Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2008

01-05-2008 | Original Article

Offline and online automatic number comparison

Auteurs: Filip Van Opstal, Agnes Moors, Wim Fias, Tom Verguts

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This paper focuses on automatic number processing as instantiated in the size congruity effect. It was recently argued that long-term “associations between individual digits and the attributes ‘small’ and ‘large’ create the size congruity effect” (Choplin and Logan 2003, abstract, p. 17). Moreover, these authors proposed the additional assumption that the relevant connections are acquired over a lifetime of experience with numbers. We show that at least one of these assumptions is not true: either the size congruity effect derives from an (online) comparison effect between two numbers at the time of stimulus presentation (violating the first assumption) or the relevant connections flexibly change (offline) between trials during the course of one experimental session (violating the second assumption).
Voetnoten
1
Other researchers already obtained size congruity effects with single digits (e.g., Algom, Dekel, & Pansky, 1996; Schwarz & Ischebeck, 2000; Tzelgov et al., 1992), but in their experiments, trials on which numerical size was irrelevant were always mixed with trials on which it was relevant (either blocked or not). On the relevant trials, participants were instructed to compare the numerical size of the presented digit with the numerical size of a fixed standard. Because of this, one could argue that participants continued engaging in a numerical comparison with the standard on the irrelevant trials, despite the instruction to respond on the basis of physical size.
 
2
The interaction test between response (small/large) and number (4/5) is equivalent to a contrast that assigns a weight of +1 to the two “response = small, number = 4” conditions and the two “response = large, number = 5” conditions, and a weight of -1 to the other four conditions. Alternatively, the eight conditions can be rearranged in a congruity (congruent/incongruent) × distance (small/large) × response (small/large) format. The interaction between congruity and distance is equivalent to a contrast that assigns a weight of +1 to the two “congruity = incongruent, distance = small” conditions and to the two “congruity = congruent, distance = large” conditions, and a weight of −1 to the other four conditions. The weight assignments are equivalent for the two situations, and therefore the response × number and congruity × distance interactions are equivalent.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Algom D., Dekel A., & Pansky, A. (1996). The perception of number from the separability of the stimulus: The Stroop effect revisited. Memory & Cognition, 24, 557–572. Algom D., Dekel A., & Pansky, A. (1996). The perception of number from the separability of the stimulus: The Stroop effect revisited. Memory & Cognition, 24, 557–572.
go back to reference Choplin, J. M. & Logan, G. D. (2003). Automaticity of number perception. In R. Alterman, & D. Kirsh (Eds), Proceedings of the Twenty-fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 252–257. Choplin, J. M. & Logan, G. D. (2003). Automaticity of number perception. In R. Alterman, & D. Kirsh (Eds), Proceedings of the Twenty-fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 252–257.
go back to reference Choplin, J. M., & Logan, G. D. (2005). A memory-based account of automatic numerosity processing. Memory & Cognition, 33, 17–28. Choplin, J. M., & Logan, G. D. (2005). A memory-based account of automatic numerosity processing. Memory & Cognition, 33, 17–28.
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 95–110.CrossRef Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 95–110.CrossRef
go back to reference Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 659–673.CrossRef Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 659–673.CrossRef
go back to reference Gevers, W., Ratinckx, E., De Baene, W., & Fias, W. (2006a). Implicit number–space links activate spatial responses: Evidence from the LRP. Experimental Psychology, 53, 58–68.PubMed Gevers, W., Ratinckx, E., De Baene, W., & Fias, W. (2006a). Implicit number–space links activate spatial responses: Evidence from the LRP. Experimental Psychology, 53, 58–68.PubMed
go back to reference Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006b). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 32–44.PubMedCrossRef Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006b). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 32–44.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Memory & Cognition, 10, 389–395. Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Memory & Cognition, 10, 389–395.
go back to reference Lammertyn, J., & Fias, W. (2005). Negative priming with numbers: No evidence for a semantic locus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: A, 58, 1153–1172. Lammertyn, J., & Fias, W. (2005). Negative priming with numbers: No evidence for a semantic locus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: A, 58, 1153–1172.
go back to reference Leth-Steensen, C., & Marley, A. A. J. (2000). A model of response time effects in symbolic comparison. Psychological Review, 107, 62–100.PubMedCrossRef Leth-Steensen, C., & Marley, A. A. J. (2000). A model of response time effects in symbolic comparison. Psychological Review, 107, 62–100.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef
go back to reference Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326.PubMedCrossRef Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.PubMedCrossRef Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80, 215–229.PubMedCrossRef Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80, 215–229.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pansky, A., & Algom, D. (1999). Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgment of numerals: The role of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 39–58.CrossRef Pansky, A., & Algom, D. (1999). Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgment of numerals: The role of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 39–58.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwarz, W., & Heinze, H. J. (1998). On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: A behavioral and event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1167–1179.PubMedCrossRef Schwarz, W., & Heinze, H. J. (1998). On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: A behavioral and event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1167–1179.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schwarz, W., & Ischebeck, A. (2000). Sequential effects in number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1606–1621.PubMedCrossRef Schwarz, W., & Ischebeck, A. (2000). Sequential effects in number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1606–1621.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sergent, C., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychological Science, 15, 720–728.PubMedCrossRef Sergent, C., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychological Science, 15, 720–728.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tzelgov, J., Meyer, J., & Henik, A. (1992). Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 166–179.CrossRef Tzelgov, J., Meyer, J., & Henik, A. (1992). Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 166–179.CrossRef
go back to reference Verguts, T., Fias, W., & Stevens, M. (2005). A model of exact small-number representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 66–80. Verguts, T., Fias, W., & Stevens, M. (2005). A model of exact small-number representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 66–80.
Metagegevens
Titel
Offline and online automatic number comparison
Auteurs
Filip Van Opstal
Agnes Moors
Wim Fias
Tom Verguts
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-007-0108-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2008

Psychological Research 3/2008 Naar de uitgave