Traditional diagnostic categories from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) and the International Classification of Disease (ICD) suffer from etiologic heterogeneity, frequent comorbidity, and arbitrary cutoffs that delimit clinical utility. These limitations prevent the translation of basic science knowledge to reduce the considerable burden of mental illness (Cuthbert & Insel,
2013). Advancing an etiology-based taxonomy of psychopathology, the NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative launched an experimental framework for researchers to use to identify reliable and valid psychobiological mechanisms that underlie psychiatric symptoms (Cuthbert,
2014). Following this conceptualization of psychopathology as alterations in biological and psychological processes across key domains of functioning, the major RDoC framework is a two-dimensional matrix of superordinate domains whose constructs can be assayed with separable units of analysis (e.g., genes, molecules, brain circuits, physiology, behavior, self-report) and assessment paradigms. By elucidating neurodevelopmental
processes, RDoC aims to achieve the highest level of construct validity (Morris & Cuthbert,
2012). In turn, identification of etiological processes may spur long-awaited innovations in assessment, prevention, and intervention efforts (Cuthbert & Insel,
2013).
RDoC has already led to changes in the conceptualization of adult psychopathology, including examination of brain-behavior dimensions of transdiagnostic constructs and treatment outcomes (Sanislow,
2020). Yet, RDoC has yet to fulfill its promises for child and adolescent mental health, a noteworthy shortfall in the context of the nation’s worsening crisis in child and adolescent psychopathology (U.S. Surgeon General’s Advisory,
2021; CDC,
2011–2021). Although the RDoC initiative has a mandate to integrate “the fundamental genetic, neurobiological, behavioral,
environmental, and experiential components that comprise mental disorders” (NIMH,
2008, emphasis added), it remains largely agnostic with respect to how the environment should be defined or measured, including intersections with components within the matrix (Conradt et al.,
2021; Mittal & Wakschlag,
2017). Early experiences with caregivers are an especially important source of environmental input that directly affect child neurodevelopment (King et al.,
2021), as evident across multiple domains (e.g., social processes, cognitive systems, regulatory systems; Feldman,
2012; Sroufe,
2005; Zeanah et al.,
2017). Consistent with recent calls to improve integration of environmental influences with constructs within the RDoC framework (Beauchaine & Hinshaw,
2020; Conradt et al.,
2021; Mittal & Wakschlag,
2017), we contend that critical intersections between the external caregiving environment and the developing child constitute the most potent risk factors for psychopathology. King et al. (
2021) offer an important first step in advancing this research agenda in their conceptualization of the caregiving environment
within the social processes domain of the existing RDoC matrix. We laud their explication of how the caregiving environment, though assessed extrinsically to the focal child, is critical to child neurodevelopment. At the same time, highly influential transactional and bioecological models contend that
both dyad members actively shape the caregiver-child relationship (Bronfenbrenner & Morris,
2006; Feldman,
2007,
2012; Sameroff,
2009) and discourage reducing dynamic relationship influences to any one individual (Bornstein,
2013).
In this paper, we first define interpersonal dynamics and review theoretical and empirical support for examining caregiver-child dynamics in moment-to-moment indices across separable brain circuits, physiology, behavior, and self-report units. Then, we offer a methodological proof-of-concept of our novel interpersonal approach for assessing intersections between the caregiving environment and biobehavioral components within the RDoC matrix vis-à-vis caregiver-child interpersonal dynamics. The review concludes with an agenda for future RDoC-advancing research and clinical implications of caregiver-child interpersonal dynamics for assessing, preventing, and mitigating child psychopathology risk.
Defining Caregiver-Child Dynamics
Harmonizing the jingle-jangle of conceptual terms in the literature (e.g., “synchrony,” “attunement,” “concordance”), we operationalize caregiver-child “interpersonal dynamics” as the temporal coordination in the ebb and flow of parents’ and their children’s biology, behavior, and experience during social interactions (Butler,
2011). Caregiver-child dynamics are tied to well-validated conceptual frameworks, including dynamic systems perspectives on emotions and developmental psychopathology (e.g., Butler,
2011; Granic,
2005), parent–child emotion regulation dynamics (Morris et al.,
2018; Ratliff et al.,
2022), and attachment-based biobehavioral regulatory models (Feldman,
2007,
2012). These frameworks share several central tenets. First, interpersonal dynamics consist of whole-body processes that involve the interpersonal maintenance of physiological and behavioral homeostasis (Butler,
2011). Second, caregiver-child dynamics typically follow a normative developmental progression. Biological preparedness for interpersonal dynamics is evident in utero (Feldman,
2007) and, after infancy, the emergence of language and symbolic thought may allow for more complex forms of synchrony than those focused on simple behavioral changes, including synchrony in attention, thoughts, intentions, goals, and experiences (Bell,
2020; Feldman,
2007; Harrist & Waugh,
2002). Finally, through repeated experiences, caregiver-child dynamics shape the evolving dyadic relationship and the future individual development of both members of the dyad (King et al.,
2021; Leclère et al.,
2014; Perlman et al.,
2022).
Evidence of Caregiver-Child Dynamics in RDoC Units
In this section, we synthesize evidence across theoretical, narrative, systematic, and meta-analytic reviews demonstrating that within-dyad caregiver-child dynamics are evident in well-established neural, physiological, behavioral, and experiential units of analysis. Under the broader umbrella of “interpersonal dynamics,” “positive synchrony” refers to reciprocated
in-phase changes that are matched in the direction of change in their partner. In contrast, “negative synchrony” refers to
anti-phase changes in one individual that are inversely associated with changes in their partner. We primarily focus on synchrony, although there are numerous operationalizations of interpersonal dynamics, including dyad-level assessment (e.g., dyadic flexibility; Grumi et al.,
2022; Hollenstein et al.,
2013; the dyad’s tendency to move in and out of synchrony; Mayo & Gordon,
2020).
Table
1 highlights the domains (superordinate constructs) for which there is empirical evidence of caregiver-child dynamics among typical developmental, at-risk, and clinical populations, organized by the units of analysis in the RDoC matrix and key paradigms. Caregiver-child dynamics have been reliably established using multiple assessment paradigms, including unstructured play tasks and standardized stress and stress and recovery tasks (Atkinson et al.,
2016; Golds et al.,
2022; Leclère et al.,
2014; Miller et al.,
2023; Provenzi et al.,
2018). Notably, units of analysis and assessment paradigms are orthogonal to the domains, such that each construct in the matrix can be assessed in various measurement classes and assessment paradigms (Cuthbert,
2014).
Table 1
Reviews of units of analysis and paradigms for assessing caregiver-child interpersonal dynamics in RDoC constructs
Negative valence systems | prefrontal cortexa,b; dorsolateral prefrontal cortexb,c | ANSa,b,d,e; PNSf; HRb,d,e; HPA, dysregulated HPA axis, average cortisol levelsb,d,e,g,h; skin conductanceb,e | crying, sadness, worrya,d,i; facial expressionsa,d,i,j,k,s; increased conflict detection, physical and relational aggressiond,j,l,m; approach, avoidance, withdrawali,n,o | self-reported distressp | labtab frustrative nonrewardb,h; fear filmf |
Positive valence systems | prefrontal cortexa,b | HR changea,b | | PANASp | |
Cognitive | prefrontal cortexa,b; oscillations (scalp eeg)a; dorsolateral prefrontal and frontopolar cortexb,c | HR decelerationa,b,e | coherent discourse, coherent sentencesj; off-task behaviorsj,l,m | | |
Social processes | prefrontal cortexa,b; superior temporal sulcusb,q; V1-FFA-STS-amygdala & V1-FFA-STS-VS b,q | Sympathetic activityb,d,e,g; HPA axis activation and down-regulationb,d,e,g, vagal tone, vagal withdrawala,b,f; HR/bp/respiration, HR variabilitya,b,d,e; skin temperatureb,e; skin conductance, skin conductance response b,e | reciprocal eye contact, gaze following, eye gaze detection, eye gaze aversion/contact, joint attentiona,i,j,n,o; behavior observation/coding systems, facial affect production, developmentally appropriate perception of one’s emotional states, mimicry; imitation of facial gestures; reciprocal emotional expressiona,d,i,j,k,n,o,s; vocalizationsa,i,k; interactive playo,i,n; distress upon separationi; gestural/postural expressionso | | multimodal social paradigmsb,e,f,g,h,i,k,r |
Arousal & regulatory systems | EEG theta rhythmsq | Eega,b,h; HRa,b,d,e; galvanic skin responseb,e | Affective states, emotional reactivitya,d,i,j,k,n,o,s | | cardiac PEPb; HRVa,b,d,f; electrodermal respondingd,e |
Sensorimotor systems | somatosensory cortexa; dorsolateral prefrontal cortexc | Oscillatory rhythma | Activity leveln,o | | |
Proof-of-Concept Study: Examining Parent–Child Coordination of Parasympathetic Nervous System Functioning During Conflict and Positive Event-Planning
Conceptualizing caregiver–child interactions as the product of interpersonal dynamics using RDoC units affords the opportunity to examine real-time intersections between children’s most salient environmental context – the caregiver-child relationship – and intrapersonal biobehavioral processes that unfold from one moment to the next. Our proof-of-concept study aims to motivate future research that prioritizes: (a) moment-to-moment assays to uncover caregiver-child dynamics, including the direction in which state-like fluctuations in caregiver’s and youth’s biobehavioral processes impact or are impacted by each other and (b) pinpoint within-dyad differences in dynamics across RDoC domains that clarify between-child differences in risk for psychopathology. We assessed parent–child synchrony involving vagal functioning (indexed by respiratory sinus arrhythmia [RSA], a measure of parasympathetically-mediated vagal influences on cardiometabolic output) in different interpersonal assessment paradigms, among a sample of emotionally at-risk preadolescent girls and their parents. Specifically, we evaluated within-dyad, parent- and child-led RSA synchrony in two ecologically valid interaction contexts (i.e., conflict discussion and positive event-planning) that each recruit processes arousal, social, and cognitive RDoC domains, but differentially elicit negative and positive valence systems, respectively. To exemplify the proposed approach, our primary aims were to evaluate variation in the presence of within-dyad parent- and child-led RSA synchrony in (a) a conflict discussion task, (b) a pleasant event discussion, and (c) to explore whether families evaluated different patterns of synchrony in these tasks.
Method
Participants
The sample for our methodological demonstration included caregiver-daughter dyads participating in an ongoing investigation of the cognitive, familial, and psychophysiological correlates of emotional development in 6- to 11-year-old girls, the Developmental Research on Emotion and Mental Health in Girls (DREAMING) Study. Consistent with principles of RDoC, targeted recruitment efforts ensured sufficient range in girls’ negative emotionality without regard to a specific diagnostic taxon. Middle childhood is a critical inflection point for changing interpersonal, emotional, and physiological dynamics. We prioritized examination of parent-daughter dynamics in particular given girls’ unique vulnerability to developing internalizing problems, especially among dyads with girls who are temperamentally at-risk for emotional problems. This prescribed developmental period also temporally precedes adolescence, which is witness to acute increases in internalizing problems. Eligibility criteria included: 1) child who identifies as female between 6 and 11 years old during the time of screening, 2) fluency in English, 3) child must not have a history of seizures or seizure disorder and 4) child must not have been previously diagnosed with intellectual disability or an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The University of California, Los Angeles IRB approved all study procedures prior to recruitment or data collection.
Data collection for the DREAMING Study was halted by the COVID-19 pandemic and recently resumed; data collection for the proof-of-concept subsample took place between March 2019 and February 2020. The sample who participated in a conflict discussion task or positive event-planning task included 28 girls between 6 and 11 years of age (Mage = 8.54 years, SD = 1.82 years) and their caregivers (92.9% female). Children and families had a racial/ethnic distribution that is representative of the local geographic area. The children were ethnically diverse; most children were identified by their caregivers as multiracial (55.7%). All caregivers were the child’s biological parent (85.7% female; 14.3% male); most caregivers were married (82.1%) and had attained a bachelor’s degree or above (89.3%). The modal average annual income of the sample was $125,000–$150,000, with a wide range from less than $25,000 to over $300,000 per year, for an average household size of 3.64 (SD = 1.06).
Recruitment
Participants were recruited for the parent study from community settings, including pediatric offices, mental health service providers, tutoring centers, community/recreation centers, and local schools. Interested families contacted the research team and were carefully screened using a standardized script. If they satisfied eligibility criteria and they remained interested in the study, families were then scheduled for a laboratory visit.
Procedures
At the laboratory visit, parents provided informed consent and children assented to study procedures; parents and daughters then completed rating scales, clinical interviews, behavioral paradigms, and laboratory-based parent–child interaction tasks. During the parent–child interaction tasks, parent-daughter dyads completed two 5-min interaction tasks in a fixed order: a Conflict Discussion followed by a Positive Event-Planning Task. Prior to the discussion tasks, parents were asked to rate common sources of conflict, using the Issues Checklist (Prinz et al.,
1979), and commonly enjoyed activities, using the Pleasant Events Checklist (MacPhillamy & Lewinsohn,
1982). For the Conflict Discussion, an experimenter selected a source of conflict that the parent rated highly and indicated was unresolved, and then asked them to discuss it for 5 min. Similarly, in the Positive Event-Planning Task, an experimenter selected a pleasant activity that the parent rated highly, and then asked them to spend 5 min planning the pleasant event. The Conflict Discussion and Positive Event-Planning Tasks have been shown to differentially elicit negative and positive negative behaviors, respectively (Richmond et al.,
2020). Participants were compensated $100 for the 4-h laboratory visit.
Measures
We estimated time-varying RSA for each five-minute discussion task using the MATLAB toolbox RSASeconds (Gates et al.,
2015). Each of the cleaned IBI series was interpolated at 4 Hz using a cubic spline to create equal data intervals. The data were then tapered using Peak Matched Multiple Windows (PM MW) and a short-time Fourier transform (STFT) was applied to moving 32-s IBI windows in order to obtain an estimate of the power spectrum for the 16th second of the window. Power estimates were obtained within the adult respiration frequency band (0.12–0.40 Hz; Berntson, Quigley, & Lozano,
2007) for caregiver participants and within age-appropriate frequency bands for child participants (Shader et al.,
2018). In short, the combination of PM MW and STFT produces point estimates of time-varying RSA for the central second in every rolling 32-s window, while drawing on information from the 16 s before and after the central second (Gates et al.,
2015); notably, this focal tapering method has been shown to capture changes in RSA without requiring first-differencing (Gatzke-Kopp et al.,
2022). The PM MW/STFT method has been validated among adult and caregiver-child dyads (Gatzke-Kopp et al.,
2022; Somers et al.,
2021; Zhang et al.,
2022).
Data Analytic Plan: Assessment of RSA Synchrony
Idiographic, single-level dynamic structural equation models (DSEMs; Asparouhov et al.,
2018) were tested using M
plus Version 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén;
1998‐
2017) to evaluate within-dyad time-lagged RSA synchrony (defined in terms of how one partner’s RSA responsivity influences their own and their partner’s RSA responsivity during the subsequent second, after accounting for intrapersonal stability in RSA responsivity) for each dyad, per task. M
plus uses Bayesian Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) with a Gibbs Sampler to estimate DSEMs. We used two unthinned chains, each running for a maximum of 100,000 iterations to ensure the estimation was stable. We allowed the algorithm to terminate prematurely if the Potential Scale Reduction factor dropped below 1.05 (Gelman & Rubin,
1992). We used the default prior distributions in M
plus. Posterior distributions were summarized with the median.
Lagged variables of daughters’ and parents’ RSA were created in M
plus and were latent mean centered to disaggregate within-person fluctuations from trait-like RSA levels (Hamaker & Grasman,
2015). In each single-level DSEM, the within-dyad intercepts, intercept variances and covariance, and all within-person stability (autoregressive paths) and within-dyad RSA synchrony (cross-lagged regression paths in daughters’ and parents’ RSA) were estimated. Within-dyad parent-driven RSA synchrony was operationalized as the effect of fluctuations in a parents’ RSA at one time point on fluctuations in their daughters’ RSA the subsequent second, adjusting for prior child RSA; conversely, within-dyad child-driven RSA synchrony was operationalized as the effect of fluctuations in a child’s RSA at one time point on subsequent fluctuations in their parents’ RSA, adjusting for prior parent RSA. Similar to a frequentist framework, if the 95% credible interval did not contain zero, the within-dyad interpersonal effect was determined to be non-null.
Results
Preliminary Analysis
Of families with task ECG data, missingness during the task ranged from 0% to 30.4% (M = 3.3%). When a period of 3 or more beats in the ECG data needed correcting, data were considered missing and not used in analysis. The interpolation generated imputed IBI values for portions of missing data; however, we excluded the interpolated values for missing portions ≥ 10 s to maintain the precision of imputation. In turn, when there was a segment of missing data in the interpolated IBI series, the RSA values would be missing from 16 s before the segment until 16 s after the segment due to the tapering approach used.
Primary Analyses
In each single-level DSEM, the intercepts, variances and covariance, and all autoregressive or AR(1) paths and cross-lagged paths in daughters’ and caregivers’ RSA were estimated. Supplementary Table
1 presents descriptive statistics and zero-order between-dyad correlations for the primary study variables.
Discussion
Historically, RDoC has viewed the units of analysis in its matrix as trait-like dispositions (Patrick & Hajcak,
2016). However, the extant interpersonal dynamics literature demonstrates youth biobehavioral components exhibit state-like fluctuations, which are inextricably linked to unfolding changes in their caregivers’ biobehavioral functioning. Using rigorous methods to assess within-dyad synchrony, most (but not all) parent-daughter dyads exhibited interpersonal RSA synchrony (either parent- or child-driven) during a paradigm designed to activate negative valence systems (the conflict discussion task). Notably, parent-daughter synchrony was generally bidirectional, with opposing patterns of parent- versus daughter-driven RSA synchrony that may jointly contribute to maintaining dyadic equilibrium. Positive, in-phase child-driven synchrony may reflect parents’ cognitive and behavioral attunement with their children (Armstrong-Carter et al.,
2021; Gao et al.,
2023; Helm et al.,
2018; McKillop & Connell,
2018), or alternatively, stress contagion in the context of heightened negative emotions or risk (Birk et al.,
2022; Davis et al.,
2018). In contrast, anti-phase parent-driven synchrony may arise from parents’ need to recruit their own physiological resources to “share the load” and regulate their child’s emotions and sustained engagement in the task (Davis et al.,
2018). These preliminary results represent a descriptive characterization of between-dyad variation in within-dyad RSA synchrony that requires replication, extension, and exploration of intrapersonal and contextual correlates of these dynamics.
Results also support the need to assess caregiver-child dynamics using assessment paradigms sensitive to domain-specific alterations. Compared to the conflict discussion task, there was weaker evidence of parent-daughter synchrony during a paradigm designed to activate positive valence systems (the positive event-planning task). Further, there was null agreement or even divergence in within-dyad synchrony across paradigms; caregiver-child dynamics may vary within dyads and across interpersonal environmental contexts, influenced by the intersecting domains that are elicited. Taken together, these preliminary results suggest that parent- and child-led RSA synchrony varies between dyads and also within dyads across meaningfully separate paradigms. In the remainder of the discussion, we propose an agenda for advancing RDoC that builds on this preliminary work and discusses the clinical implications for dyadic assessment and intervention techniques.
Our proposed approach for advancing RDoC
Link Alterations in Intrapersonal Functioning with Within-Dyad Processes
Moment-to-moment interpersonal dynamics are likely to constrain children’s neurodevelopment and emerging mental health problems and social competencies. At the same time, early-emerging mental health concerns may impact interpersonal dynamics in ways that exacerbate or perpetuate symptoms through transactions with the environment (Lougheed,
2020). Identifying intrapersonal correlates of maladaptive interpersonal dynamics could suggest novel, tractable endophenotypes that serve as early risk indicators and/or targets for interventions that seek to improve person-environment transactions and in turn prevent clinically significant impairment (Cuthbert,
2014). An intermediary step in this research agenda is to develop an archaeology of interpersonal dynamics, including assessment of how intrapersonal functioning in RDoC units may influence interpersonal dynamics across RDoC units. Although we focus on caregiver-child dynamics in units that can be assayed using moment-to-moment techniques, genes and molecules (e.g., oxytocin) are also relevant to interpersonal dynamics (e.g., Feldman et al.,
2007,
2010; Markova & Nguyen,
2022). Heritable individual characteristics non-randomly elicit responses from the environment (i.e., evocative gene-environment correlation; Scarr & McCartney,
1983), such that child-led dynamics, or children’s influence on their caregivers, may in part reflect heritable child risk factors (e.g., Liu et al.,
2020). Concurrent cross-unit associations also point to intrapersonal building blocks of caregiver-child dynamics during real-time interactions (e.g., evidence that greater conversational turn-taking is associated with more positive brain-based interpersonal dynamics; Ratliff et al.,
2022).
Study design features could also be leveraged to improve traction on understanding between-person differences in within-dyad caregiver-child interpersonal dynamics. The active roles that children play in shaping their environments exist on two interdependent levels of analysis: (1) between-families, and (2) within families, during moment-to-moment interactions. These often-overlooked or uncovered child-driven influences may be more apparent when researchers: (a) recruit samples enriched for
child transdiagnostic risk factors (e.g., negative emotionality); (b) minimize effects of potential confounders (e.g., child gender) through within-group designs; and (c) evaluate interpersonal dynamics during developmental transition periods (e.g., transition to adolescence) where these dynamics are hypothesized to undergo recalibration in light of developing biological, psychological, and social processes. Whereas prior work that failed to detect evidence of child-driven RSA synchrony across conflict discussion recruited families with adolescents, with a range of exposure to maternal depressive symptoms (e.g., McKillop & Connell,
2018), our sample of emotionally at-risk preadolescent girls and their caregivers may have optimized detection of child-driven RSA synchrony during conflict discussion. The broader environmental context (e.g., household socioeconomic resources, culture, neighborhood, etc.) may influence interpersonal dynamics directly (e.g., through influencing the likelihood of experiencing or expressing certain types of affect or behavior) or indirectly (e.g., through influencing appraisal of interaction goals); contextual factors may also alter the effects of interpersonal dynamics on health (Paley & Hajal,
2022). Future research using two-level DSEMs, with larger samples (e.g., 200 dyads with 100 or more time points; Schultzberg & Muthén,
2018), are needed to examine within-dyad synchrony across units of analysis and between-dyad differences in synchrony due to child and parent emotional risk, gender, age, and contextual risk due to socioeconomic disadvantage or ethnic minority status. Longitudinal designs (e.g., pre-post DSEM; measurement burst designs) are also needed to evaluate stability and change in interpersonal dynamics and their correlates in the context of specific developmental challenges and transdiagnostic risk factors (Davis et al.,
2018; DePasquale,
2020).
Clinical implications
A translational approach requires accumulating knowledge of disrupted interpersonal dynamics implicated in youth psychological distress and integrating these findings into clinical practice. Clinical assessment techniques that leverage the interpersonal context of child psychopathology should be informed by evidence (including that provided by our proof-of-concept study) that these dynamics are state-like properties of the dyad
and the interaction context in which they are elicited. In addition to informing development of new diagnostic indexes, interpersonal dynamics that reinforce or exacerbate child psychopathology through transactions with the caregiving environment may also be prognostic indicators of worsening symptom course or (individually-focused) treatment response (De Rubeis & Granic,
2012). Dyad-oriented assessment and treatment strategies may mitigate stigma associated with treatment-seeking for parenting support, while also underscoring each individual’s agency within the social environment. Assessing interpersonal dynamics in multiple interaction contexts that differentially elicit key domains of functioning may also help families identify strengths as well as areas for growth.
The study of interpersonal dynamics also points to family-centered intervention strategies for multiple youth risk or clinical conditions. Interventions that leverage dynamic caregiver-child interactions have traditionally focused on families with infants and younger children (e.g., infant-parent psychotherapy); however, the extant literature, including the present findings, underscores how the interpersonal context remains salient across the lifespan. Empirically-supported interventions that facilitate adaptive bidirectional dynamics, including both parent-led dynamics (e.g., teaching parents constructive emotion socialization skills, such as modeling coping strategies) and child-led dynamics (e.g., teaching parents to be aware of their own responses in the face of child distress), are needed for families with youth of all ages. Practitioners could use dyadic neurophysiological feedback, along with well-validated video feedback techniques (Balldin et al.,
2018; Fukkink,
2008), to tailor feedback for parents and children about their interpersonal dynamics. Dyadic feedback could be used as an initial assessment strategy and intervention procedure; with repeated feedback over time, parents and children may respond to each other more contingently and ultimately adaptively (Ratliff et al.,
2022). Outside of therapy, other reflective practices (e.g., mindful awareness, Feelings Thermometer) could be used to help inhibit prepotent responses, thus leading to new, adaptive interpersonal dynamics (Hajal & Paley,
2020).
Conclusions
To advance innovation in clinical assessment and intervention, a rigorous understanding of biobehavioral and environmental mechanisms of child psychopathology is needed. The RDoC matrix provides a guiding framework for this goal but traditional intrapersonal assessment of its units of analysis fails to consider critical, real-time interpersonal influences on caregivers’ and children’s functioning. Compelling, well-replicated evidence of caregiver-child interpersonal dynamics in each unit of analysis illuminates that the interpersonal environment is already intrinsic to the RDoC matrix. Studying each unit of analysis through the lens of interpersonal dynamics makes explicit this intrinsic connection between intrapersonal functioning and moment-to-moment environmental influences during social interactions. Further, we argue that the RDoC framework and the study of interpersonal dynamics can be deepened through the examination of cross-domain intersections, which involve interrelated domains elicited by units of analysis and assessment paradigms, and evaluation of the drivers of interpersonal dynamics. Our methodological proof-of-concept among a sample of emotionally at-risk preadolescent girls and their parents offers empirical support for the proposed research agenda. Clinically, assessment and intervention techniques that leverage the caregiver-child interpersonal context may bring light to novel approaches by to fulfill the promises of the RDoC framework for improving child and adolescent mental health.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.