Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 7/2023

13-02-2023 | Research

Individual differences in the frequency of voluntary & involuntary episodic memories, future thoughts, and counterfactual thoughts

Auteur: Jared G. Branch

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 7/2023

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Voluntary and involuntary mental time travel can take the form of episodic memory, episodic future thinking, and episodic counterfactual thinking. This study uses an individual-differences approach to understand why people engage in these forms of mental time travel. The individual-differences variables include trait-level personality, boredom proneness, depression, anxiety, stress, emotion regulation, mindfulness, mind-wandering, positive and negative affect, rumination, optimism, thinking styles, and time perspective. Across two studies, our results indicate that individual differences underlie these forms of mental time travel. The most unique, episodic counterfactual thinking, was alone positively correlated with negative emotionality and negatively correlated with optimism. We also observe differences as a function of voluntariness and discuss these findings in relation to the cognitively demanding nature of constructing future and counterfactual thoughts. We discuss the importance of distinguishing voluntary from involuntary thinking and assessing episodic counterfactual thinking in relation to episodic memory and episodic future thinking.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Addis, D. R. (2018). Are episodic memories special? On the sameness of remembered and imagined event simulation. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand, 48(2–3), 64–88.CrossRef Addis, D. R. (2018). Are episodic memories special? On the sameness of remembered and imagined event simulation. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand, 48(2–3), 64–88.CrossRef
go back to reference Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19(1), 33–41.PubMedCrossRef Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19(1), 33–41.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N., & Schacter, D. L. (2009). Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsychologia, 47(11), 2222–2238.PubMedCrossRef Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N., & Schacter, D. L. (2009). Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsychologia, 47(11), 2222–2238.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Arch, J. J., Wilcox, R. R., Ives, L. T., Sroloff, A., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2021). Off-task thinking among adults with and without social anxiety disorder: An ecological momentary assessment study. Cognition and Emotion, 35(2), 269–281.PubMedCrossRef Arch, J. J., Wilcox, R. R., Ives, L. T., Sroloff, A., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2021). Off-task thinking among adults with and without social anxiety disorder: An ecological momentary assessment study. Cognition and Emotion, 35(2), 269–281.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bacon, A. M., Walsh, C. R., & Martin, L. (2013). Fantasy proneness and counterfactual thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(4), 469–473.CrossRef Bacon, A. M., Walsh, C. R., & Martin, L. (2013). Fantasy proneness and counterfactual thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(4), 469–473.CrossRef
go back to reference Bacon, A. M., Walsh, C. R., Briazu, A., & R. (2020). Looking behind and looking ahead: Personality differences in counterfactual and prefactual thinking. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 40(2), 111–125.CrossRef Bacon, A. M., Walsh, C. R., Briazu, A., & R. (2020). Looking behind and looking ahead: Personality differences in counterfactual and prefactual thinking. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 40(2), 111–125.CrossRef
go back to reference Barzykowski, K., & Niedźwieńska, A. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memories are relatively more often reported during high cognitive load tasks. Acta Psychologica, 182, 119–128.PubMedCrossRef Barzykowski, K., & Niedźwieńska, A. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memories are relatively more often reported during high cognitive load tasks. Acta Psychologica, 182, 119–128.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Oettingen, G. (2016). Pragmatic prospection: How and why people think about the future. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 3–16.CrossRef Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Oettingen, G. (2016). Pragmatic prospection: How and why people think about the future. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 3–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F., Hofmann, W., Summerville, A., Reiss, P. T., & Vohs, K. D. (2020). Everyday thoughts in time: Experience sampling studies of mental time travel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(12), 1631–1648.PubMedCrossRef Baumeister, R. F., Hofmann, W., Summerville, A., Reiss, P. T., & Vohs, K. D. (2020). Everyday thoughts in time: Experience sampling studies of mental time travel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(12), 1631–1648.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2019). Thinking about the past and future in daily life: An experience sampling study of individual differences in mental time travel. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 805–816.PubMedCrossRef Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2019). Thinking about the past and future in daily life: An experience sampling study of individual differences in mental time travel. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 805–816.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Benoit, R. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450–457.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Benoit, R. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450–457.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D. (2021). Involuntary autobiographical memories and their relation to other forms of spontaneous thoughts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1817), 20190693.CrossRef Berntsen, D. (2021). Involuntary autobiographical memories and their relation to other forms of spontaneous thoughts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1817), 20190693.CrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 352–372.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 352–372.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D., Hoyle, R. H., & Rubin, D. C. (2019). The Autobiographical Recollection Test (ART): A measure of individual differences in autobiographical memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(3), 305–318.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Berntsen, D., Hoyle, R. H., & Rubin, D. C. (2019). The Autobiographical Recollection Test (ART): A measure of individual differences in autobiographical memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(3), 305–318.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Biderman, N., Bakkour, A., & Shohamy, D. (2020). What are memories for? The hippocampus bridges past experience with future decisions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(7), 542–556.PubMedCrossRef Biderman, N., Bakkour, A., & Shohamy, D. (2020). What are memories for? The hippocampus bridges past experience with future decisions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(7), 542–556.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blix, I., Kanten, A. B., Birkeland, M. S., & Thoresen, S. (2018). Imagining what could have happened: Types and vividness of counterfactual thoughts and the relationship with post-traumatic stress reactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 515.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Blix, I., Kanten, A. B., Birkeland, M. S., & Thoresen, S. (2018). Imagining what could have happened: Types and vividness of counterfactual thoughts and the relationship with post-traumatic stress reactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 515.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Branch, J. G., & Zickar, M. J. (2021). Mental time travel into the episodic future, episodic past, and episodic counterfactual past in everyday life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 35(1), 293–299.CrossRef Branch, J. G., & Zickar, M. J. (2021). Mental time travel into the episodic future, episodic past, and episodic counterfactual past in everyday life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 35(1), 293–299.CrossRef
go back to reference Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822.PubMedCrossRef Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brunette, A. M., & Schacter, D. L. (2021). Cognitive mechanisms of episodic simulation in psychiatric populations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 136, 103778.PubMedCrossRef Brunette, A. M., & Schacter, D. L. (2021). Cognitive mechanisms of episodic simulation in psychiatric populations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 136, 103778.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carciofo, R., & Jiang, P. (2021). Deliberate and spontaneous mind wandering in Chinese students: Associations with mindfulness, affect, personality, and life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 180, 110982.CrossRef Carciofo, R., & Jiang, P. (2021). Deliberate and spontaneous mind wandering in Chinese students: Associations with mindfulness, affect, personality, and life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 180, 110982.CrossRef
go back to reference Carriere, J. S., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19.PubMedCrossRef Carriere, J. S., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chaieb, L., Hoppe, C., & Fell, J. (2022). Mind wandering and depression: a status report. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 133, 104505.CrossRef Chaieb, L., Hoppe, C., & Fell, J. (2022). Mind wandering and depression: a status report. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 133, 104505.CrossRef
go back to reference Cole, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2021). Spontaneous and deliberate future thinking: A dual process account. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(2), 464–479.PubMedCrossRef Cole, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2021). Spontaneous and deliberate future thinking: A dual process account. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(2), 464–479.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S. R., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44(3), 376–389.CrossRef Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S. R., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44(3), 376–389.CrossRef
go back to reference Cole, S. N., Smith, D. M., Ragan, K., Suurmond, R., & Armitage, C. J. (2021). Synthesizing the effects of mental simulation on behavior change: Systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(5), 1514–1537.CrossRef Cole, S. N., Smith, D. M., Ragan, K., Suurmond, R., & Armitage, C. J. (2021). Synthesizing the effects of mental simulation on behavior change: Systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(5), 1514–1537.CrossRef
go back to reference Danckert, J., Hammerschmidt, T., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2018). Boredom: Under-aroused and restless. Consciousness and Cognition, 61, 24–37.PubMedCrossRef Danckert, J., Hammerschmidt, T., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2018). Boredom: Under-aroused and restless. Consciousness and Cognition, 61, 24–37.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 96–103.CrossRef D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 96–103.CrossRef
go back to reference De Brigard, F. (2014). Is memory for remembering? Recollection as a form of episodic hypothetical thinking. Synthese, 191(2), 155–185.CrossRef De Brigard, F. (2014). Is memory for remembering? Recollection as a form of episodic hypothetical thinking. Synthese, 191(2), 155–185.CrossRef
go back to reference De Brigard, F., & Giovanello, K. S. (2012). Influence of outcome valence in the subjective experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1085–1096.PubMedCrossRef De Brigard, F., & Giovanello, K. S. (2012). Influence of outcome valence in the subjective experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1085–1096.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Brigard, F., & Parikh, N. (2019). Episodic counterfactual thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 59–66.CrossRef De Brigard, F., & Parikh, N. (2019). Episodic counterfactual thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 59–66.CrossRef
go back to reference De Brigard, F., Addis, D. R., Ford, J. H., Schacter, D. L., & Giovanello, K. S. (2013a). Remembering what could have happened: Neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking. Neuropsychologia, 51(12), 2401–2414.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef De Brigard, F., Addis, D. R., Ford, J. H., Schacter, D. L., & Giovanello, K. S. (2013a). Remembering what could have happened: Neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking. Neuropsychologia, 51(12), 2401–2414.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference De Brigard, F., Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013b). Coming to grips with the past: Effect of repeated simulation on the perceived plausibility of episodic counterfactual thoughts. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1329–1334.PubMedCrossRef De Brigard, F., Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013b). Coming to grips with the past: Effect of repeated simulation on the perceived plausibility of episodic counterfactual thoughts. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1329–1334.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Brigard, F., Rodriguez, D. C., & Montañés, P. (2017). Exploring the experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking in younger and older adults: A study of a Colombian sample. Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 258–267.PubMedCrossRef De Brigard, F., Rodriguez, D. C., & Montañés, P. (2017). Exploring the experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking in younger and older adults: A study of a Colombian sample. Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 258–267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2019). The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: An exploratory study. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 788–804.PubMedCrossRef del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2019). The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: An exploratory study. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 788–804.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2020). Involuntary autobiographical memories and future projections in social anxiety. Memory, 28(4), 516–527.PubMedCrossRef del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2020). Involuntary autobiographical memories and future projections in social anxiety. Memory, 28(4), 516–527.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deng, Y. Q., Li, S., & Tang, Y. Y. (2014). The relationship between wandering mind, depression and mindfulness. Mindfulness, 5(2), 124–128.CrossRef Deng, Y. Q., Li, S., & Tang, Y. Y. (2014). The relationship between wandering mind, depression and mindfulness. Mindfulness, 5(2), 124–128.CrossRef
go back to reference Espino, O., & Byrne, R. M. (2021). How people keep track of what is real and what is imagined: The epistemic status of counterfactual alternatives to reality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(4), 547.PubMed Espino, O., & Byrne, R. M. (2021). How people keep track of what is real and what is imagined: The epistemic status of counterfactual alternatives to reality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(4), 547.PubMed
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Feng, X., Gu, R., Liang, F., Broster, L. S., Liu, Y., Zhang, D., & Luo, Y. J. (2015). Depressive states amplify both upward and downward counterfactual thinking. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(2), 93–98.PubMedCrossRef Feng, X., Gu, R., Liang, F., Broster, L. S., Liu, Y., Zhang, D., & Luo, Y. J. (2015). Depressive states amplify both upward and downward counterfactual thinking. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(2), 93–98.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the big-five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528.CrossRef Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the big-five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528.CrossRef
go back to reference Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348.PubMedCrossRef Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hallford, D. J., & D’Argembeau, A. (2022). Why we imagine our future: Introducing the Functions of Future Thinking Scale (FoFTS). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 44(2), 376–395.CrossRef Hallford, D. J., & D’Argembeau, A. (2022). Why we imagine our future: Introducing the Functions of Future Thinking Scale (FoFTS). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 44(2), 376–395.CrossRef
go back to reference Hallford, D. J., Barry, T. J., Austin, D. W., Raes, F., Takano, K., & Klein, B. (2020). Impairments in episodic future thinking for positive events and anticipatory pleasure in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 536–543.PubMedCrossRef Hallford, D. J., Barry, T. J., Austin, D. W., Raes, F., Takano, K., & Klein, B. (2020). Impairments in episodic future thinking for positive events and anticipatory pleasure in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 536–543.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoppen, T. H., Heinz-Fischer, I., & Morina, N. (2020). If only… a systematic review and meta-analysis of social, temporal and counterfactual comparative thinking in PTSD. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1737453.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hoppen, T. H., Heinz-Fischer, I., & Morina, N. (2020). If only… a systematic review and meta-analysis of social, temporal and counterfactual comparative thinking in PTSD. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1737453.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Isacescu, J., Struk, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 31(8), 1741–1748.PubMedCrossRef Isacescu, J., Struk, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 31(8), 1741–1748.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kane, M. J., Gross, G. M., Chun, C. A., Smeekens, B. A., Meier, M. E., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2017). For whom the mind wanders, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1271–1289.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kane, M. J., Gross, G. M., Chun, C. A., Smeekens, B. A., Meier, M. E., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2017). For whom the mind wanders, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1271–1289.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kvavilashvili, L., & Rummel, J. (2020). On the nature of everyday prospection: A review and theoretical integration of research on mind-wandering, future thinking, and prospective memory. Review of General Psychology, 24(3), 210–237.CrossRef Kvavilashvili, L., & Rummel, J. (2020). On the nature of everyday prospection: A review and theoretical integration of research on mind-wandering, future thinking, and prospective memory. Review of General Psychology, 24(3), 210–237.CrossRef
go back to reference Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343.PubMedCrossRef Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, A. K. (2016). Prospection, well-being and memory. Memory Studies, 9(3), 266–274.CrossRef MacLeod, A. K. (2016). Prospection, well-being and memory. Memory Studies, 9(3), 266–274.CrossRef
go back to reference Maillet, D., Beaty, R. E., Jordano, M. L., Touron, D. R., Adnan, A., Silvia, P. J., Kwapil, T. R., Turner, G. R., Spreng, R. N., & Kane, M. J. (2018). Age-related differences in mind-wandering in daily life. Psychology and Aging, 33(4), 643.PubMedCrossRef Maillet, D., Beaty, R. E., Jordano, M. L., Touron, D. R., Adnan, A., Silvia, P. J., Kwapil, T. R., Turner, G. R., Spreng, R. N., & Kane, M. J. (2018). Age-related differences in mind-wandering in daily life. Psychology and Aging, 33(4), 643.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation model of comparative thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(3), 244–267.PubMedCrossRef Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation model of comparative thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(3), 244–267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martarelli, C. S., Bertrams, A., & Wolff, W. (2021). A personality trait-based network of boredom, spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. Assessment, 28(8), 1915–1931.PubMedCrossRef Martarelli, C. S., Bertrams, A., & Wolff, W. (2021). A personality trait-based network of boredom, spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. Assessment, 28(8), 1915–1931.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mazzoni, G. (2019). Involuntary memories and involuntary future thinking differently tax cognitive resources. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 684–697.PubMedCrossRef Mazzoni, G. (2019). Involuntary memories and involuntary future thinking differently tax cognitive resources. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 684–697.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Michaelian, K., Perrin, D., & Sant’Anna, A. (2020). Continuities and discontinuities between imagination and memory: The view from philosophy. The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination, 293, 293–310.CrossRef Michaelian, K., Perrin, D., & Sant’Anna, A. (2020). Continuities and discontinuities between imagination and memory: The view from philosophy. The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination, 293, 293–310.CrossRef
go back to reference Mitchell, M. A., Contractor, A. A., Dranger, P., & Shea, M. T. (2016). Unique relations between counterfactual thinking and DSM–5 PTSD symptom clusters. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 8(3), 293.PubMedCrossRef Mitchell, M. A., Contractor, A. A., Dranger, P., & Shea, M. T. (2016). Unique relations between counterfactual thinking and DSM–5 PTSD symptom clusters. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 8(3), 293.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 560.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 560.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Nicosia, J., & Balota, D. (2021). Dispositional factors account for age differences in self-reported mind-wandering. Psychology and Aging, 4, 421.CrossRef Nicosia, J., & Balota, D. (2021). Dispositional factors account for age differences in self-reported mind-wandering. Psychology and Aging, 4, 421.CrossRef
go back to reference Norris, P., & Epstein, S. (2011). An experiential thinking style: Its facets and relations with objective and subjective criterion measures. Journal of Personality, 79(5), 1043–1080.PubMedCrossRef Norris, P., & Epstein, S. (2011). An experiential thinking style: Its facets and relations with objective and subjective criterion measures. Journal of Personality, 79(5), 1043–1080.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Özbek, M., Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2017). Imagining the personal past: Episodic counterfactuals compared to episodic memories and episodic future projections. Memory & Cognition, 45(3), 375–389.CrossRef Özbek, M., Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2017). Imagining the personal past: Episodic counterfactuals compared to episodic memories and episodic future projections. Memory & Cognition, 45(3), 375–389.CrossRef
go back to reference Özbek, M., Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2020). Characteristics of personally important episodic memories, counterfactual thoughts, and future projections across age and culture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(5), 1020–1033.CrossRef Özbek, M., Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2020). Characteristics of personally important episodic memories, counterfactual thoughts, and future projections across age and culture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(5), 1020–1033.CrossRef
go back to reference Ozdes, A. (2021). How did I do it then? How will I do it later? A theoretical review of the impact of mental time travel on decision-making processes. New Ideas in Psychology, 62, 100869.CrossRef Ozdes, A. (2021). How did I do it then? How will I do it later? A theoretical review of the impact of mental time travel on decision-making processes. New Ideas in Psychology, 62, 100869.CrossRef
go back to reference Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1842–1846.PubMedCrossRef Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1842–1846.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Robison, M. K., Gath, K. I., & Unsworth, N. (2017). The neurotic wandering mind: An individual differences investigation of neuroticism, mind-wandering, and executive control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(4), 649–663.PubMedCrossRef Robison, M. K., Gath, K. I., & Unsworth, N. (2017). The neurotic wandering mind: An individual differences investigation of neuroticism, mind-wandering, and executive control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(4), 649–663.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Robison, M. K., Miller, A. L., & Unsworth, N. (2020). A multi-faceted approach to understanding individual differences in mind-wandering. Cognition, 198, 104078.PubMedCrossRef Robison, M. K., Miller, A. L., & Unsworth, N. (2020). A multi-faceted approach to understanding individual differences in mind-wandering. Cognition, 198, 104078.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking: New evidence, new challenges, new insights. Academic Press. Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking: New evidence, new challenges, new insights. Academic Press.
go back to reference Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773–786.CrossRef Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773–786.CrossRef
go back to reference Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063.PubMedCrossRef Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Seli, P., Beaty, R. E., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2019). Depression, anxiety, and stress and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(2), 163. Seli, P., Beaty, R. E., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2019). Depression, anxiety, and stress and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(2), 163.
go back to reference Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(1), 117.PubMedCrossRef Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(1), 117.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spreng, R. N., & Levine, B. (2006). The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan. Memory & Cognition, 34(8), 1644–1651.CrossRef Spreng, R. N., & Levine, B. (2006). The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan. Memory & Cognition, 34(8), 1644–1651.CrossRef
go back to reference Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S., Cheyne, J. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment, 24(3), 346–359.PubMedCrossRef Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S., Cheyne, J. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment, 24(3), 346–359.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299–313.PubMedCrossRef Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299–313.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Get real: Effects of repeated simulation and emotion on the perceived plausibility of future experiences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 323.PubMedCrossRef Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Get real: Effects of repeated simulation and emotion on the perceived plausibility of future experiences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 323.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.CrossRef Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.CrossRef
go back to reference Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.CrossRef Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1–25.PubMedCrossRef Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Hoeck, N., Ma, N., Ampe, L., Baetens, K., Vandekerckhove, M., & Van Overwalle, F. (2013). Counterfactual thinking: An fMRI study on changing the past for a better future. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(5), 556–564.PubMedCrossRef Van Hoeck, N., Ma, N., Ampe, L., Baetens, K., Vandekerckhove, M., & Van Overwalle, F. (2013). Counterfactual thinking: An fMRI study on changing the past for a better future. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(5), 556–564.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(1–2), 115–123.CrossRef Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(1–2), 115–123.CrossRef
go back to reference Vowinckel, J. C., Westerhof, G. J., Bohlmeijer, E. T., & Webster, J. D. (2017). Flourishing in the now: Initial validation of a present-eudaimonic time perspective scale. Time & Society, 26(2), 203–226.CrossRef Vowinckel, J. C., Westerhof, G. J., Bohlmeijer, E. T., & Webster, J. D. (2017). Flourishing in the now: Initial validation of a present-eudaimonic time perspective scale. Time & Society, 26(2), 203–226.CrossRef
go back to reference Webb, C. A., Israel, E. S., Belleau, E., Appleman, L., Forbes, E. E., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2021). Mind-wandering in adolescents predicts worse affect and is linked to aberrant default mode network–salience network connectivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(3), 377–387.CrossRef Webb, C. A., Israel, E. S., Belleau, E., Appleman, L., Forbes, E. E., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2021). Mind-wandering in adolescents predicts worse affect and is linked to aberrant default mode network–salience network connectivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(3), 377–387.CrossRef
go back to reference Wu, J. Q., Szpunar, K. K., Godovich, S. A., Schacter, D. L., & Hofmann, S. G. (2015). Episodic future thinking in generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 36, 1–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wu, J. Q., Szpunar, K. K., Godovich, S. A., Schacter, D. L., & Hofmann, S. G. (2015). Episodic future thinking in generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 36, 1–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Individual differences in the frequency of voluntary & involuntary episodic memories, future thoughts, and counterfactual thoughts
Auteur
Jared G. Branch
Publicatiedatum
13-02-2023
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 7/2023
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01802-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2023

Psychological Research 7/2023 Naar de uitgave