Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2004

01-08-2004 | Original Article

Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms

Auteurs: Bruno H. Repp, Amandine Penel

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2004

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

People often move in synchrony with auditory rhythms (e.g., music), whereas synchronization of movement with purely visual rhythms is rare. In two experiments, this apparent attraction of movement to auditory rhythms was investigated by requiring participants to tap their index finger in synchrony with an isochronous auditory (tone) or visual (flashing light) target sequence while a distractor sequence was presented in the other modality at one of various phase relationships. The obtained asynchronies and their variability showed that auditory distractors strongly attracted participants' taps, whereas visual distractors had much weaker effects, if any. This asymmetry held regardless of the spatial congruence or relative salience of the stimuli in the two modalities. When different irregular timing patterns were imposed on target and distractor sequences, participants' taps tended to track the timing pattern of auditory distractor sequences when they were approximately in phase with visual target sequences, but not the reverse. These results confirm that rhythmic movement is more strongly attracted to auditory than to visual rhythms. To the extent that this is an innate proclivity, it may have been an important factor in the evolution of music.
Voetnoten
1
The IOI was described nominally as 500 ms in Repp and Penel (2002), with a footnote pointing out that it was in fact 2.4% shorter.
 
2
After Experiment 1 had been completed, two studies in which relative phase was varied came to our attention. Peryer, Sloboda, and Nte (2002) used isochronous visual target sequences in combination with faster or slower auditory distractor sequences. The auditory sequences significantly increased the variability of synchronization performance. Aschersleben and Bertelson (Gisa Aschersleben, personal communication, June 2002) have conducted an experiment apparently similar to ours. However, we have not yet seen a written report.
 
3
That is, we were prepared to interpret effects of relative phase as effects of absolute temporal distance. For reasons of convenience, however, we used normalized relative phase (ranging from −.5 to .5) rather than temporal separation in milliseconds as the independent variable. Whether relative phase as such plays a role in governing the magnitude of distractor effects is a question we did not address directly in the present study because this would have required varying the IOI duration, resulting in an excessively large design.
 
4
We are assuming that peripherally visible movements of the finger contribute little to error correction in synchronization. If they do play a role, this would work in favor of the visual modality.
 
5
The data of two additional participants were excluded, in one case (not analyzed) because of recording artifacts and in the other case because of atypical and highly variable results that were difficult to make sense of. For example, that participant tapped after rather than before visual stimuli (i.e., with positive asynchronies), which is quite unusual.
 
6
It is known from earlier waveform measurements that the output is 2.4% faster than specified in MAX (cf. Footnote 1). Thus, the actual IOI was 625 ms. (Note that relative phases are independent of this scaling factor.) All millisecond values reported in the Method and Results sections are nominal, as specified or recorded by the MAX program. Actual values were 2.4% shorter. Apart from this scaling factor, input and output timing are believed to be accurate within ±1 ms.
 
7
According to the first author's informal impressions, the deviations from isochrony were detectable but not particularly salient in auditory sequences. They were undetectable in visual sequences (cf. Repp & Penel, 2002).
 
8
We are not aware of any observations that deaf individuals synchronize more readily with visual stimuli than hearing individuals, although this would be worth investigating.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aschersleben, G. (2002). Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronization. Brain and Cognition, 48, 66–79.CrossRefPubMed Aschersleben, G. (2002). Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronization. Brain and Cognition, 48, 66–79.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bartlett, N. R., & Bartlett, S. C. (1959). Synchronization of a motor response with an anticipated sensory event. Psychological Review, 66, 203–218. Bartlett, N. R., & Bartlett, S. C. (1959). Synchronization of a motor response with an anticipated sensory event. Psychological Review, 66, 203–218.
go back to reference Bertelson, P., & Aschersleben, G. (1998). Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 482–489. Bertelson, P., & Aschersleben, G. (1998). Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 482–489.
go back to reference Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106, 3–22.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106, 3–22.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chen, Y., Repp, B. H., & Patel, A. D. (2002). Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: Comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, 21, 515–532.CrossRefPubMed Chen, Y., Repp, B. H., & Patel, A. D. (2002). Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: Comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, 21, 515–532.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183, 99–101.PubMed Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183, 99–101.PubMed
go back to reference Drake, C. (1997). Motor and perceptually preferred synchronisation by children and adults: Binary and ternary ratios. Polish Quarterly of Developmental Psychology, 3, 43–61. Drake, C. (1997). Motor and perceptually preferred synchronisation by children and adults: Binary and ternary ratios. Polish Quarterly of Developmental Psychology, 3, 43–61.
go back to reference Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition, 77, 251–288.CrossRefPubMed Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition, 77, 251–288.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Driver, J., & Spence, C. (2000). Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence. Current Biology, 10, R731–R735.CrossRefPubMed Driver, J., & Spence, C. (2000). Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence. Current Biology, 10, R731–R735.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fendrich, R., & Corballis, P. M. (2001). The temporal cross-capture of audition and vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 719–725. Fendrich, R., & Corballis, P. M. (2001). The temporal cross-capture of audition and vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 719–725.
go back to reference Fraisse, P. (1948). Rythmes auditifs et rythmes visuels. [Visual and auditory rhythms.] L'Année Psychologique, 49, 21–41. Fraisse, P. (1948). Rythmes auditifs et rythmes visuels. [Visual and auditory rhythms.] L'Année Psychologique, 49, 21–41.
go back to reference Fraisse, P., Pichon, P., & Clairouin, G. (1949). Les aptitudes rythmiques: Etude comparée des oligophrènes et des enfants normaux. [Rhythmical aptitudes: A comparative study of mentally retarded and normal children.] Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique, 42, 309–330. Fraisse, P., Pichon, P., & Clairouin, G. (1949). Les aptitudes rythmiques: Etude comparée des oligophrènes et des enfants normaux. [Rhythmical aptitudes: A comparative study of mentally retarded and normal children.] Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique, 42, 309–330.
go back to reference Gault, R. H., & Goodfellow, L. D. (1938). An empirical comparison of audition, vision, and touch in the discrimination of temporal patterns and ability to reproduce them. Journal of General Psychology, 18, 41–47. Gault, R. H., & Goodfellow, L. D. (1938). An empirical comparison of audition, vision, and touch in the discrimination of temporal patterns and ability to reproduce them. Journal of General Psychology, 18, 41–47.
go back to reference Glenberg, A., & Jona, M. (1991). Temporal coding in rhythm tasks revealed by modality effects. Memory & Cognition, 19, 514–522. Glenberg, A., & Jona, M. (1991). Temporal coding in rhythm tasks revealed by modality effects. Memory & Cognition, 19, 514–522.
go back to reference Glenberg, A., Mann, S., Altman, L., Forman, T., & Procise, S. (1989). Modality effects in the coding and reproduction of rhythms. Memory & Cognition, 17, 373–383. Glenberg, A., Mann, S., Altman, L., Forman, T., & Procise, S. (1989). Modality effects in the coding and reproduction of rhythms. Memory & Cognition, 17, 373–383.
go back to reference Goldstone, S., Boardman, W. K., & Lhamon, W. T. (1959). Intersensory comparisons of temporal judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 243–248. Goldstone, S., Boardman, W. K., & Lhamon, W. T. (1959). Intersensory comparisons of temporal judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 243–248.
go back to reference Goodfellow, L. D. (1934). An empirical comparison of audition, vision, and touch in the discrimination of short intervals of time. American Journal of Psychology, 46, 243–258. Goodfellow, L. D. (1934). An empirical comparison of audition, vision, and touch in the discrimination of short intervals of time. American Journal of Psychology, 46, 243–258.
go back to reference Grondin, S. (1993). Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 383–394. Grondin, S. (1993). Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 383–394.
go back to reference Grondin, S., & Rousseau, R. (1991). Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 245–256. Grondin, S., & Rousseau, R. (1991). Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 245–256.
go back to reference Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., Ouellette, C., & Macar, F. (1998). Sensory effects on judgments of short time-intervals. Psychological Research, 61, 261–268.CrossRefPubMed Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., Ouellette, C., & Macar, F. (1998). Sensory effects on judgments of short time-intervals. Psychological Research, 61, 261–268.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grondin, S., Ouellet, B., & Roussel, M.-E. (2001). About optimal timing and stability of Weber fraction for duration discrimination. Acoustical Science and Technology, 22, 370–372.CrossRef Grondin, S., Ouellet, B., & Roussel, M.-E. (2001). About optimal timing and stability of Weber fraction for duration discrimination. Acoustical Science and Technology, 22, 370–372.CrossRef
go back to reference Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347–356.PubMed Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347–356.PubMed
go back to reference Hargreaves, D. J. (1986). The developmental psychology of music. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Hargreaves, D. J. (1986). The developmental psychology of music. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Harrington, L. K., & Peck, C. K. (1998). Spatial disparity affects visual-auditory interactions in human sensorimotor processing. Experimental Brain Research, 122, 247–252.CrossRef Harrington, L. K., & Peck, C. K. (1998). Spatial disparity affects visual-auditory interactions in human sensorimotor processing. Experimental Brain Research, 122, 247–252.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Kelso, J. A. S., DelColle, J. D., & Schöner, G. (1990). Action-perception as a pattern formation process. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 138–169). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kelso, J. A. S., DelColle, J. D., & Schöner, G. (1990). Action-perception as a pattern formation process. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 138–169). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Kolers, P. A., & Brewster, J. M. (1985). Rhythms and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 150–167.CrossRefPubMed Kolers, P. A., & Brewster, J. M. (1985). Rhythms and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 150–167.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lewald, J., & Guski, R. (2003). Cross-modal perceptual integration of spatially and temporally disparate auditory and visual stimuli. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 468–478.CrossRefPubMed Lewald, J., & Guski, R. (2003). Cross-modal perceptual integration of spatially and temporally disparate auditory and visual stimuli. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 468–478.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lewald, J., Ehrenstein, W. H., & Guski, R. (2001). Spatio-temporal constraints for auditory-visual integration. Behavioral Brain Research, 121, 69–79.CrossRef Lewald, J., Ehrenstein, W. H., & Guski, R. (2001). Spatio-temporal constraints for auditory-visual integration. Behavioral Brain Research, 121, 69–79.CrossRef
go back to reference Michon, J. A. (1967). Timing in temporal tracking. Assen, NL: Van Gorcum Michon, J. A. (1967). Timing in temporal tracking. Assen, NL: Van Gorcum
go back to reference Morein-Zamir, S., Soto-Faraco, S., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Auditory capture of vision: Examining temporal ventriloquism. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 154–163.CrossRefPubMed Morein-Zamir, S., Soto-Faraco, S., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Auditory capture of vision: Examining temporal ventriloquism. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 154–163.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Peryer, G., Sloboda, & Nte, S. (2002). How is the synchronisation of tapping to a visual isochronous pulse affected by an interfering auditory pulse? In C. Stevens, D. Burnham, G. McPherson, E. Schubert, & J. Renwick (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Sydney, 2002 (pp. 783–786). Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions (CD-ROM). Peryer, G., Sloboda, & Nte, S. (2002). How is the synchronisation of tapping to a visual isochronous pulse affected by an interfering auditory pulse? In C. Stevens, D. Burnham, G. McPherson, E. Schubert, & J. Renwick (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Sydney, 2002 (pp. 783–786). Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions (CD-ROM).
go back to reference Pressing, J. (1998). Error correction processes in temporal pattern production. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 42, 63–101.CrossRefPubMed Pressing, J. (1998). Error correction processes in temporal pattern production. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 42, 63–101.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rainbow, E. L., & Owen, D. (1979). A progress report on a three year investigation of the rhythmic ability of pre-school aged children. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 59, 84–86. Rainbow, E. L., & Owen, D. (1979). A progress report on a three year investigation of the rhythmic ability of pre-school aged children. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 59, 84–86.
go back to reference Recanzone, G. H. (2003). Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 1078–1093.PubMed Recanzone, G. H. (2003). Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 1078–1093.PubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1997). Acoustics, perception, and production of legato articulation on a computer-controlled grand piano. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1878–1890CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H. (1997). Acoustics, perception, and production of legato articulation on a computer-controlled grand piano. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1878–1890CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research, 63, 106–128.PubMed Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research, 63, 106–128.PubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 600–621.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 600–621.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2002a). Automaticity and voluntary control of phase correction following event onset shifts in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 410–430. Repp, B. H. (2002a). Automaticity and voluntary control of phase correction following event onset shifts in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 410–430.
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2002b). The embodiment of musical structure: Effects of musical context on sensorimotor synchronization with complex timing patterns. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 245–265). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press. Repp, B. H. (2002b). The embodiment of musical structure: Effects of musical context on sensorimotor synchronization with complex timing patterns. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 245–265). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2002c). Perception of timing is more context sensitive than sensorimotor synchronization. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 703–716. Repp, B. H. (2002c). Perception of timing is more context sensitive than sensorimotor synchronization. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 703–716.
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2003). Phase attraction in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory sequences: Effects of single and periodic distractors on synchronization accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 290–309.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H. (2003). Phase attraction in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory sequences: Effects of single and periodic distractors on synchronization accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 290–309.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (in press). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. Journal of Motor Behavior. Repp, B. H. (in press). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. Journal of Motor Behavior.
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2002). Auditory dominance in temporal processing: New evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1085–1099.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2002). Auditory dominance in temporal processing: New evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1085–1099.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rousseau, R., Poirier, J., & Lemyre, L. (1983). Duration discrimination of empty time intervals marked by intermodal pulses. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 541–548. Rousseau, R., Poirier, J., & Lemyre, L. (1983). Duration discrimination of empty time intervals marked by intermodal pulses. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 541–548.
go back to reference Semjen, A., & Ivry, R. B. (2001). The coupled oscillator model of between-hand coordination in alternate-hand tapping: A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 251–265.CrossRefPubMed Semjen, A., & Ivry, R. B. (2001). The coupled oscillator model of between-hand coordination in alternate-hand tapping: A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 251–265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Slutsky, D. A., & Recanzone, G. H. (2001). Temporal and spatial dependency of the ventriloquism effect. NeuroReport, 12, 7–12.PubMed Slutsky, D. A., & Recanzone, G. H. (2001). Temporal and spatial dependency of the ventriloquism effect. NeuroReport, 12, 7–12.PubMed
go back to reference Spence, C., Shore, D. I., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Multisensory prior entry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 799–832.CrossRef Spence, C., Shore, D. I., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Multisensory prior entry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 799–832.CrossRef
go back to reference Spence, C., Baddeley, R., Zampini, M., James, R., & Shore, D. I. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments: When two locations are better than one. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 318–328. Spence, C., Baddeley, R., Zampini, M., James, R., & Shore, D. I. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments: When two locations are better than one. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 318–328.
go back to reference Stein, B. E. (1998). Neural mechanisms for synthesizing sensory information and producing adaptive behaviors. Experimental Brain Research, 123, 124–135.CrossRef Stein, B. E. (1998). Neural mechanisms for synthesizing sensory information and producing adaptive behaviors. Experimental Brain Research, 123, 124–135.CrossRef
go back to reference Trevarthen, C. (1999–2000). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae (special issue), 155–215. Trevarthen, C. (1999–2000). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae (special issue), 155–215.
go back to reference Tuller, B., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1989). Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 75, 306–316. Tuller, B., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1989). Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 75, 306–316.
go back to reference Walker, J. T., & Scott, K. J. (1981). Auditory-visual conflicts in the perceived duration of lights, tones, and gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1327–1339.CrossRefPubMed Walker, J. T., & Scott, K. J. (1981). Auditory-visual conflicts in the perceived duration of lights, tones, and gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1327–1339.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yamanishi, Y., Kawato, M., & Suzuki, R. (1980). Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands. Biological Cybernetics, 37, 219–225.PubMed Yamanishi, Y., Kawato, M., & Suzuki, R. (1980). Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands. Biological Cybernetics, 37, 219–225.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms
Auteurs
Bruno H. Repp
Amandine Penel
Publicatiedatum
01-08-2004
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2004
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0143-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2004

Psychological Research 4/2004 Naar de uitgave