Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2006

01-01-2006 | Original Article

Distance cognition in virtual environmental space: Further investigations to clarify the route-angularity effect

Auteurs: Petra Jansen-Osmann, Gunnar Wiedenbauer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Two experiments with 72 participants in total investigated the route-angularity effect. This effect is shown when a greater number of turns along a route increase the estimated length of this route. In this study it was shown that the route-angularity effect is likely to be a memory-based effect depending on task difficulty. The important factor seems to be how heavily memory is loaded during learning. The route-angularity effect even appears in intentional learning, when memory is loaded heavily. Under this learning condition participants know beforehand that they have to estimate distances. All experiments were conducted in a controlled virtual environment, which allows a reliable investigation of distance estimations in environmental space.
Literatuur
go back to reference Albert, W. S., Rensink, R. A., & Beusmanns, J. (1999). Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 131–144.CrossRef Albert, W. S., Rensink, R. A., & Beusmanns, J. (1999). Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 131–144.CrossRef
go back to reference Allen, G. (1981). A developmental perspective on the effects of “subdividing” macrospatial experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 120–132.CrossRef Allen, G. (1981). A developmental perspective on the effects of “subdividing” macrospatial experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 120–132.CrossRef
go back to reference Allen, G. L. (1988). The acquisition of spatial knowledge under conditions of temporospatial discontinuity. Psychological Research, 50, 183–190.PubMed Allen, G. L. (1988). The acquisition of spatial knowledge under conditions of temporospatial discontinuity. Psychological Research, 50, 183–190.PubMed
go back to reference Allen, G. L., & Kirasic, K. C. (1985). Effects of the cognitive organization of route knowledge on judgements of macrospatial distance. Memory & Cognition, 13, 218–227. Allen, G. L., & Kirasic, K. C. (1985). Effects of the cognitive organization of route knowledge on judgements of macrospatial distance. Memory & Cognition, 13, 218–227.
go back to reference Cohen, R., Weatherford, D. L., Lomenick, T., & Koeller, K. (1979). Development of spatial representations: The role of task demands and familiarity with the environment. Child Development, 50, 1257–1260.PubMed Cohen, R., Weatherford, D. L., Lomenick, T., & Koeller, K. (1979). Development of spatial representations: The role of task demands and familiarity with the environment. Child Development, 50, 1257–1260.PubMed
go back to reference Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhardt, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Behavior, 11, 671–684. Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhardt, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Behavior, 11, 671–684.
go back to reference Eysenck, M. W. (1979). Depth, elaboration, and distinctiveness. In L. S. Cermak, & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.). Levels of processing in human memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eysenck, M. W. (1979). Depth, elaboration, and distinctiveness. In L. S. Cermak, & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.). Levels of processing in human memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Gillner, S., & Mallot, H. (1998). Navigation and acquisition of spatial knowledge in a virtual maze. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 445–463.CrossRefPubMed Gillner, S., & Mallot, H. (1998). Navigation and acquisition of spatial knowledge in a virtual maze. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 445–463.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Herman, J. F., Norton, L. M., & Klein, C. A. (1986). Children’s distance estimates in a large-scale environment: A search for the route angularity effect. Environment and Behavior, 18, 533–558. Herman, J. F., Norton, L. M., & Klein, C. A. (1986). Children’s distance estimates in a large-scale environment: A search for the route angularity effect. Environment and Behavior, 18, 533–558.
go back to reference Holyoak, K., & Mah, W. (1982). Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitude. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 328–352. Holyoak, K., & Mah, W. (1982). Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitude. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 328–352.
go back to reference Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimation spatial location. Psychological Review, 98, 352–376.CrossRefPubMed Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimation spatial location. Psychological Review, 98, 352–376.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jansen-Osmann, P. (2001). Der Einfluss der Gestaltung einer Route auf das Distanzwissen. [The influence of environmental features of a route on distance knowledge.] Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 327–338. Jansen-Osmann, P. (2001). Der Einfluss der Gestaltung einer Route auf das Distanzwissen. [The influence of environmental features of a route on distance knowledge.] Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 327–338.
go back to reference Jansen-Osmann, P. (2002). Using desktop virtual environments to investigate the role of landmarks. Computers in Human Behavior, 18, 427–436.CrossRef Jansen-Osmann, P. (2002). Using desktop virtual environments to investigate the role of landmarks. Computers in Human Behavior, 18, 427–436.CrossRef
go back to reference Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2002). Investigating distance knowledge using virtual environments. Environment and Behavior, 34, 178–193.CrossRef Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2002). Investigating distance knowledge using virtual environments. Environment and Behavior, 34, 178–193.CrossRef
go back to reference Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2004). What makes a route appear longer? An experimental and computational perspective on features, route segmentation, and distance knowledge. Manuscript submitted for publication. Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2004). What makes a route appear longer? An experimental and computational perspective on features, route segmentation, and distance knowledge. Manuscript submitted for publication.
go back to reference Jansen-Osmann, P., & Wiedenbauer, G. (2004). The influence of turns on distance cognition: New experimental approaches to clarify the route-angularity effect. Environment and Behavior, in press. Jansen-Osmann, P., & Wiedenbauer, G. (2004). The influence of turns on distance cognition: New experimental approaches to clarify the route-angularity effect. Environment and Behavior, in press.
go back to reference Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.) (1982). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.) (1982). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., Cicenelli, J. G., Doherty, S., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1990). Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 19–43.PubMed Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., Cicenelli, J. G., Doherty, S., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1990). Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 19–43.PubMed
go back to reference Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R., Golledge, R. G., & Philbeck, J. W. (1999). Human navigation by path integration. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding: cognitive mapping and spatial behavior. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press. Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R., Golledge, R. G., & Philbeck, J. W. (1999). Human navigation by path integration. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding: cognitive mapping and spatial behavior. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.
go back to reference Mallot, H.-P. (1999). Spatial cognition: Behavioral competences, neural mechanisms and evolutionary scaling. Kognitionswissenschaft, 8, 40–48.CrossRef Mallot, H.-P. (1999). Spatial cognition: Behavioral competences, neural mechanisms and evolutionary scaling. Kognitionswissenschaft, 8, 40–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Mandler, J. M., Seegmiller, D., & Day, J. (1977). On the coding of spatial information. Memory & Cognition, 5, 10–16. Mandler, J. M., Seegmiller, D., & Day, J. (1977). On the coding of spatial information. Memory & Cognition, 5, 10–16.
go back to reference Manktelow, K. (1999). Reasoning and thinking. Hove, UK: Psychology. Manktelow, K. (1999). Reasoning and thinking. Hove, UK: Psychology.
go back to reference May, M., & Klatzky, R. L. (2000). Path integration while ignoring irrelevant movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 169–186. May, M., & Klatzky, R. L. (2000). Path integration while ignoring irrelevant movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 169–186.
go back to reference McNamara, T. P., & Diwadkar, V. A. (1997). Symmetry and asymmetry of human spatial memory. Cognitive Psychology, 34, 160–190.CrossRefPubMed McNamara, T. P., & Diwadkar, V. A. (1997). Symmetry and asymmetry of human spatial memory. Cognitive Psychology, 34, 160–190.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Milgram, S. (1973). Introduction. In W. H. Ittelson (Ed.), Environment and Cognition (pp. 21–27). New York: Seminary. Milgram, S. (1973). Introduction. In W. H. Ittelson (Ed.), Environment and Cognition (pp. 21–27). New York: Seminary.
go back to reference Montello, D. R. (1995). The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Processes and knowledge sources. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Santa Barbara. Montello, D. R. (1995). The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Processes and knowledge sources. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Santa Barbara.
go back to reference Montello, D. R. (1997). The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Direct sources of information. In S. C. Hirtle, & A. U. Frank (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 297–311). Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 1329. Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Montello, D. R. (1997). The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Direct sources of information. In S. C. Hirtle, & A. U. Frank (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 297–311). Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 1329. Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Beck, J. L., Bottalico, K. S., & Molloy, R. D. (1990). Effects of intentional versus incidental learning on explicit and implicit tests of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 457–463. Neill, W. T., Beck, J. L., Bottalico, K. S., & Molloy, R. D. (1990). Effects of intentional versus incidental learning on explicit and implicit tests of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 457–463.
go back to reference Pelham, B. W., Sumarta, T. T., & Myaskovsky, L. (1994). The easy path from many to much: the numerosity heuristic. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 103–133.CrossRef Pelham, B. W., Sumarta, T. T., & Myaskovsky, L. (1994). The easy path from many to much: the numerosity heuristic. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 103–133.CrossRef
go back to reference Péruch, P., Belingard, L., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (2000). Transfer of spatial knowledge from virtual to real environments. In C. Freska, W. Bauer, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial Cognition II, LNAI 1849. Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Péruch, P., Belingard, L., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (2000). Transfer of spatial knowledge from virtual to real environments. In C. Freska, W. Bauer, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial Cognition II, LNAI 1849. Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Radvansky, G. A., Carlson-Radvansky, L., & Irwin, D. (1995). Uncertainty in estimating distance from memory. Memory & Cognition, 23, 596–606. Radvansky, G. A., Carlson-Radvansky, L., & Irwin, D. (1995). Uncertainty in estimating distance from memory. Memory & Cognition, 23, 596–606.
go back to reference Ruddle, R. A., Payne, S. J., & Jones, D. M. (1997). Navigating buildings in “desk-top” virtual environments: Experimental investigations using extended navigational experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 143–159. Ruddle, R. A., Payne, S. J., & Jones, D. M. (1997). Navigating buildings in “desk-top” virtual environments: Experimental investigations using extended navigational experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 143–159.
go back to reference Sadalla, E. K., & Magel, S. G. (1980). The perception of traversed distance. Environment and Behavior, 12, 65–79. Sadalla, E. K., & Magel, S. G. (1980). The perception of traversed distance. Environment and Behavior, 12, 65–79.
go back to reference Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. (1980a). An information storage model for distance cognition. Environment and Behavior, 12, 183–193. Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. (1980a). An information storage model for distance cognition. Environment and Behavior, 12, 183–193.
go back to reference Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. (1980b). The perception of traversed distance: Intersections. Environment and Behavior, 12, 167–182. Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. (1980b). The perception of traversed distance: Intersections. Environment and Behavior, 12, 167–182.
go back to reference Sadalla, E. K., Staplin, L. J., & Burroughs, W. J. (1979). Retrieval processes in distance cognition. Memory & Cognition, 7, 291–296. Sadalla, E. K., Staplin, L. J., & Burroughs, W. J. (1979). Retrieval processes in distance cognition. Memory & Cognition, 7, 291–296.
go back to reference Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 560–589.PubMed Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 560–589.PubMed
go back to reference Tversky, B. (2003). Structures of mental spaces: How people think about space. Environment and Behavior, 35, 66–80.CrossRef Tversky, B. (2003). Structures of mental spaces: How people think about space. Environment and Behavior, 35, 66–80.CrossRef
go back to reference Waller, D., Knapp, D., & Hunt, E. (2001). Spatial representations of virtual mazes: The role of visual fidelity and individual differences. Human Factors, 43, 147–158.PubMed Waller, D., Knapp, D., & Hunt, E. (2001). Spatial representations of virtual mazes: The role of visual fidelity and individual differences. Human Factors, 43, 147–158.PubMed
go back to reference Westerman, S. J., Cribbin, T., & Wilson, R. (2001). Virtual information space navigation: Evaluating the use of head tracking. Behaviour and Information Technology, 20, 419–426.CrossRef Westerman, S. J., Cribbin, T., & Wilson, R. (2001). Virtual information space navigation: Evaluating the use of head tracking. Behaviour and Information Technology, 20, 419–426.CrossRef
go back to reference Wiest, W., & Bell, B. (1985). Stevens’s exponent for psychophysical scaling of perceived, remembered and inferred distance. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 457–479.CrossRefPubMed Wiest, W., & Bell, B. (1985). Stevens’s exponent for psychophysical scaling of perceived, remembered and inferred distance. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 457–479.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Willemsen, P., & Gooch, A. A. (2002). Perceived egocentric distances in real, image-based, and traditional virtual environments. In Proceedings of the IEEE Virtual Reality 2002 (VR.02). Willemsen, P., & Gooch, A. A. (2002). Perceived egocentric distances in real, image-based, and traditional virtual environments. In Proceedings of the IEEE Virtual Reality 2002 (VR.02).
go back to reference Witmer, B. G., & Kline, P. B. (1998). Judging perceived and traversed distance in virtual environments. Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments, 7, 144–167.CrossRef Witmer, B. G., & Kline, P. B. (1998). Judging perceived and traversed distance in virtual environments. Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments, 7, 144–167.CrossRef
go back to reference Witmer, B. G., & Sadowski, W. J. (1998). Nonvisually guided locomotion to a previously viewed target in real and virtual environments. Human Factors, 40, 478–488. Witmer, B. G., & Sadowski, W. J. (1998). Nonvisually guided locomotion to a previously viewed target in real and virtual environments. Human Factors, 40, 478–488.
go back to reference Witmer, B. G., Bailey, J. H., Knerr, B. W., & Parsons, K. C. (1996). Virtual spaces and real world places: Transfer of route knowledge. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 45, 413–428.CrossRef Witmer, B. G., Bailey, J. H., Knerr, B. W., & Parsons, K. C. (1996). Virtual spaces and real world places: Transfer of route knowledge. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 45, 413–428.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Distance cognition in virtual environmental space: Further investigations to clarify the route-angularity effect
Auteurs
Petra Jansen-Osmann
Gunnar Wiedenbauer
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0183-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2006

Psychological Research 1/2006 Naar de uitgave