Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2006

01-01-2006 | Original Article

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints

Auteurs: Arno Koning, Rob van Lier

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The presupposed advantage of symmetrical objects over asymmetrical objects was investigated in an object matching task, using accidental and non-accidental viewpoints. In addition, the accidental views could be symmetric or asymmetric. When two non-accidental views were presented, symmetrical objects were matched faster than asymmetrical objects. When an accidental view was presented first (followed by a non-accidental view), the matching of symmetrical objects was equal to that of asymmetrical objects. When a non-accidental view was presented first (followed by an accidental view), matching was again equal for the symmetrical and asymmetrical objects, although much faster compared with the opposite sequence of presented views. No effects of image symmetry in the accidental viewpoints were found. Apparently, the advantage of symmetrical objects over asymmetrical objects is only present in object matching when 3-D object structures are visible.
Voetnoten
1
It has been argued that there is a preferred, or canonical, view for the recognition of objects (Palmer, Rosche, & Chase, 1981). Palmer et al. define an object’s canonical view as “the view that reveals the most information of greatest salience about it.” However, although a person may have a strong intuition as to what the canonical view might be for any given object, this cannot be easily generalized across objects. In the study by Palmer et al., most objects were preferably viewed from slightly above, with both the front and the side of the objects clearly visible. Similar results were found by Verfaillie and Boutsen (1995). Even though the canonical views used here might not be the best possible views of the objects, the term C-view is used to contrast it with the term A-view
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bar, M. (2001) Viewpoint dependency in visual object recognition does not necessarily imply viewer-centered representation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 793–799. Bar, M. (2001) Viewpoint dependency in visual object recognition does not necessarily imply viewer-centered representation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 793–799.
go back to reference Barlow, H. B., & Reeves, B. C. (1979). The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays. Vision Research, 19, 783–793.CrossRefPubMed Barlow, H. B., & Reeves, B. C. (1979). The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays. Vision Research, 19, 783–793.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1994). Parallel computation of symmetry but not repetition within single visual shapes. Visual Cognition, 1, 377–400. Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1994). Parallel computation of symmetry but not repetition within single visual shapes. Visual Cognition, 1, 377–400.
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., Corballis, M., & Lawson, R. (1998). The influence of perceived rotary motion on the recognition of objects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 140–146. Jolicoeur, P., Corballis, M., & Lawson, R. (1998). The influence of perceived rotary motion on the recognition of objects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 140–146.
go back to reference Kahn, J. I., & Foster, D. H. (1986). Horizontal–vertical structure in the visual comparison of rigidly transformed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 422–433.CrossRefPubMed Kahn, J. I., & Foster, D. H. (1986). Horizontal–vertical structure in the visual comparison of rigidly transformed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 422–433.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Large, M., McMullen, P. A., & Hamm, J. P. (2003). The role of axes of elongation and symmetry in rotated object naming. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 1–19.PubMed Large, M., McMullen, P. A., & Hamm, J. P. (2003). The role of axes of elongation and symmetry in rotated object naming. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 1–19.PubMed
go back to reference Lawson, R. (1999). Achieving visual object constancy across plane rotation and depth rotation. Acta Psychologica, 102, 221–245.CrossRefPubMed Lawson, R. (1999). Achieving visual object constancy across plane rotation and depth rotation. Acta Psychologica, 102, 221–245.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lawson, R., & Humphreys, G. W. (1998). View-specific effects of depth rotations and foreshortening on the initial recognition and priming of familiar objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 60, 1052–1066.PubMed Lawson, R., & Humphreys, G. W. (1998). View-specific effects of depth rotations and foreshortening on the initial recognition and priming of familiar objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 60, 1052–1066.PubMed
go back to reference Mach, E. (1886). Beiträge zur Analyze der Empfindungen. Jena, Germany: Fisher. Mach, E. (1886). Beiträge zur Analyze der Empfindungen. Jena, Germany: Fisher.
go back to reference McMullen, P. A., & Farah, M. J. (1991). Viewer-centered and object-centered representations in the recognition of naturalistic line drawings. Psychological Science, 2, 275–277. McMullen, P. A., & Farah, M. J. (1991). Viewer-centered and object-centered representations in the recognition of naturalistic line drawings. Psychological Science, 2, 275–277.
go back to reference Palmer, S. E., & Hemenway, K. (1978). Orientation and symmetry: Effects of multiple, rotational, and near symmetries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 691–702.CrossRefPubMed Palmer, S. E., & Hemenway, K. (1978). Orientation and symmetry: Effects of multiple, rotational, and near symmetries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 691–702.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Palmer, S. E., Rosch, E., & Chase, P. (1981). Canonical perspective and the perception of objects. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and Performance IX. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Palmer, S. E., Rosch, E., & Chase, P. (1981). Canonical perspective and the perception of objects. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and Performance IX. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Rock, I., & Leaman, R. (1963). An experimental analysis of visual symmetry. Acta Psychologica, 21, 171–183.CrossRef Rock, I., & Leaman, R. (1963). An experimental analysis of visual symmetry. Acta Psychologica, 21, 171–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Tarr, M. J., & Pinker, S. (1990). When does human object recognition use a viewer-centered reference frame? Psychological Science, 1, 253–256. Tarr, M. J., & Pinker, S. (1990). When does human object recognition use a viewer-centered reference frame? Psychological Science, 1, 253–256.
go back to reference Troje, N. F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (1996). Face recognition under varying poses: The role of texture and shape. Vision Research, 36, 1761–1771.CrossRefPubMed Troje, N. F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (1996). Face recognition under varying poses: The role of texture and shape. Vision Research, 36, 1761–1771.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van der Helm, P. A., & Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1996). Goodness of visual regularities: A nontransformational approach. Psychological Review, 103, 429–456.CrossRefPubMed Van der Helm, P. A., & Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1996). Goodness of visual regularities: A nontransformational approach. Psychological Review, 103, 429–456.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Lier, R. J., & Wagemans, J. (1999). From images to objects: Global and local completions of self-occluded parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1721–1741.CrossRef Van Lier, R. J., & Wagemans, J. (1999). From images to objects: Global and local completions of self-occluded parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1721–1741.CrossRef
go back to reference Verfaillie, K., & Boutsen, L. (1995). A corpus of 714 full-color images of depth-rotated objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 925–961.PubMed Verfaillie, K., & Boutsen, L. (1995). A corpus of 714 full-color images of depth-rotated objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 925–961.PubMed
go back to reference Vetter, T., & Poggio, T. (1994). Symmetric 3D objects are an easy case for 2D object recognition. Spatial Vision, 8, 443–453.PubMed Vetter, T., & Poggio, T. (1994). Symmetric 3D objects are an easy case for 2D object recognition. Spatial Vision, 8, 443–453.PubMed
go back to reference Wagemans, J. (1995). Detection of visual symmetries. Spatial Vision, 9, 9–32.PubMed Wagemans, J. (1995). Detection of visual symmetries. Spatial Vision, 9, 9–32.PubMed
go back to reference Wenderoth, P. (1995). The role of pattern outline in bilateral symmetry detection with briefly flashed dot patterns. Spatial Vision, 9, 57–77.PubMed Wenderoth, P. (1995). The role of pattern outline in bilateral symmetry detection with briefly flashed dot patterns. Spatial Vision, 9, 57–77.PubMed
go back to reference Willems, B., & Wagemans, J. (2001). Matching multicomponent objects from different viewpoints: Mental rotation as normalization? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1090–1115.CrossRefPubMed Willems, B., & Wagemans, J. (2001). Matching multicomponent objects from different viewpoints: Mental rotation as normalization? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1090–1115.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wilson, K. D., & Farah, M. J. (2003). When does the visual system use viewpoint-invariant representations during recognition? Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 399–415.CrossRefPubMed Wilson, K. D., & Farah, M. J. (2003). When does the visual system use viewpoint-invariant representations during recognition? Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 399–415.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints
Auteurs
Arno Koning
Rob van Lier
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0191-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2006

Psychological Research 1/2006 Naar de uitgave