costs. (Current issue). Hazeltine, E., & Wifall, T. (2011) Searching working memory for the source of dual-task costs. (Current issue).
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Teague, D., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(3), 527–545.PubMedCrossRef Hazeltine, E., Teague, D., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(3), 527–545.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heilman, K. M., & Van den Abell, T. (1979). Right hemispheric dominance for mediating cerebral activation. Neuropsychologia, 17, 315–321.PubMedCrossRef Heilman, K. M., & Van den Abell, T. (1979). Right hemispheric dominance for mediating cerebral activation. Neuropsychologia, 17, 315–321.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huestegge, L.(2011) The role of saccades in multitasking: Towards an output-related view of eye movements. (Current issue). Huestegge, L.(2011) The role of saccades in multitasking: Towards an output-related view of eye movements. (Current issue).
go back to reference Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2009). Dual-task crosstalk between saccades and manual responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 352–362.PubMedCrossRef Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2009). Dual-task crosstalk between saccades and manual responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 352–362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Crossmodal action selection: evidence from dual-task compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 38, 493–501.CrossRef Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Crossmodal action selection: evidence from dual-task compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 38, 493–501.CrossRef
go back to reference Israel, M., & Cohen, A. (2011) Involuntary strategy-dependent dual task performance. (Current issue). Israel, M., & Cohen, A. (2011) Involuntary strategy-dependent dual task performance. (Current issue).
go back to reference Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
go back to reference Kahneman, D., Beatty, J., & Pollack, I. (1967). Perceptual deficit during a mental task. Science, 157(3785), 218.PubMedCrossRef Kahneman, D., Beatty, J., & Pollack, I. (1967). Perceptual deficit during a mental task. Science, 157(3785), 218.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kimura, D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 166–171.CrossRef Kimura, D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 166–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Kimura, D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 3, 163–178. Kimura, D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 3, 163–178.
go back to reference Kubose, T. T., Bock, K., Dell, G. S., Garnsey, S. M., Kramer, A. F., & Mayhugh, J. (2006). The effects of speech production and speech comprehension on driving performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 43–63.CrossRef Kubose, T. T., Bock, K., Dell, G. S., Garnsey, S. M., Kramer, A. F., & Mayhugh, J. (2006). The effects of speech production and speech comprehension on driving performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 43–63.CrossRef
go back to reference Kunar, M. A., Carter, R., Cohen, M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2008). Telephone conversation impairs sustained visual attention via a central bottleneck. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1135–1140.CrossRef Kunar, M. A., Carter, R., Cohen, M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2008). Telephone conversation impairs sustained visual attention via a central bottleneck. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1135–1140.CrossRef
go back to reference Levy, J., Pashler, H., & Boer, E. (2006). Central interference in driving: is there any stopping the psychological refractory period? Psychological Science, 17, 228–235.PubMedCrossRef Levy, J., Pashler, H., & Boer, E. (2006). Central interference in driving: is there any stopping the psychological refractory period? Psychological Science, 17, 228–235.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Magen, H., & Cohen, A. (2010). Modularity beyond perception: evidence from the prp paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 395–414.PubMedCrossRef Magen, H., & Cohen, A. (2010). Modularity beyond perception: evidence from the prp paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 395–414.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McEvoy, S., Stevenson, M., & Woodward, M. (2006). Phone use and crashes while driving: a representative survey of drivers in two Australian states. Medical Journal of Australia, 185, 630.PubMed McEvoy, S., Stevenson, M., & Woodward, M. (2006). Phone use and crashes while driving: a representative survey of drivers in two Australian states. Medical Journal of Australia, 185, 630.PubMed
go back to reference Mesulam, M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10, 309–325.PubMedCrossRef Mesulam, M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10, 309–325.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mozolic, J. L., Joyner, D., Hugenschmidt, C. E., Peiffer, A. M., Kraft, R. A., Maldjian, J. A., et al. (2008). Cross-modal deactivations during modality-specific selective attention. BMC Neurology, 8, 35.PubMedCrossRef Mozolic, J. L., Joyner, D., Hugenschmidt, C. E., Peiffer, A. M., Kraft, R. A., Maldjian, J. A., et al. (2008). Cross-modal deactivations during modality-specific selective attention. BMC Neurology, 8, 35.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 435–448.PubMedCrossRef Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 435–448.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nelson, E., Atchley, P., & Little, T. (2009). The effects of perception of risk and importance of answering and initiating a cellular phone call while driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 41, 438–444.PubMedCrossRef Nelson, E., Atchley, P., & Little, T. (2009). The effects of perception of risk and importance of answering and initiating a cellular phone call while driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 41, 438–444.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Owsley, C., Ball, K., McGwin, G., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., White, M. F., et al. (1998). Visual impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. Journal of the American Medical Association, 279, 1083–1088.PubMedCrossRef Owsley, C., Ball, K., McGwin, G., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., White, M. F., et al. (1998). Visual impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. Journal of the American Medical Association, 279, 1083–1088.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.PubMedCrossRef Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3, 179–197.PubMedCrossRef Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3, 179–197.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ravizza, S. M., Behrmann, M., & Fiez, J. A. (2005). Right pareital contributions to verbal working memory: spatial or executive? Neuropsychologia, 14, 2057–2067.CrossRef Ravizza, S. M., Behrmann, M., & Fiez, J. A. (2005). Right pareital contributions to verbal working memory: spatial or executive? Neuropsychologia, 14, 2057–2067.CrossRef
go back to reference Redelmeier, D. A., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1997). Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. The New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 453–458.PubMedCrossRef Redelmeier, D. A., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1997). Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. The New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 453–458.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools.
go back to reference Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., et al. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101.PubMedCrossRef Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., et al. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schumacher E. H., Schwarb, H., Lightman, E., & Hazeltine, E. (2011). Investigating the modality specificity of response selection using a temporal flanker task. (Current issue). Schumacher E. H., Schwarb, H., Lightman, E., & Hazeltine, E. (2011). Investigating the modality specificity of response selection using a temporal flanker task. (Current issue).
go back to reference Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2004). Control of attention shifts between vision and audition in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 10702–10706.PubMedCrossRef Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2004). Control of attention shifts between vision and audition in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 10702–10706.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spence, C., & Read, L. (2003). Speech shadowing while driving: on the diffculty of splitting attention between eye and ear. Psychological Science, 14, 251–256.PubMedCrossRef Spence, C., & Read, L. (2003). Speech shadowing while driving: on the diffculty of splitting attention between eye and ear. Psychological Science, 14, 251–256.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stelzel, C., & Schubert, T. (2011). Interference effects of stimulus-response modality pairings in dual tasks and their boundaries. (Current issue). Stelzel, C., & Schubert, T. (2011). Interference effects of stimulus-response modality pairings in dual tasks and their boundaries. (Current issue).
go back to reference Stephan, D.N., & Koch, I. (2011). The role of input-output modality compatibility in task switching. (Current issue). Stephan, D.N., & Koch, I. (2011). The role of input-output modality compatibility in task switching. (Current issue).
go back to reference Stoffregen, T. A., Pagulayan, R. J., Bardy, B. G., & Hettinger, L. J. (2000). Modulating postural control to facilitate visual performance. Human Movement Science, 19, 203–220.CrossRef Stoffregen, T. A., Pagulayan, R. J., Bardy, B. G., & Hettinger, L. J. (2000). Modulating postural control to facilitate visual performance. Human Movement Science, 19, 203–220.CrossRef
go back to reference Strayer, D. L., & Drews, F. A. (2007). Cell-phone-induced driver distraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 128–131.CrossRef Strayer, D. L., & Drews, F. A. (2007). Cell-phone-induced driver distraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 128–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Strayer, D., & Johnston, W. (2001). Driven to distraction: dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12, 463–466.CrossRef Strayer, D., & Johnston, W. (2001). Driven to distraction: dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12, 463–466.CrossRef
go back to reference Strayer, D., Drews, F., & Johnston, W. (2003). Cell-phone induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 23–32.PubMedCrossRef Strayer, D., Drews, F., & Johnston, W. (2003). Cell-phone induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 23–32.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tellinghuisen, D. J., & Nowak, E. J. (2003). The inability to ignore auditory distractors as a function of visual task perceptual load. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 817–828.CrossRef Tellinghuisen, D. J., & Nowak, E. J. (2003). The inability to ignore auditory distractors as a function of visual task perceptual load. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 817–828.CrossRef
go back to reference Voyer, D., Bowes, A., & Techentin, C. (2008). On the perception of sarcasm in dichotic listening. Neuropsychology, 22, 390–399.PubMedCrossRef Voyer, D., Bowes, A., & Techentin, C. (2008). On the perception of sarcasm in dichotic listening. Neuropsychology, 22, 390–399.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 239–257). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 239–257). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–102). San Diego: Academic Press. Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–102). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Wickens, C. D. (1991). Processing resources and attention. In D. Damos (Ed.), Multiple-task performance (pp. 3–34). London: Taylor & Francis. Wickens, C. D. (1991). Processing resources and attention. In D. Damos (Ed.), Multiple-task performance (pp. 3–34). London: Taylor & Francis.
go back to reference Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3, 159–177.CrossRef Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3, 159–177.CrossRef
go back to reference Woollacot, M., & Vander Velde, T. J. (2008). Non-visual spatial tasks reveal increased interactions with stance postural control. Brain Research, 1208, 95–102.CrossRef Woollacot, M., & Vander Velde, T. J. (2008). Non-visual spatial tasks reveal increased interactions with stance postural control. Brain Research, 1208, 95–102.CrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G., Sumowski, J. F., & Murray, M. (2011). Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied? (Current issue). Wylie, G., Sumowski, J. F., & Murray, M. (2011). Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied? (Current issue).
Metagegevens
Titel
Talking and driving: applications of crossmodal action reveal a special role for spatial language
Auteurs
Paul Atchley
Jeff Dressel
Todd C. Jones
Rebecca A. Burson
David Marshall
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0342-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2011

Psychological Research 6/2011 Naar de uitgave