Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2006

01-01-2006 | Original Article

The time course of intended and unintended allocation of attention

Auteur: Gernot Horstmann

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

According to the contingent involuntary orienting hypothesis, only stimuli that match the attentional control settings based on intentions capture attention. In contrast, the surprise-capture hypothesis states that expectancy-discrepant stimuli can capture attention even if they do not match the control settings, implying unintended capture. The purpose of this study is to investigate whether unintended and intended attentional shifts are characterized by different time courses, indicating different underlying mechanisms. An unintended attentional shift was tested by the first, unannounced presentation of a color singleton at the location of a visual search target, and intended shifts by the following repeated presentations of a predictive singleton. Differences in time course were revealed by varying the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between singleton and target. Results showed that accuracy with expected singletons was barely affected by SOA, whereas SOA strongly affected accuracy with the unexpected singleton. The results are interpreted as supporting the surprise-capture hypothesis. It is furthermore argued that a division of labor between contingent capture and surprise in the control of attention supports adaptive behavior.
Voetnoten
1
Actually, Wilcocks (1928), following a hypothesis by Selz (1922), conducted the first experiments using unexpected singletons, but he did not use a visual search task in the strictest sense.
 
2
It should be emphasized that it is not supposed that the attentional shift is caused by the feeling of surprise. Instead, surprise is conceived of as a syndrome of responses to unexpected (schema-discrepant) events, with one of the components of this syndrome being the attentional shift.
 
3
To what extent this is possible remains to be examined. A good guess, however, appears to be that only basic features, but not their conjunctions, capture attention in the way examined in the present experiments (cf. Treisman & Gelade, 1980).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ansorge, U. & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 937–948.CrossRefPubMed Ansorge, U. & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 937–948.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496. Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.
go back to reference Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh, Unintended Thought (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford. Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh, Unintended Thought (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Chastain, G., & Cheal, M. L. (1998). Automatic versus directed attention with single-element and multiple-element precues. Visual Cognition, 5, 339–364. Chastain, G., & Cheal, M. L. (1998). Automatic versus directed attention with single-element and multiple-element precues. Visual Cognition, 5, 339–364.
go back to reference Cheal, M. L., & Chastain, G. (1998). Attention effects of moving and stationary single-element and multiple-element precues: Limits of automaticity. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 873–887. Cheal, M. L., & Chastain, G. (1998). Attention effects of moving and stationary single-element and multiple-element precues: Limits of automaticity. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 873–887.
go back to reference Desai, M. M. (1939). Surprise: A historical and experimental study. British Journal of Psychology: Monograph Supplements, 22. Desai, M. M. (1939). Surprise: A historical and experimental study. British Journal of Psychology: Monograph Supplements, 22.
go back to reference Di Lollo, V., Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (2000). Competition for consciousness among visual events: The psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 481–507. Di Lollo, V., Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (2000). Competition for consciousness among visual events: The psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 481–507.
go back to reference Enns, J. T., Austen, E. L., Di Lollo, V., Rauschenberger, R., & Yantis, S. (2001). New objects dominate luminance transients in setting attentional priority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27, 1287–1302.CrossRef Enns, J. T., Austen, E. L., Di Lollo, V., Rauschenberger, R., & Yantis, S. (2001). New objects dominate luminance transients in setting attentional priority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27, 1287–1302.CrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 847–858. Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 847–858.
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.CrossRefPubMed Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C. (1993). Contingent attentional capture: A reply to Yantis (1993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 682–685.CrossRef Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C. (1993). Contingent attentional capture: A reply to Yantis (1993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 682–685.CrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Wright, J. H. (1994). The structure of attentional control: Contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 317–329. Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Wright, J. H. (1994). The structure of attentional control: Contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 317–329.
go back to reference Gibson, B. S., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Surprise! An unexpected colour singleton does not capture attention in visual search. Psychological Science, 9, 176–182.CrossRef Gibson, B. S., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Surprise! An unexpected colour singleton does not capture attention in visual search. Psychological Science, 9, 176–182.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, B. S., & Kelsey, E. M. (1998). Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for display-wide visual features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 699–706.CrossRefPubMed Gibson, B. S., & Kelsey, E. M. (1998). Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for display-wide visual features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 699–706.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horstmann, G. (2002). Evidence for attentional capture by a surprising colour singleton in visual search. Psychological Science, 13, 499–505.CrossRefPubMed Horstmann, G. (2002). Evidence for attentional capture by a surprising colour singleton in visual search. Psychological Science, 13, 499–505.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horstmann, G. (2004). Attentional capture by a surprising colour singleton depends on its expectancy discrepancy. Manuscript submitted for publication. Horstmann, G. (2004). Attentional capture by a surprising colour singleton depends on its expectancy discrepancy. Manuscript submitted for publication.
go back to reference Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye movement. In J. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention & Performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye movement. In J. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention & Performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 346–354. Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 346–354.
go back to reference Kim, M. & Cave, K. R. (1999). Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1009–1023. Kim, M. & Cave, K. R. (1999). Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1009–1023.
go back to reference Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Meyer, W.-U. (1988). Die Rolle von Überraschung im Attributionsprozeß. Psychologische Rundschau, 39, 136–147. Meyer, W.-U. (1988). Die Rolle von Überraschung im Attributionsprozeß. Psychologische Rundschau, 39, 136–147.
go back to reference Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M. (1994). Surprise. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 353–358). Orlando, FL: Academic. Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M. (1994). Surprise. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 353–358). Orlando, FL: Academic.
go back to reference Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., & Schützwohl, A. (1991). An experimental analysis of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 295–311. Meyer, W.-U., Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., & Schützwohl, A. (1991). An experimental analysis of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 295–311.
go back to reference Meyer, W.-U., Reisenzein, R., & Schützwohl, A. (1997). Toward a process analysis of emotions: The case of surprise. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 251–274.CrossRef Meyer, W.-U., Reisenzein, R., & Schützwohl, A. (1997). Toward a process analysis of emotions: The case of surprise. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 251–274.CrossRef
go back to reference Most, S. B., & Simons, D. J. (2001). Attention capture, orienting, and awareness. In C. Folk & B. Gibson (Eds.), Attraction, distraction, and action: Multiple perspectives on attentional capture (pp. 151–173). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Most, S. B., & Simons, D. J. (2001). Attention capture, orienting, and awareness. In C. Folk & B. Gibson (Eds.), Attraction, distraction, and action: Multiple perspectives on attentional capture (pp. 151–173). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Most, S. B., Simons, D. J., Scholl, B. J., Jimenez, R., Clifford, E., & Chabris, C. F. (2001). How not to be seen: The contribution of similarity and selective ignoring to sustained inattentional blindness. Psychological Science, 12, 9–17.CrossRefPubMed Most, S. B., Simons, D. J., Scholl, B. J., Jimenez, R., Clifford, E., & Chabris, C. F. (2001). How not to be seen: The contribution of similarity and selective ignoring to sustained inattentional blindness. Psychological Science, 12, 9–17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive psychology. San Francisco: Freeman. Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive psychology. San Francisco: Freeman.
go back to reference Neisser, U. (1979). The control of information pickup in selective looking. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Perception and its development: A tribute to Eleanor J. Gibson (pp. 201–219). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Neisser, U. (1979). The control of information pickup in selective looking. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Perception and its development: A tribute to Eleanor J. Gibson (pp. 201–219). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Neumann, O. (1984). Automatic processing: A review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders, Cognition and Motor Processes (pp. 255–293). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Neumann, O. (1984). Automatic processing: A review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders, Cognition and Motor Processes (pp. 255–293). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., Schützwohl, A. & Meyer, W.-U. (1994). Temporal characteristics of the surprise reaction induced by schema-discrepant visual and auditory events. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 433–452. Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., Schützwohl, A. & Meyer, W.-U. (1994). Temporal characteristics of the surprise reaction induced by schema-discrepant visual and auditory events. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 433–452.
go back to reference Nothdurft, H.-C. (2000). Salience from feature contrast: Variations with texture density. Vision Research, 40, 3181–3200.CrossRefPubMed Nothdurft, H.-C. (2000). Salience from feature contrast: Variations with texture density. Vision Research, 40, 3181–3200.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 55–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 55–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1983). Wahrnehmung und Handlungssteuerung. Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Prinz, W. (1983). Wahrnehmung und Handlungssteuerung. Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1990). Unwillkürliche Aufmerksamkeit. In C. Meinecke & L. Kehrer (Eds.), Bielefelder Beiträge zur Kognitionspsychologie (pp. 49–57). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Prinz, W. (1990). Unwillkürliche Aufmerksamkeit. In C. Meinecke & L. Kehrer (Eds.), Bielefelder Beiträge zur Kognitionspsychologie (pp. 49–57). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Rauschenberger, R. (2003). When something old becomes something new: Spatiotemporal object continuity and attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 600–615.CrossRefPubMed Rauschenberger, R. (2003). When something old becomes something new: Spatiotemporal object continuity and attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 600–615.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rumelhart, D. E. (1984). Schemata and the cognitive system. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 161–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rumelhart, D. E. (1984). Schemata and the cognitive system. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 161–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Schützwohl, A. (1998). Surprise and schema strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1182–1199. Schützwohl, A. (1998). Surprise and schema strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1182–1199.
go back to reference Selz, O. (1922). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens und des Irrtums : eine experimentelle Untersuchung. Bonn, Germany: Cohen. Selz, O. (1922). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens und des Irrtums : eine experimentelle Untersuchung. Bonn, Germany: Cohen.
go back to reference Simons, D. J. (2000). Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 147–155.CrossRefPubMed Simons, D. J. (2000). Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 147–155.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Simons, D. J, & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.PubMed Simons, D. J, & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.PubMed
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606. Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1994). Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: Selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 799–806.CrossRefPubMed Theeuwes, J. (1994). Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: Selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 799–806.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Burger, R. (1998). Attentional control during visual search: The effect of irrelevant singletons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 1342–1353. Theeuwes, J., Burger, R. (1998). Attentional control during visual search: The effect of irrelevant singletons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 1342–1353.
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. & Godijn, R. (2001). Attentional and oculomotor capture. In Folk, C. L. & Gibson, B. S. (Eds.), Attraction, distraction and action: Multiple perspectives on attentional capture (pp. 121–149). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Theeuwes, J. & Godijn, R. (2001). Attentional and oculomotor capture. In Folk, C. L. & Gibson, B. S. (Eds.), Attraction, distraction and action: Multiple perspectives on attentional capture (pp. 121–149). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., & Irwin, D. E. (1998). Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science, 9, 379–385.CrossRef Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., & Irwin, D. E. (1998). Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science, 9, 379–385.CrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMed Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMed
go back to reference Wilcocks, R.-W. (1928). The effect of an unexpected heterogeneity on attention. Journal of General Psychology, 1, 286–319. Wilcocks, R.-W. (1928). The effect of an unexpected heterogeneity on attention. Journal of General Psychology, 1, 286–319.
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238. Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.
go back to reference Yantis, S. (1993). Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 676–681.CrossRefPubMed Yantis, S. (1993). Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 676–681.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Egeth, H. E. (1999). On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 661–676.CrossRefPubMed Yantis, S., & Egeth, H. E. (1999). On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 661–676.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 95–107.CrossRefPubMed Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 95–107.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.CrossRefPubMed Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
The time course of intended and unintended allocation of attention
Auteur
Gernot Horstmann
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0184-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2006

Psychological Research 1/2006 Naar de uitgave