We will refer to the experience that is aimed for in a practice as a “goal-state.” In the main strand of academic literature on contentless states, goal-states like those in Shamatha, Thai Forest, and Stillness Meditation have been treated as contentless experience, and this experience is described as having no content (e.g., Fasching,
2008; Forman,
2011; Shear,
1999; Stace,
1961; see further Woods et al.,
2022a). In his foundational text, W. T. Stace (
1961, p. 86) stated that, “There [is] no mental content whatever but rather a complete emptiness, vacuum, void.” Robert Forman described the experience as involving “the barest being conscious” (Forman,
1999, p. 132) and as a virtual or complete blankness (
1986, p. 49,
1998, p. 7), and said that afterwards, “One just knows that one wasn’t ‘gone’, dead, blacked out” (Forman,
1998, p. 7). Jonathan Shear (
1990a, p. 396) stated that the “defining characteristic” of the experience is that “it is completely devoid of all empirical qualities and content – including even abstract contents such as blissfulness, ‘the divine’, etc.” Academics have frequently argued or assumed that, since contentless experiences have no content, there is nothing to differentiate them and they are therefore identical: An individual may have the experience at different time points, or different individuals may have the experience, but in each case what it is like to have the experience is the same (Almond,
1982; Bernhardt,
1990; Bucknell,
1989a,
1989b; Forman,
1990a; Shear,
1990b). From these understandings, it follows that the goal-states in Shamatha, Thai Forest, and Stillness Meditation are identical experiences lacking all content.
As the analysis of the traditional texts has indicated that the Shamatha, Thai Forest, and Stillness Meditation goal-states involve numerous forms of abstract content, it has suggested that the goal-states in the practices are not truly contentless. The analysis has indicated that the goal-states are contentless in the more limited sense of lacking well-recognized forms of content such as thoughts, perceptions, and mental images. Analysis of the traditional texts has also pointed to possible differences in the contentless goal-states across the practices. Most features of the goal-states are reported or implied in the traditional texts in all three practices, however the precise nature and level/degree of those features may vary.
What does one find if they take a sample of meditators in each of the three traditions and gather and analyze reports of their experiences using scientific method? Do meditators report that all or virtually all content is absent, as per the classical academic understanding of the contentless goal-states? Do they report that content such as thoughts and perceptions is absent but that abstract content is present, as per the understanding in the meditation traditions? Or is it the case that both these forms of contentless experience are just some distant ideal, far removed from anything meditators actually report? A further question concerns differences in the reports across the practices. Are there differences, and if so do they fit with what we understand about the meditation techniques?
The present study addresses these questions. To our knowledge, it is the first participant-based research comparing experiences reported in the three practices, or any pairing of them. Using a structured questionnaire participants reported their deepest experience of stillness/silence during a retreat (Shamatha and Thai Forest) or during class and home practice over a 7-day period (Stillness Meditation). The study investigates the subjective character of the typical experience reported in each practice, and determines whether there are differences in the reported experiences across the three traditions. The study examines meditators as they actually do the practices, rather than modifying the techniques or meditators’ practice regimens for the purposes of the research.
Discussion
Contentless experience is commonly treated as
pure consciousness or consciousness itself (e.g., Fasching,
2008; Forman,
1990b; Metzinger,
2020a; Stace,
1961). It is an important subject for cognitive science and philosophy, but is much in need of empirical research. In traditional texts, the experience is typically described as a complete stillness or silence, and it is a goal of many practices including Shamatha, Thai Forest, and Stillness Meditation (e.g., Brahm,
2014; McKinnon,
2011; Meares,
1986; Wallace,
2011b; for reviews see Woods et al.,
2020,
2022a,
2022b). To our knowledge, the present study is the first to compare experiences reported in those three practices or any pairing of them. In each tradition, participants were committed practitioners with varying levels of past practice, practising in a naturalistic manner with leading teachers. Participants indicated the extent to which each of the 48 dimensional items was part of their deepest experience of stillness/silence during a particular retreat (Shamatha and Thai Forest) or during class and home practice over a 7-day period (Stillness Meditation). The study examined the subjective character of the typical experience reported in each tradition, and whether there were differences in the reports across the practices.
Participants provided their responses for each of the 48 dimensional items and the three foil items using a 7-point scale (1 = No/none, 2 = Very low, 3 = Low, 4 = Lower-end-moderate, 5 = Higher-end-moderate, 6 = High, 7 = Very high) or by answering “I don’t remember or can’t say.” In this “Discussion” section, the term “low” (with the word low in lower case and not hyphenated) refers to scores in the range No/none to Low, the term “moderate” refers to scores in the range Lower-end-moderate to Higher-end-moderate, and the term “high” refers to scores in the range High to Very high.
In each practice, the deepest experience of stillness/silence typically reported by participants involved low or very close to low awareness of thoughts, emotions, images, memories, things around them, their body, their breath, and mental activity, low drowsiness and effort, and was not at all negative. The reported experience involved a high degree of stillness, calm, peacefulness, ease, and mental relaxation, and was highly positive, pleasant, and good. Nine of the 10 items with the lowest scores in each practice were the same across the three traditions, although the precise rankings differed (Table
4). This was also the case for the items with the highest scores: 9 of the 10 items were the same (Table
4). These findings show a clear similarity in the experiential profiles across the practices. This similarity is also reflected in the finding that there were no significant differences for 27 of the 48 dimensional items (Fig.
5).
In each practice, there is a broad resemblance between the deepest experience of stillness/silence typically reported by participants and the goal-states described in the traditional texts (Brahm,
2014; Woods et al.,
2022a). Participants on average gave low ratings for almost all items that the texts report/imply are absent in the goal-states and moderate or high ratings for almost all items the texts indicate are present.
The resemblance between the reports and the goal-states is not perfect. One example that stands out is that Shamatha and Thai Forest participants gave mean ratings for the item
awareness that I am having the experience that are at the upper end of the moderate band. This item concerns participants’ awareness
during the experience of stillness/silence that they were having the experience. The traditional texts indicate that during the goal-states in all three practices meditators have
no awareness that they are having the experience (Brahm,
2014; Meares,
1986; Wallace,
2010,
2011a,
2011b; for review see Woods et al.,
2022a). The goal-states involve an experience of stillness/silence with mental content such as stillness, silence, wakefulness, naturalness, and calm, but meditators only become aware that they have had this experience upon emerging from it.
Another notable discrepancy between the participant reports and the goal-states is that Stillness Meditation participants gave a mean rating for the item
wakefulness that is at the lower end of the moderate band. The traditional texts indicate that in the goal-states in each practice there is a heightened level of wakefulness (Brahm,
2014; Meares,
1968; Wallace,
2011a,
2014a,
2014b; for review see Woods et al.,
2022a).
Significant differences across the practice groups were found for 21 of the 48 dimensional items (Fig.
4). The most robust differences were found for nine items:
bliss,
joy,
losing normal ego/self via absorption,
pure being with a complete absence of doing,
reaching a ground state of the mind,
breath,
awareness that I am having the experience,
wakefulness, and
drowsiness. These differences remained significant after the correction for multiple comparisons, and our additional analyses provided confidence that they were most likely not due to differences in past practice or reasons for practising.
For two of the nine items with the most robust differences (
bliss and
joy), there was a difference between Shamatha and Thai Forest: Thai Forest scores were significantly higher than Shamatha scores. Bliss and joy tend to be emphasized more in Thai Forest teachings than in Shamatha (compare, e.g., Brahm,
2014 and Wallace,
2006). It could be that there is something about the Thai Forest technique that leads to greater experiences of bliss and joy. For example, arguably the Thai Forest teachings accentuate the need for the meditator to “let go” more than in Shamatha (see Brahm,
2014; but see also, e.g., Wallace,
2011a, pp. 179–184). A further possibility is that the Thai Forest technique does not lead to a greater experience of bliss and joy, but that the greater emphasis on those qualities in the teachings influences participant reports. Thai Forest participants might report greater bliss and joy simply because they are more familiar with those terms, or have a stronger sense that those qualities are considered desirable within the tradition (see further the comments about theory contamination below).
Another plausible explanation for the findings is that Thai Forest participants experienced more bliss and joy than Shamatha participants because they did significantly more practice in the target period. We did not attempt statistical analyses controlling for hours of practice in the target period because those hours very clearly differentiate the two practice groups, and controlling for them would therefore have distorted the independent variable (practice group) (Field,
2018; Miller & Chapman,
2001).
For the other seven of the nine items with the most robust differences, the differences were between Shamatha and Thai Forest on the one hand and Stillness Meditation on the other. Stillness Meditation scores were significantly higher than Shamatha and Thai Forest scores for the items losing normal ego/self via absorption, pure being with a complete absence of doing, and reaching a ground state of the mind. Stillness Meditation scores were significantly lower for the items breath and awareness that I am having the experience.
Meditators’ progression towards and into the goal-states is said to involve a dissolution of their normal sense of self, or ego, and as this dissolution occurs meditators are said to become absorbed in the stillness/silence (Brahm,
2014; Woods et al.,
2022a,
2022b). Meditators are fully absorbed in the stillness/silence if they are experiencing that alone, and if they have no sense of themselves as being separate from it (Brahm,
2014; Woods et al.,
2022a). Based on these understandings, Stillness Meditation participants’ higher scores for the item
losing normal ego/self via absorption provide an indication that their reported experience involved greater absorption in the stillness/silence. Their lower scores for the items
breath and
awareness that I am having the experience provide additional indications. If meditators are aware of their breath, they are not experiencing stillness/silence alone, and if they are aware of the stillness/silence as it occurs, they are in some sense standing apart from it (Brahm,
2014; Woods et al.,
2022a).
A further difference across the practices concerns arousal. Stillness Meditation scores were significantly lower than Shamatha and Thai Forest scores for the item wakefulness. On average, Stillness Meditation participants reported low drowsiness, but scores for that item were still significantly higher than in the other two practices. In summary, Stillness Meditation participants reported stillness/silence that was less wakeful, but more absorptive, with more the quality of pure being and reaching a ground state of the mind.
Stillness Meditation participants reported having the experience for a significantly greater proportion of their total practice, and being significantly more confident that they could have the experience again in their next session (Fig.
6). In all three practices, it has been traditionally understood that—in general—with more practice meditators experience stillness/silence more frequently and easily, and their experiences of stillness/silence become deeper and more absorptive (Brahm,
2014; McKinnon,
2011,
2016; Meares,
1986; Wallace,
2006). In the present study, however, Stillness Meditation participants reported the more absorptive experience, and had the higher scores for pure being, reaching a ground state, proportion, and confidence, despite reporting significantly fewer lifetime hours practice and doing significantly fewer hours of practice in the period targeted in the research. While retreats are generally regarded as especially conducive to deep experiences (King et al.,
2019), only Shamatha and Thai Forest participants attended retreats in the relevant period: Stillness Meditation participants reported the more absorptive experience and had the higher scores on the other items despite having done only class and home practice.
The findings in the paragraph above may seem surprising. How does one make sense of them? Does Stillness Meditation provide a shortcut to the deep and absorptive stillness/silence that is aimed for in the other two practices? Two possible explanations for the findings stand out: one, that Stillness Meditation does provide a form of shortcut, and two, that the differences in the reported experiences do not reflect differences in the actual experiences.
The first of the two possible explanations is that Stillness Meditation does provide a shortcut to deep and absorptive stillness/silence, but that the attentional quality in that experience is different to that aimed for in Shamatha and Thai Forest. Shamatha and Thai Forest involve systematic training of attention before reaching the goal-states, and this requires considerable time and effort. According to the traditional texts, the goal-states in those practices involve deep calm and relaxation and exceptionally focused attention (Brahm,
2014; Wallace,
2006,
2014a; Wallace & Hodel,
2008; for review see Woods et al.,
2022a). The goal-states in Stillness Meditation are also said to involve deep calm and relaxation, but that practice does not require systematic training of attention (Woods et al.,
2022a,
2022b). Analysis of the traditional texts has indicated that reaching the goal-states is quicker and easier in Stillness Meditation, but that in that practice attention is less focused and therefore less stable and vivid (e.g., Woods et al.,
2022a,
2022b).
The finding of greater wakefulness in Shamatha and Thai Forest also fits with this explanation. Unlike Stillness Meditation, those practices involve systematic training of attention, and that entails arousal of attention, which is likely associated with greater wakefulness (Britton et al.,
2014; Woods et al.,
2022a,
2022b).
The second possible explanation for the findings is that, although Stillness Meditation participants
reported an experience that was deeper in some respects, more absorptive, and less wakeful, their
actual experience was not deeper, more absorptive, or less wakeful. This could occur because a participant’s experience, their meditation background, and a range of other variables might affect their understanding of the items in the questionnaire, and the meaning that they give the scale points (low, moderate, high, etc.). For example, the present sample of Stillness Meditation participants reported significantly fewer lifetime hours practice than the retreat participants and a significantly greater proportion said they were practising to reduce psychological symptoms. A Stillness Meditation participant might give the rating high for the item
reaching a ground state of the mind because their experience seems deep in the context of their limited past practice and transcends any psychological distress. A retreat participant might have the same experience (or one that is even deeper) but assign the item a lower score because the participant’s greater practice has provided experiential or conceptual insight into how much deeper it would be possible to go, and because distress is not their baseline. Consistent with this, Wallace (
2006, pp. 109–110) notes that beginners and advanced practitioners can have quite different understandings of terms used to describe the goal-states (see also Grossman,
2011).
As a further example, a retreat participant might give the rating high for the item wakefulness in part because, although thoughts and mental images have dropped away, they still have awareness of the breath. A Stillness Meditation participant might have an equally wakeful experience without awareness of the breath, but give a lower rating because being awake is ordinarily associated with having sense or body perceptions.
A concern in this type of research is that meditators’ reports might be contaminated by their background knowledge and assumptions derived from sources such as the traditional texts (Metzinger,
2020a,
2020b; Sedlmeier et al.,
2016). We used preambles to reduce the risk of gross forms of this contamination. For instance, we explained to participants that we were interested in how the experience felt to them, rather than how others might describe it.
As with any study of meditators practising within an established tradition, there will be contamination to some degree. However, it is not the case that participants were simply echoing all of the language and concepts emphasized in the descriptions of the goal-states in the meditation traditions. Had they been doing this, we would have found an even closer resemblance between the reported experiences and the descriptions in the traditional texts.
The Stillness Meditation texts indicate there is heightened wakefulness in the goal-states, but careful comparison of the traditional texts across the practices has indicated that this heightened wakefulness could still be lower than in Shamatha and Thai Forest (see, e.g., Woods et al.,
2022a). If a Stillness Meditation participant was simply echoing the descriptions in that tradition, they would have reported a high degree of wakefulness, rather than wakefulness at the lower end of the moderate band as was actually reported. Similarly, if participants in any of the three practices were merely echoing the descriptions in the traditional texts, they would have reported having no awareness of the stillness/silence as it occurred.
Thai Forest participants reported greater bliss and joy than in Shamatha. Earlier, we noted that a possible explanation for this is that those qualities are emphasized more in the Thai Forest teachings. Emphasis on particular qualities in the Stillness Meditation teachings could provide a similar explanation for the differences with respect to that practice, however this possibility is not as clear-cut as in the case of bliss and joy in the Thai Forest / Shamatha comparison.
By way of example, the Stillness Meditation teachings emphasize pure being and a complete absence of doing more than in Shamatha and Thai Forest because the complete absence of doing is part of the Stillness Meditation technique from the outset of that practice. Stillness Meditation participants could have given higher ratings for the item pure being with a complete absence of doing simply because they were more familiar with those terms or viewed that quality as more desirable. “Pure being” sounds like a very basic state, so Stillness Meditation participants’ greater familiarity with that term, or their seeing that quality as more desirable, could have also led them to give higher ratings for the item reaching a ground state of the mind. The Stillness Meditation teachings, however, do not use the term “ground state of the mind.” That makes this different to the Thai Forest case, where the relevant terms—bliss and joy—are used frequently in the teachings.
The term stillness is used commonly in all three practices, but it is used most in Stillness Meditation because it is part of the name of that practice. Stillness Meditation scores were significantly higher than Shamatha and Thai Forest scores for the item stillness, but this was not one of the most robust differences across the practices.
As a separate matter, the questionnaire included three foil items designed to identify participants who might simply be endorsing all items that sound positive or like advanced experiences. In each practice, mean scores for the foil items were low or very close to low. They were much closer to the mean for dimensional items that the traditional texts indicate are absent in the goal-states than to the mean for items that the texts indicate are present (Fig.
3). These findings provide a measure of confidence that participants were making a genuine effort to reflect and report on their actual experience.
For the third foil item, progressing into more and more complex states, Stillness Meditation scores were significantly higher than Shamatha scores, and it is therefore possible that Stillness Meditation participants were inclined to over-endorse the positive and advanced sounding items. Notably, however, the difference was only between Stillness Meditation and Shamatha, and only on one of the three foil items.
Implications
The findings have implications for consciousness and neuroscientific research, and for clinical research and practice. In the main strand of academic literature on contentless states, goal-states like those in Shamatha, Thai Forest, and Stillness Meditation have been classed as contentless experiences, and those experiences have been described as having no content (e.g., Fasching,
2008; Forman,
2011; Shear,
1999; Stace,
1961; see further Woods et al.,
2022a) and as therefore identical (Almond,
1982; Bernhardt,
1990; Bucknell,
1989a,
1989b; Forman,
1990a; Shear,
1990b). The deepest experiences of stillness/silence typically reported in the present study are contentless or low-content in the limited sense that participants gave low ratings for well-recognized forms of content such as thoughts, perceptions, and images. The experiences are not contentless in the more complete sense referred to in the academic literature: Participants gave moderate or high ratings for numerous forms of abstract content, including wakefulness, naturalness, calm, bliss, joy, and freedom. The study also found robust differences in the reported experiences across the practices, contrasting with the academic understanding that contentless experiences are identical.
The finding that the reported experiences are neither truly contentless nor identical is supported by analysis of the goal-states described in the traditional texts (e.g., Woods et al.,
2022a). That analysis has indicated that the goal-states also involve numerous forms of abstract content and has identified various features of the goal-states that may differ across the practices. As explained above, there is a broad resemblance between participants’ reported experiences and the goal-states detailed in the traditional texts.
One way that contentless experience has been described in the academic literature is as a “barest being conscious” (Forman,
1999, p. 132) and a virtual or complete blankness (Forman,
1986, p. 49,
1998, p. 7). The quality of blankness is also referred to in an independent stream of academic research which has identified
mind-blanking as a form of attentional lapse that occurs in everyday life (Ward & Wegner,
2013; Watts & Sharrock,
1985). There is not yet a precise, consensus definition of mind-blanking (Fell,
2022), but elements that have been put forward include being off-task, and experiencing a blank or empty mind, an absence of thought and perception, and minimal or no other content (Andrillon et al.,
2019,
2021; Mortaheb et al.,
2022; Ward & Wegner,
2013).
The findings in the present study can inform future work aimed at determining the similarities and differences between contentless experiences in meditation and mind-blanking. The exact content (if any) that is present in mind-blanking has not yet been scientifically examined. One potential difference concerns wakefulness versus sleepiness. In the present study, participants in each practice typically reported low drowsiness and a moderate degree of wakefulness. Mind-blanking in certain types of laboratory experiment has been associated with sleepiness (Andrillon et al.,
2019,
2021), but it is not yet known whether that association also exists for mind-blanking in other contexts.
Our finding that there are significant differences in the reported experiences across the practices should be considered in neuroscientific studies of so-called contentless or low-content states. For example, in brain-imaging studies where meditators report successfully following an instruction to enter a state of “contentless stillness,” “content-minimized awareness,” “thoughtless emptiness,” or similar (e.g., Hinterberger et al.,
2014; Winter et al.,
2020), researchers should bear in mind that there may be considerable diversity in the experiences attained. Detailed self-reporting of meditation states, like that in the present study (see also, e.g., Costines et al.,
2021; Gamma & Metzinger,
2021; Nave et al.,
2021), may in cases be required to avoid inappropriately conflating imaging data from quite different experiences.
In each of the three traditions, the goal-states are said to produce major benefits in terms of mental health and well-being (e.g., Meares,
1986; Wallace,
2006). One of the key benefits is said to be that meditators retain some of the calm and relaxation from the meditation, with the effect that anxiety is reduced in daily life (e.g., Meares,
1986; Wallace,
2007). In the present study, participants in each practice reported that their experience was highly calm, peaceful, relaxing, positive, pleasant, and good. These findings suggest the need for clinical research in this area, investigating the benefits of experiencing stillness/silence in these and other practices, including any impact on anxiety.
Our findings can contribute to recommendations by clinicians that are personalized to individual clients, matching a particular practice to the client’s needs and preferences (Kok & Singer,
2017). Further work is required, but if it does turn out that Stillness Meditation achieves a deep and absorptive experience reasonably quickly and easily, that is one factor that should be taken into account in making such recommendations. If it turns out that Shamatha and Thai Forest lead to a more vivid and wakeful experience due to active training of attention, that should also be taken into account. Another line of research will be required to examine clinical benefits associated with these or other differences in experience.
Limitations and Future Research Directions
The study has a high degree of ecological validity, but a corollary of this is that there are differences between the practice groups besides the different techniques. We have already mentioned the differences with respect to lifetime hours practice, hours of practice in the target period, reasons for practising, and practice environment (i.e., retreat vs. classes/home). Differences on such variables could contribute to the differences on the dimensional, proportion, and confidence items, although our analyses accounting for differences in lifetime hours and reasons for practising provide a level of comfort.
Use of active control groups was designed to reduce differences between the groups on other non-specific factors such as social desirability, investment, and aspirational biases, but in each practice such factors were likely present to some degree, and the groups could still differ on some or all of them. It could be, for example, that the way a practice is taught in a particular tradition leads to more pronounced pressures to report a certain type of experience than in the other traditions. Cultural and linguistic differences across the groups could also have influenced the findings. Nearly a third of the Thai Forest participants (27.5%) reported that English was not the primary language that they spoke at home, as compared to 2.4% for Shamatha and 1.1% for Stillness Meditation. However, the Thai Forest retreats were conducted in Australia, the teaching was all in English, and none of the participants expressed any difficulty in understanding the questionnaire.
To eliminate group differences such as those above, the ideal approach would be a longitudinal study with randomization of participants to the practice groups and matching of the practices on key aspects extrinsic to the techniques themselves (e.g., having all participants attend classes rather than retreats) (Davidson & Kaszniak,
2015; Lindahl et al.,
2017; Slagter et al.,
2011; Tang et al.,
2015). However, for practical reasons, continuing such a study until participants achieve advanced levels is unlikely to be feasible.
Another consideration is the delay between participants’ experience of stillness/silence and their completion of the questionnaire. Shamatha and Thai Forest participants were asked about their deepest experience on the relevant retreat and on average completed the questionnaire 3 days after the retreat concluded. Stillness Meditation participants were asked about their deepest experience in the previous 7 days of class and home practice. On average, only a small proportion of all participants across the three practices responded “I don’t remember or can’t say” for the dimensional items, and those answers were excluded from analyses. However, any delay between experience and report provides scope for failures of memory and associated biases and distortions (Hurlburt & Schwitzgebel,
2007). Future studies could further minimize the delay. Researchers could even interrupt a meditation session to obtain reports, using experience sampling or other methods (see, e.g., Petitmengin et al.,
2017; Rodriguez-Larios & Alaerts,
2021).
Seventeen of the 88 Stillness Meditation participants (19.3%) completed the questionnaire 8 days or more after their most recent class, and therefore could not have undertaken a class during the target period. Some of those participants did only a small amount of practice in the target period, and it is possible that they reported on previous experiences in Stillness Meditation rather than on their experience in the target period. Participants were, however, asked to focus on the target period, and they could still provide legitimate reports where they had done only modest practice in that period. There was no significant correlation between the time it took Stillness Meditation participants to complete the questionnaire after their most recent class, and their ratings on any of the seven dimensional items for which we found the most robust differences vis-à-vis Shamatha and Thai Forest (range ρ = 0.00–0.21).
In the present study, we covered a large number of dimensions, but there were some that we left out. For example, attentional stability and luminosity are referred to frequently in the texts on Shamatha but not the other two practices. We did not cover those dimensions as we thought they might be meaningless or confusing for large numbers of meditators without further explanation. Future studies could analyze the dimensions that we omitted.
The study was not pre-registered. While this represents a limitation, the study was exploratory in nature and therefore pre-specification of outcomes is not only challenging but may not be necessary from an open science perspective (see e.g., Scheel et al.,
2021).
In this “Discussion” section, we have identified a number of areas where future research would be valuable. A further candidate for such research is the possible explanations that we have put forward for the differences in the reported experiences across the three practices. Such research could investigate which of the explanations is correct, whether they each apply to some degree, or whether some other explanation is needed.
Future research could also investigate the basis on which participants give ratings for the dimensional items. For example, where a participant reports little awareness of thoughts, is this because any thoughts are subtle, because they are infrequent, because the meditator is not engaging with them, or is there some other reason? Would the participant report greater awareness of thoughts if their mindfulness/attentional skills were more developed? Where a participant reports a high degree of calm, is this because the calm was intense, deep, profound, all three, and/or had some other experiential quality?
Scientific research on meditation has tended to focus on a narrow range of practices, and there are many other traditions that have not yet been explored (Dahl et al.,
2015; Goleman & Davidson,
2017; Matko et al.,
2021). Future research should examine Shamatha, Thai Forest, and Stillness Meditation, as well as other practices. A range of methods should be used, since all methods for investigating experience have unique strengths and serious limitations (Berkovich-Ohana et al.,
2020; Hurlburt & Schwitzgebel,
2007,
2011). Methods could include approaches that target experience directly, such as microphenomenology (e.g., Nave et al.,
2021; Petitmengin et al.,
2019; Przyrembel & Singer,
2018), and techniques such as brain-imaging (e.g., Hernández et al.,
2018; Mahone et al.,
2018; Winter et al.,
2020; Zanesco et al.,
2021), physiological measurement using wearable devices (e.g., Steinhubl et al.,
2015), and attentional testing (e.g., Lutz et al.,
2015; Shields et al.,
2020) that may indirectly tell us about the experience.
Future research could also compare broader aspects of the practices. These include theoretical, metaphysical, teleological, soteriological, therapeutic, and cultural understandings and contexts. Comparative work of this kind could help to understand whether and how such factors influence participant reports of experiences.
Practices like Shamatha, Thai Forest, and Stillness Meditation, which aim for calm, tranquility, or quiescence, are frequently contrasted with other practices that are explicitly concerned with developing insight (e.g., Goleman,
1988; Rapgay & Bystrisky,
2009; Sharf,
1995; Wallace,
2011a,
2018). Calm practices can also lead to forms of insight though—for example, insight into the nature of the mind and into one’s place and purpose in the world (e.g., Brahm,
2014; Meares,
1986). Such insights can arise both during and after practice, and would be an interesting subject for future research.