Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2011

01-11-2011 | Original Article

Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied?

Auteurs: Glenn R. Wylie, James F. Sumowski, Micah Murray

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Generally, so-called control processes are thought to be necessary when we must perform one out of several competing actions. Some examples include performance of a less well-practiced action instead of a well-practiced one (prepotency); learning a new action (novelty); and rapidly switching from one action to another (task-switching). While it certainly is difficult to perform the desired action in these circumstances, it is less clear that a separate set of processes (e.g., control processes) are necessary to explain the observed behavior. Another way to approach the study of control processes is to investigate physiological dependent measures (e.g., electrophysiological or neuroimaging measures). Although these offer another avenue of inquiry into control processes, they have yet to furnish unambiguous evidence that control processes exist. While this might suggest that there are no control processes, it is also possible that our methods are insufficiently sensitive to measure control processes. We have investigated this latter possibility using tasks that are neuroanatomically distinct, though within the same modality (vision). This approach did not yield evidence for a separable set of control processes. However, recent works using a task-switching paradigm in which subjects switch between a visual and an auditory task suggest that switching both task and modality may be importantly different than switching task within a given modality. This may represent a way forward in the study of control processes.
Literatuur
go back to reference Alain, C., Arnott, S. R., Hevenor, S., Graham, S., & Grady, C.L. (2001). “What” and “where” in the human auditory system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(21), 12301–12306.PubMedCrossRef Alain, C., Arnott, S. R., Hevenor, S., Graham, S., & Grady, C.L. (2001). “What” and “where” in the human auditory system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(21), 12301–12306.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. C. Umilta & Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 107–132). Cambridge: MIT Press. Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. C. Umilta & Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 107–132). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Allport, D. A., & Wylie, G. R. (2000). Task-switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and Performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge: MIT Press. Allport, D. A., & Wylie, G. R. (2000). Task-switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and Performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., & Kerns, K. A. (2001). Utilization behavior: Clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review, 11(3), 117–130.PubMedCrossRef Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., & Kerns, K. A. (2001). Utilization behavior: Clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review, 11(3), 117–130.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch. Psychological Science, 15(9), 610–615.PubMedCrossRef Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch. Psychological Science, 15(9), 610–615.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Arrington, C. M., Weaver, S. M., & Pauker, R. L. (2010). Stimulus-based priming of task choice during voluntary task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1060–1067.PubMedCrossRef Arrington, C. M., Weaver, S. M., & Pauker, R. L. (2010). Stimulus-based priming of task choice during voluntary task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1060–1067.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(1), 5–28.CrossRef Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(1), 5–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., Ruge, H., Rubin, O., Koch, I., Prinz, W., von Cramona, D. Y., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). When the same response has different meanings: Recoding the response meaning in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 20(2), 1026–1031.PubMedCrossRef Brass, M., Ruge, H., Rubin, O., Koch, I., Prinz, W., von Cramona, D. Y., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). When the same response has different meanings: Recoding the response meaning in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 20(2), 1026–1031.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cerebral Cortex, 12(9), 908–914.PubMedCrossRef Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cerebral Cortex, 12(9), 908–914.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Decomposing components of task preparation with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 609–620.PubMedCrossRef Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Decomposing components of task preparation with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 609–620.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chomsky, N. (1971). The case against B.F. Skinner. New York: The New York Review of Books. Chomsky, N. (1971). The case against B.F. Skinner. New York: The New York Review of Books.
go back to reference De Santis, L., Clarke, S., & Murray, M. M. (2007). Automatic and intrinsic auditory “what” and “where” processing in humans revealed by electrical neuroimaging. Cerebral Cortex, 17(1), 9–17.PubMedCrossRef De Santis, L., Clarke, S., & Murray, M. M. (2007). Automatic and intrinsic auditory “what” and “where” processing in humans revealed by electrical neuroimaging. Cerebral Cortex, 17(1), 9–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference DeJong, R. (2001). An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. Control of cognitive processes. S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 357–376). Cambridge: MIT Press. DeJong, R. (2001). An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. Control of cognitive processes. S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 357–376). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Demakis, G. J. (2003). A meta-analytic review of the sensitivity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to frontal and lateralized frontal brain damage. Neuropsychology, 17(2), 255–264.PubMedCrossRef Demakis, G. J. (2003). A meta-analytic review of the sensitivity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to frontal and lateralized frontal brain damage. Neuropsychology, 17(2), 255–264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dove, A., Pollmann, S., et al. (2000). Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: An event-related fMRI study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 9(1), 103–109.PubMedCrossRef Dove, A., Pollmann, S., et al. (2000). Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: An event-related fMRI study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 9(1), 103–109.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dreher, J. C., Koechlin, E., Ali, S. O., & Grafman, J. (2002). The roles of timing and task order during task switching. Neuroimage, 17(1), 95–109.PubMedCrossRef Dreher, J. C., Koechlin, E., Ali, S. O., & Grafman, J. (2002). The roles of timing and task order during task switching. Neuroimage, 17(1), 95–109.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Forstmann, B. U., Brass, M., Koch, I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Voluntary selection of task sets revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 388–398.PubMedCrossRef Forstmann, B. U., Brass, M., Koch, I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Voluntary selection of task sets revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 388–398.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Forstmann, B. U., Wolfensteller, U., Derrfuss, J., Neumann, J., Brass, M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & von Cramon, D. V. (2008). When the choice is ours: Context and agency modulate the neural bases of decision-making. PLoS One, 3(4), e1899.PubMedCrossRef Forstmann, B. U., Wolfensteller, U., Derrfuss, J., Neumann, J., Brass, M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & von Cramon, D. V. (2008). When the choice is ours: Context and agency modulate the neural bases of decision-making. PLoS One, 3(4), e1899.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gurd, J. M., Amunts, K., Weia, P. H., Zafiris, O., Zilles, K., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2002). Posterior parietal cortex is implicated in continuous switching between verbal fluency tasks: An fMRI study with clinical implications. Brain, 125(Pt 5), 1024–1038.PubMedCrossRef Gurd, J. M., Amunts, K., Weia, P. H., Zafiris, O., Zilles, K., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2002). Posterior parietal cortex is implicated in continuous switching between verbal fluency tasks: An fMRI study with clinical implications. Brain, 125(Pt 5), 1024–1038.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Maisog, J. M., Pietrini, P., & Grady, C. L. (1994). “The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: A PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations.”. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(11 Pt 1), 6336–6353.PubMed Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Maisog, J. M., Pietrini, P., & Grady, C. L. (1994). “The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: A PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations.”. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(11 Pt 1), 6336–6353.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event Files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1), 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Event Files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1), 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Hubner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 640–653.PubMedCrossRef Hubner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 640–653.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hunt, A. R., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Multisensory executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 55(2), 325–327.PubMedCrossRef Hunt, A. R., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Multisensory executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 55(2), 325–327.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jenkins, I. H., Brooks, D. J., Nixon, P. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Passingham, R. E. (1994). Motor sequence learning: A study with positron emission tomography. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(6), 3775–3790.PubMed Jenkins, I. H., Brooks, D. J., Nixon, P. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Passingham, R. E. (1994). Motor sequence learning: A study with positron emission tomography. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(6), 3775–3790.PubMed
go back to reference Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of psychology 89, 5–82. Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of psychology 89, 5–82.
go back to reference Kaas, J. H., & Hackett, T. A. (2000). Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(22), 11793–11799.PubMedCrossRef Kaas, J. H., & Hackett, T. A. (2000). Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(22), 11793–11799.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874. Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874.
go back to reference Kimberg, D. Y., Aguirre, G. K., & D'Esposito, M. (2000). Modulation of task-related neural activity in task-switching: An fMRI study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 10(1–2), 189–196.PubMedCrossRef Kimberg, D. Y., Aguirre, G. K., & D'Esposito, M. (2000). Modulation of task-related neural activity in task-switching: An fMRI study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 10(1–2), 189–196.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62(4), 300–312.CrossRef Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62(4), 300–312.CrossRef
go back to reference Koch, I. (2003). The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 488–492.CrossRef Koch, I. (2003). The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 488–492.CrossRef
go back to reference Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 1–14.CrossRef Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 1–14.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (2007). What it costs to implement a plan: Plan-level and task-level contributions to switch costs. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 591–602.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (2007). What it costs to implement a plan: Plan-level and task-level contributions to switch costs. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 591–602.CrossRef
go back to reference Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010a). The role of preparation and cue-modality in crossmodal task switching. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam), 134(3), 318–322.CrossRef Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010a). The role of preparation and cue-modality in crossmodal task switching. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam), 134(3), 318–322.CrossRef
go back to reference Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010b). Switching attention between modalities: Further evidence for visual dominance. Psychological Research, 74(3), 255–267.PubMedCrossRef Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010b). Switching attention between modalities: Further evidence for visual dominance. Psychological Research, 74(3), 255–267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: Evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(1), 126–135.PubMedCrossRef MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: Evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(1), 126–135.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Maeder, P. P., Meuli, R. A., Adriani, M., Bellmann, A., Fornari, E., Thiran, J. P., Pittet, A., & Clarke, S. (2001). Distinct pathways involved in sound recognition and localization: A human fMRI study. Neuroimage, 14(4), 802–816.PubMedCrossRef Maeder, P. P., Meuli, R. A., Adriani, M., Bellmann, A., Fornari, E., Thiran, J. P., Pittet, A., & Clarke, S. (2001). Distinct pathways involved in sound recognition and localization: A human fMRI study. Neuroimage, 14(4), 802–816.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Bell, T. (2006). On how to be unpredictable: Evidence from the voluntary task-switching paradigm. Psychological Science, 17(9), 774–780.PubMedCrossRef Mayr, U., & Bell, T. (2006). On how to be unpredictable: Evidence from the voluntary task-switching paradigm. Psychological Science, 17(9), 774–780.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1–20.CrossRef Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Mesulam, M. M. (2000). Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Mesulam, M. M. (2000). Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Murray, M. M., De Santis, L., Thut, G., & Wylie, G. R. (2009). The costs of crossing paths and switching tasks between audition and vision. Brain and Cognition, 69(1), 47–55.PubMedCrossRef Murray, M. M., De Santis, L., Thut, G., & Wylie, G. R. (2009). The costs of crossing paths and switching tasks between audition and vision. Brain and Cognition, 69(1), 47–55.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nicholson, R., Karayanidis F., Bumak, E., Poboka, D., & Michie, P. T. (2006). ERPs dissociate the effects of switching task sets and task cues. Brain Research, 1095(1), 107–123. Nicholson, R., Karayanidis F., Bumak, E., Poboka, D., & Michie, P. T. (2006). ERPs dissociate the effects of switching task sets and task cues. Brain Research, 1095(1), 107–123.
go back to reference Nicholson, R., Karayanidis, F., Poboka, D., Heathcote, A., & Michie, P. T. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory task-switching processes. Psychophysiology, 42(5), 540–554.PubMed Nicholson, R., Karayanidis, F., Poboka, D., Heathcote, A., & Michie, P. T. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory task-switching processes. Psychophysiology, 42(5), 540–554.PubMed
go back to reference Philipp, A. M., Kalinich, C., Koch, I., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions. Psychological Research, 72(4), 405–414.PubMedCrossRef Philipp, A. M., Kalinich, C., Koch, I., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions. Psychological Research, 72(4), 405–414.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2005). Switching of response modalities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 58(7), 1325–1338. Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2005). Switching of response modalities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 58(7), 1325–1338.
go back to reference Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). The integration of task-set components into cognitive task representations. Psychologica Belgica, 50(3&4), 383–411. Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). The integration of task-set components into cognitive task representations. Psychologica Belgica, 50(3&4), 383–411.
go back to reference Rivier, F., & Clarke, S. (1997). Cytochrome oxidase, acetylcholinesterase, and NADPH-diaphorase staining in human supratemporal and insular cortex: Evidence for multiple auditory areas. Neuroimage, 6(4), 288–304.PubMedCrossRef Rivier, F., & Clarke, S. (1997). Cytochrome oxidase, acetylcholinesterase, and NADPH-diaphorase staining in human supratemporal and insular cortex: Evidence for multiple auditory areas. Neuroimage, 6(4), 288–304.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). The cost of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Discussion, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). The cost of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Discussion, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rauschecker, J. P. (1999). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(12), 1131–1136.PubMedCrossRef Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rauschecker, J. P. (1999). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(12), 1131–1136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763–797.PubMedCrossRef Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763–797.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2010). The target of task switching. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(2), 129–133.PubMed Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2010). The target of task switching. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(2), 129–133.PubMed
go back to reference Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf. Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.
go back to reference Sohn, M. H., Ursu, S., Anderson, J. R., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Inaugural article: The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(24), 13448–13453. Sohn, M. H., Ursu, S., Anderson, J. R., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Inaugural article: The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(24), 13448–13453.
go back to reference Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2010). Central cross-talk in task switching: Evidence from manipulating input-output modality compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1075–1081.PubMedCrossRef Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2010). Central cross-talk in task switching: Evidence from manipulating input-output modality compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1075–1081.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRef Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRef
go back to reference Sylvester, C. Y., Wager, T. D., Lacey, S. C., Hernandez, L., Nichols, T. E., Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (2003). Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 357–370. Sylvester, C. Y., Wager, T. D., Lacey, S. C., Hernandez, L., Nichols, T. E., Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (2003). Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 357–370.
go back to reference Toni, I., Krams, M., Turner, R., & Passingham, R. E. (1998). The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: A whole-brain fMRI study. Neuroimage, 8(1), 50–61.PubMedCrossRef Toni, I., Krams, M., Turner, R., & Passingham, R. E. (1998). The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: A whole-brain fMRI study. Neuroimage, 8(1), 50–61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. D. J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). London: The MIT Press. Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. D. J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). London: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, D. A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46(4), 361–413.PubMedCrossRef Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, D. A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46(4), 361–413.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G., & Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 212–233.PubMedCrossRef Wylie, G., & Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 212–233.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2003a). Cognitive control processes during an anticipated switch of task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(3), 667–672.PubMedCrossRef Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2003a). Cognitive control processes during an anticipated switch of task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(3), 667–672.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2003b). Task switching: A high-density electrical mapping study. Neuroimage, 20(4), 2322–2342.PubMedCrossRef Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2003b). Task switching: A high-density electrical mapping study. Neuroimage, 20(4), 2322–2342.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2004a). Don’t think of a white bear: An fMRI investigation of the effects of sequential instructional sets on cortical activity in a task-switching paradigm. Human Brain Mapping, 21, 279–297.PubMedCrossRef Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2004a). Don’t think of a white bear: An fMRI investigation of the effects of sequential instructional sets on cortical activity in a task-switching paradigm. Human Brain Mapping, 21, 279–297.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., Foxe, J. J. (2004b). The role of response requirements in task switching: Dissolving the residue. Neuroreport, 15(6), 1079–1087.PubMedCrossRef Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., Foxe, J. J. (2004b). The role of response requirements in task switching: Dissolving the residue. Neuroreport, 15(6), 1079–1087.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2006). Jumping the gun: Is effective preparation contingent upon anticipatory activation in task-relevant neural circuitry? Cerebral Cortex, 16(3), 394–404.PubMedCrossRef Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2006). Jumping the gun: Is effective preparation contingent upon anticipatory activation in task-relevant neural circuitry? Cerebral Cortex, 16(3), 394–404.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wylie, G. R., Murray, M. M., & Foxe, J. J. (2009). Distinct neurophysiological mechanisms mediate mixing costs and switch costs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 105–118.PubMedCrossRef Wylie, G. R., Murray, M. M., & Foxe, J. J. (2009). Distinct neurophysiological mechanisms mediate mixing costs and switch costs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 105–118.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yeung, N. (2010). Bottom-up influences on voluntary task switching: The elusive homunculus escapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(2), 348–362.PubMedCrossRef Yeung, N. (2010). Bottom-up influences on voluntary task switching: The elusive homunculus escapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(2), 348–362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yeung, N., & Monsell, S. (2003). The effects of recent practice on task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 919–936.PubMedCrossRef Yeung, N., & Monsell, S. (2003). The effects of recent practice on task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 919–936.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yeung, N., Nystrom, L. E., Aronson, J. A., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Between-task competition and cognitive control in task switching. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(5), 1429–1438.PubMedCrossRef Yeung, N., Nystrom, L. E., Aronson, J. A., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Between-task competition and cognitive control in task switching. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(5), 1429–1438.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied?
Auteurs
Glenn R. Wylie
James F. Sumowski
Micah Murray
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0354-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2011

Psychological Research 6/2011 Naar de uitgave