Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder, defined by persistent patterns of inattention and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity, causing impairment in several life domains (American Psychiatric Association,
2013). ADHD is one of the most prevalent psychiatric disorders in adolescence (Polanczyk et al.,
2014), and besides personal impairment, the financial burden of ADHD on society is high (Robb et al.,
2011). A substantial part of these costs is related to risk-taking behavior (RTB; Matza et al.,
2005). ADHD is associated with several forms of real life risk-taking behavior (RTB) like substance abuse, reckless driving, gambling or unsafe sex (see Pollak et al.,
2019 for a review). Also on laboratory risk-taking tasks, children and adolescents with ADHD demonstrate more risk taking than controls (see Dekkers et al.,
2016 for a meta-analysis). Given the vulnerability of adolescents to RTB in general (Crone & Dahl,
2012), and considering that adolescents with ADHD seem even more vulnerable to RTB, it is important to study underlying mechanisms that can help understand and ultimately reduce RTB in this group. In the current investigation, we focus on one such putative mechanism – peer influence.
Susceptibility to Peer Influence
Adolescence is a period of elevated peer influence on RTB (Somerville,
2013; Steinberg & Morris,
2001). Several experimental studies demonstrate that the presence of and/or the encouragement by peers increases risk taking in typically developing adolescents (Cavalca et al.,
2013; Chein et al.,
2011; Gardner & Steinberg,
2005; Rhodes et al.,
2015; Smith et al.,
2014; van Hoorn et al.,
2017; Weigard et al.,
2014). Real-life data reveal that among adolescent drivers, risky driving and the risk of fatal injuries increases with more same-aged passengers in the car (Chen et al.,
2000; Ouimet et al.,
2010; Simons-Morton et al.,
2011,
2012).
According to the widely used dual-systems model, this increase in RTB during adolescence can be explained by a more rapid development of socioemotional brain systems relative to cognitive control systems, causing an increase in reward-seeking behavior (Steinberg,
2010; Strang et al.,
2013). Because peers trigger socioemotional brain systems by activating reward-related regions like the ventral striatum (Chein et al.,
2011; Gardner & Steinberg,
2005; Somerville,
2013), and peer presence is associated with an increase in the subjective value of immediate rewards (Albert et al.,
2013), more cognitive control is required to control behavior in the presence of peers. As ADHD is characterized by pronounced inhibitory deficits and a delay in cortical maturation (Barkley,
1997; Lijffijt et al.,
2005; Rubia et al.,
2005; Shaw et al.,
2007,
2018), it follows that a larger imbalance between these brain systems is to be expected in this group (Sonuga-Barke,
2003), potentially making adolescents with ADHD unusually susceptible to peer influence.
Social factors may also be related to elevated susceptibility to peer influence in adolescents with ADHD. Generally, adolescents with social problems and weak social skills are most susceptible to peer influence (Allen et al.,
2012; Steinberg et al.,
1994; Urberg et al.,
2003). Adolescents with ADHD experience myriad social problems. Specifically, ADHD is associated with a wide range of socially inadequate behaviors such as social intrusiveness, difficulties attuning social behavior, violation of social rules, socially dominant behavior, verbal aggression, talking when inappropriate, and being easily distracted in conversation (Huang-Pollock et al.,
2009; Nijmeijer et al.,
2008). These behaviors are associated with lower popularity among peers (Bagwell et al.,
2001; Hoza et al.,
2005). Children with ADHD also encounter more peer rejection relative to their peers without ADHD (de Boo & Prins,
2007; Hoza,
2007), which persists into adolescence (Bagwell et al.,
2001). Peer rejection in turn may increase susceptibility to peer influence by a process called reputation management: displaying RTB to gain status and/or to avoid rejection (Brechwald & Prinstein,
2011). Indeed, peer rejection in adolescents with ADHD is associated with externalizing disorders, antisocial behavior and substance use (Greene,
1997; Mikami & Hinshaw,
2006; Mrug et al.,
2012).
To summarize, several lines of evidence lead us to hypothesize that adolescents with ADHD are more susceptible to peer influence, which has the potential to increase RTB. This hypothesis has – to our knowledge – never been tested. In the current preregistered study, we therefore investigate whether adolescents with ADHD are more susceptible to peer influence than are typically developing (TD) adolescents. To do this, we developed a paradigm combining risk taking and peer influence. A virtual risk-encouraging peer was integrated in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (Lejuez et al.,
2003), which was administered twice (peer and solo condition).
In adolescence, peer influence typically imposes stress (Byrne et al.,
2007), potentially because of the fear of exclusion and feelings of need-to-belong that peers may trigger (Baumeister et al.,
2005; Pickett et al.,
2004). For example, in typically developing adolescents, unexpected social rejection is associated with a parasympathetic response (Gunther Moor et al.,
2014). In adolescents with ADHD, the physiological effects of peer influence are yet unknown. As adolescents with ADHD experience more peer rejection relative to TD adolescents (Bagwell et al.,
2001; de Boo & Prins,
2007), we reasoned that peer influence may elicit increased stress (i.e., ANS reactivity) in the ADHD group. However, a recent meta-analysis indicates that ADHD is mostly associated with physiological
hypoactivation, although results are mixed and most studies investigated this during resting state or cognitive tasks; evidence on physiological reactivity to social information is heterogeneous and inconclusive (Bellato et al.,
2020).
In the non-preregistered part of current study, we assess physiological responding to peer influence by measuring autonomic nervous system (ANS) reactivity. We tested if adolescents with and without ADHD differed in ANS reactivity to peer influence. In addition, as fear of exclusion can promote risk taking (Pickett et al.,
2004), we reasoned that increased ANS reactivity is linked to more risk taking in the peer condition, thereby testing the link between physiological and behavioral effects of peer influence.