Introduction
Unraveling the Developmental Basis of Psychopathic Traits: The Role of Emotion Recognition
Emotion Recognition and Psychopathic Traits in Childhood and Adolescence
The Present Study
Method
Systematic Search Strategy
Database | PsycInfo | Scopus | Pubmed | WOS |
---|---|---|---|---|
Keywords | L1. (callous* unemotion* or CU or psychopathy or psychopathic) #a L2. (emotion* recognition or emotion* process* or emotion* identification or eye gaze or eye track* or eye fix* or facial emotion* or emotion* attent*) # L3. (child* or adolesc*) |
Fields | Any field (ALL) | TITLE-ABS-KEY | All fields | All fields |
---|---|---|---|---|
Resultsb | 939 | 321 | 1044 | 1273 |
Filters | 1. Language: English, Spanish 2. Age: 0–18 3. Peer Review 4. Humans (male, female, inpatient, outpatient) | 1. Language: English | 1. Language: English, Spanish 2. Humans 3. Age: 0–18 | 1. Language: English, Spanish |
Eligibility Criteria
Study Identification and Selection
Quality Assessment
Data Extraction and Synthesis
Study | Participants | CU measure (informant) | Emotion recognition | Main outcome specific to emotion recognition | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (female) | Sample type | Age range (M) | Type of stimuli | Emotions | Measurement | |||
Aghajani et al. (2021) | n = 81 (–) CD/LPE+ = 19 CD/LPE- = 31 *22% comorbid ADHD CG = 31 | Forensic Community | 15–19 (M CD/LPE ± = 16.84 M HC = 17.02) | LPE proxy from the Remorseless, Callousness and Unemotionality subscales of the YPI (SR) CD/LPE+ = two or more items > 5 | Facial | Fear Sad | RT and accuracy (% of correct attribution) | Controlling for age and IQ, but not ADHD: 1. There were no significant differences across groups in ER as measured by RT and % of correct attribution of other’s emotions |
Bennett and Kerig (2014) | n = 417 (111) Primary CU (1) = 55 Secondary CU (2) = 76 Low CU (3) = 279 | Forensic | 12–18 (16.5) (M1 = 15.85 M2 = 16.06 M3 = 16.26) | ICU Total score (SR) CU > 26 CG < 26 | Facial | Fear Anger Sad Shame Disgust | Accuracy (% of correct responses) | 1. The acquired (secondary) CU group were more accurate in recognizing others disgust 2. Membership in the primary CU group was less than the acquired group as accuracy increased for recognition of disgust, but was more likely as accuracy increased for shame 3. In relation to the Low CU group, the primary CU were more accurate in the recognition of anger. There were no differences with the acquired CU group |
Dadds et al. (2018) | n = 364 (102) *53% ODD/CD; 35.6% ADHD | Clinical | 3–16 (8.93) | APSD/SDQ CU derived measure (Combined PR, TR, SR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear Disgust Neutral | Accuracy (Total score) | Controlling for the effects of ADHD medication: 1. Low CU traits were related with more accurate ER 2. The relationship between CU traits and ER differed according to maltreatment history; CU traits were associated with poorer recognition of those with zero or negligible history of maltreatment 3. Patterns of moderation effects of maltreatment were inconsistent across subgroups. For the group who self-reported maltreatment, CU traits were related with poor ER in those with lower anxiety 4. Both direct and moderated effects of CU on ER were not limited to fear or sadness. Negative correlations were to some extent evident for all the analyzed emotions, except happiness |
De Ridder et al. (2016) | n = 55 (10) | Forensic | NR (14.8) | YPI CU subscale (SR) | Ecological | Anger Distress | Accuracy (Total score) | 1. High CU adolescents were as accurate as low CU adolescents in inferring distress and anger in staff members 2. High CU adolescents notably overestimated the general intensity of both anger and distress 3. High CU adolescents perceived more anger in staff members when they were misbehaving, particularly breaking the rules |
Ezpeleta et al. (2017) | n = 320 (155) CU+ /ODD+ = 51 CU+/ODD− = 24 CU−/ODD+ = 38 CU−/ODD− = 207 *8.7% comorbid ADHD (CG) = 207 | Community | 8 (–) | ICU Total score (TR) | Emoticons | Happy Sad Anger Fear Neutral | Accuracy RT | Controlling for sex, SES and comorbidities: 1. Children within the CU−/ODD+ and the CU+/ODD+ were less accurate than the CG in processing the information, specifically when the stimuli expressed happiness, fear or neutral 2. Children high on CU traits but low on ODD performed similarly to the CG with respect to both accuracy and RT with one exception: children high on CU traits responded faster to fear 3. Children within the CU+/ODD+ group differed in accuracy but not in RT in relation to the CG |
Kahn et al. (2017) | 112 (–) | Forensic | 12–20 (15.5) | ICU Total score (SR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Disgust Fear Neutral | Accuracy | 1. There were no significant main effects for CU traits or interactions with anxiety in predicting overall accuracy in the affective facial recognition task When predicting accuracy for independently identify the 6 analyzed emotions: 2. CU traits were positively associated with fear recognition accuracy at lower levels of anxiety 3. CU traits were related with poorer results in the recognition of disgust, with a trend suggesting that this relationship tend to occur at high levels of anxiety |
Kimonis et al. (2016) | n = 214 (100) | At-risk Community | 3–6 (4.7) | ICU Total score (24 and 12 items), Callousness and Uncaring subscales (Combined PR, TR) UNSW CU | Facial static and dynamic | Anger Fear Happy Sad Pain (only in dynamic) | Accuracy | Controlling for CP and sociodemographics: 1. Children scoring high on ICU Total and Uncaring subscale scores were less accurate in recognizing anger, fear, happiness, and sadness 2. Callousness was only associated with poor fear and sadness recognition, but not after covaring with uncaring 3. Uncaring remained significantly associated with anger, happy and sad recognition after controlling for callousness 4. There was a significant effect of age, with older children being more accurate in emotion recognition |
Klapwijk et al. (2016) | n = 79 (–) ASD = 23 CD/CU+ = 23 *34.78% comorbid ADHD CG = 33 | Clinical Forensic Community | 12–19 (MASD = 17 MCD/CU+ = 16.6 MCG = 17.1) | ICU Total score (SR) CU+ > 27 (median) | Facial | Anger Fear Neutral | Accuracy Congruency RT | 1. The CD/CU+ group reacted faster than the CG 2. There were no differences across groups in the identification of other’s emotional expression 3. The CD/CU+ group reported more congruency for anger as compared to fearful emotions |
Kohls et al. (2020a) | n = 1252 (796) CD = 542 (317) CG = 710 (479) | Clinical Forensic Community | 9–18 (MCD = 14.4 MCG = 14.0) | YPI CU dimension (SR) ICU (PR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear Disgust Surprise | Accuracy (% correct) | 1. There were higher levels of CU traits in the CD group, with no significant differences across boys and girls 2. Relative to the CG, youths with CD showed impaired emotion recognition, which was related to higher CU traits a more physical and proactive aggression 3. Elevated CU traits within the CD group were associated with overall emotion recognition impairments rather than deficits in particular emotions 3. Parent reported CU traits were not related with emotion recognition within the CD group 4.Overall, girls outperformed boys in emotion recognition |
Kohls et al. (2020b) | n = 1252 (796) CD = 542 (317) CG = 710 (479) | Clinical Forensic Community | 9–18 (MCD = 14.4 MCG = 14.0) | CU scale and LPE proxy from the Remorseless, Callousness and Unemotionality subscales of the YPI (SR) LPE+ = two or more items > 4 | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear Disgust Surprise | Accuracy (% correct) | 1. The subgroup of youths with CD with an emotion recognition deficit did not differ significantly from those without such deficit regarding the presence of the LPE specifier 2. Deficits in emotion processing, including emotion recognition, related to CD do not map neatly onto established DSM-5 subtypes, such as the CD+ LPE |
Lui et al. (2016) | n = 103 (33) | At-risk | 16–18 (16.93) | ICU Total score (SR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Disgust Fear Neutral | Accuracy (Total score) | 1. CU traits were negatively correlated with emotion recognition 2. Emotion recognition did not mediate the association between CU traits and both affective and cognitive empathy |
Martin-Key et al. (2017) | n = 77 (–) CD = 37 (CU+ = 20; CU− = 17) *46% comorbid ADHD CG = 40 | Forensic Community | 13–18 (M CD = 16.03) M CG = 16.20) | ICU Total score (SR) CD/CU+ > 30 (median split) CD/CU− ≤ 30 | Facial dynamic | Happy Anger Sad Disgust Fear Surprise | Accuracy | 1. There were no differences between CD groups in terms of ADHD 2. Participants in the CD group were less accurate than CG in the recognition of sadness, fear and disgust 3. Yet, no significant differences were found between CD/CU+ and CD/CU− groups, for any emotion 4. When CU traits were treated dimensionally, no significant correlations were found between CU traits and emotion recognition |
Martin-Key et al. (2020) | n = (52) CD = 23 (CU+ = 10 CU− = 13) *13% comorbid ADHD CG = 29 | Forensic Community | 13–18 (MCD = 16.06 MCG = 16.22) | ICU Total score (SR) CD/CU+ > 28 (median split) CD/CU− ≤ 28 | Facial dynamic | Anger Happy Sad Disgust Fear Surprise | Accuracy | 1. There were no significant differences in emotion recognition between CD and CG, nor between CD/CU+ and CD/CU− groups 2. When CU traits were treated dimensionally, there were no significant correlations between CU traits in the CD group and emotion recognition for any of the six emotions |
Milone et al. (2019) | n = 60 (–) | Clinical | 11–17 (13.27) | APSD CU (Combined PR, SR) | Facial (eye region) | Happy Sad | Accuracy | 1. The levels of CU traits were not related with abilities in emotion recognition 2. CD males high and low on CU traits did not differ in emotion recognition performance |
Moore et al. (2019) | n = 1214 (656) (607 twin pairs, 251 MZ, 356 DZ) | Community | 9.1–20.8 (14.1) | ICU Total score, Uncaring/Callous and Unemotional subscales (PR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear Disgust Surprise | Accuracy: Unbiased hit rate (UBHR) | 1. The uncaring/callous dimension was significantly associated with impaired recognition of happiness, sadness, fear, surprise and disgust 2. The unemotional dimension was significantly associated with improved recognition of surprise and disgust 3. The total ICU score was significantly associated with impaired recognition of sadness 4. The relationship between uncaring/callousness and deficits in distress cues recognition (i.e., fear and sadness) was entirely accounted by shared genetic influences |
Muñoz (2009) | n = 55 (–) | At-risk | 8–16 (11.8) | ICU Total score and subscales (SR) | Facial and body posture | Happy Sad Fear Anger Surprise Disgust (only in faces) | Accuracy | 1.Youths who were CU+ traits had fewer correct responses to fear and anger faces 2.Deficit in labelling fear faces and fear body postures among youths scoring highly on CU traits, although accuracy was lower for more types of emotions than fear Deficits specific to fear for CU+ 3.Boys who were poor at labelling both the fear faces and fear body postures had the highest levels of total CU traits |
O’Kearney et al. (2017) | n = 124 (45) *ODD = 43 (31) CG = 17 (14) *55.4% comorbid ADHD | Clinical Community | 4–8 (MODD = 5.9 MCG = 5.9) | ICU Total score (PR) ODD/CU+ ≥ 67% (median split) ODD/CU− < 67% | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear | Accuracy | 1.The were no significant differences between groups for emotion recognition |
O’Kearney et al. (2020) | n = 74 (31) ODD = 74 | Clinical | 4–8 (MODD = 5.9) | ICU Total score (PR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear | Accuracy | 1.CU had a small negative first-order association with the ability to understand mixed emotions but not with the other emotions abilities 2. Only at lower levels of affect arousal/dysregulation there is a notable association between higher CU and poorer emotion recognition |
Pauli et al. (2021) | n = 1263 (536) CD/CU+ = 248 (153) CD/CU− = 230 (130) CG = 785 (523) | Clinical Forensic Community | 9–18 | ICU Total score (SR) CD/CU+ ≥ 39 (median split) CD/CU− < 29 | Facial | Anger Disgust Fear Happy Sad Surprise | Accuracy (% correct) | 1.The CD/CU+ group performed significantly worse than de GC group, but the CD/CU− group did not differ from either group 2.Recognition accuracy was better for high-intensity than for low- intensity emotions and GC and CD/CU− groups benefited more from the increased expression intensity than the CD/CU+ 3.Group differences in emotion recognition abilities were not driven by difficulties with specific emotions such as fear and sadness 4. Female youths significantly outperformed male youths, as evidenced by a main effect of sex |
Peticlerc et al. (2019) | n = 1005 (–) (504 twin pairs, 209 MZ, 295 DZ) | Community | 6–7 | Four items ad hoc 1. “he/she didn’t seem to feel guilty after misbehaving” 2.” he/she has been insensitive to others' feelings” 3.” his/her emotions appear superficial” 4.” has not kept his/her promises” | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear | Accuracy | Controlling for other problems (physical aggression, ADHD and depressive symptoms), and for the recognition of other emotions: 1.There was a relationship between CU traits, measured in kindergarten and first grade, and poor emotional recognition of fear, measured in first grade 2. The relationship between CU traits and deficits in fear recognition was genetically influenced 3. The relationship between CU traits and deficits in the recognition of sadness did not hold after controlling for other problems and the recognition of other emotions. No genetic influence was reflected in this relationship, although evidence was provided for the influence of the non-shared environment |
Rehder et al. (2017) | n = 761 (657) EA = 446 CP+ CU = 20 CG = 442 AA = 315 CP+ CU = 16 CG = 291 | Community at risk | 6–7 | ICU Empathic-prosocial and callous subscales (PR) | Facial | Anger Sad Happy Fear | Accuracy (overall and specific emotions) | Controlling for primary caregivers’ years of education and children’s age: 1.Differences among typical children (CG), children with CP-only and children with CP + CU were moderated by child race and family income for overall emotion recognition, and by child race for specific emotion recognition 2. Only among EA children, and children within families with higher income-to-needs ratio (i.e., no extreme poverty), CG showed better accuracy for overall emotion recognition than children in the CP only and CP + CU groups 3. Only among EA children, CG showed better recognition for happy faces than children in the CP only and CP + CU group 4. Children in the CP only and CP + CU groups often did not perform differently on emotion recognition accuracy |
Schwenck et al. (2011) | n = 192 (–) ASD = 55 CD = 70 (CD/CU+ = 36 CD/CU− = 34) *44.8% comorbid ADHD CG = 67 | Clinical | 6–17 (12.3) | ICU Total score (PR) CD/CU+ ≥ 32 (median split) CD/CU− < 32 | Facial morphed | Anger Happy Sad Fear Disgust | Accuracy RT | 1. There were no significant differences between CD groups (CU+/CU−) in emotion recognition based on RT and the number of correctly identified emotions 2. Removing ADHD children receiving medical treatment did not change the pattern of results |
Schwenck et al. (2014) | n = (64) CP = 32 (CP/CU+ = 16; CP/CU− = 16) *62.6% comorbid ADHD CG = 32 | Clinical | 8–16 (13.23) | ICU Total score (PR) CP/CU+ > 35.5 (median split) CP/CU− < 35.5 | Facial morphed | Anger Happy Sad Fear Disgust | Accuracy RT | There were no significant differences in the analyzed variables between children with and without comorbid ADHD Regarding RT: 1. Girls with CP/CU− reacted more slowly than CG to faces developing happy, sad, and fearful expressions 2. Girls with CP/CU+ did not differ from CP/CU− and CG Regarding accuracy: 3. Girls with CP/CU+ recognized fearful expressions better than the other two groups 4. Girls with CP/CU− recognized sad expressions less often than CG 5. Girls with CP/CU+ mistook sad faces more commonly as disgusted faces than CG; whilst girls with CP/CU− identified fearful faces as disgusted more often than the CP/CU+ group |
White et al. (2016) | n = 337 (185) (High LC = 107; Low LC- = 230; High PI = 96; Low PI = 241) | At-risk | 3–7 (4.82) | MAP-DB Low Concern, Punishment Insensitivity (PR) High/Low groups ≥ /≤ 80th percentile | Facial | Fear Happy Anger Neutral | Accuracy Latency (mean RT) | Controlling for age, sex, temper loss, aggression, impulsivity, and the control block In terms of Accuracy: 1. The High LC group was less accurate in identifying fearful faces than the low LC; the accuracy rates did not differ for angry or happy faces 2. There were no differences between High and Low PI groups 3. Female were more accurately to emotional facial expressions than males In terms of Latency: 3. No significant differences were observed between High/Low LC and PI groups |
Wolf and Muñoz (2014) | n = 50 (–) | At-risk | 11–16 (14.3) | YPI CU subscale ICU Total score (SR) | Facial and Body postures Dynamic | Fear Anger Happy Disgust Sad Pain | Accuracy | Controlling for age and violent delinquency: 1. Fearful facial and bodily expressions were unrelated with CU traits 2. For the YPI-CU, there were deficits in the recognition of pain in faces, often misidentified as sadness and disgust, and anger in postures, often misidentified as happiness and disgust 3. For the ICU, results showed a significant enhancement in the recognition of anger in faces and disgust in postures |
Woodworth and Waschbusch (2007) | n = 73 (14) CP = 32 CP/CU = 24 CG = 18 *84.9% comorbid ADHD | Clinical Community | 7–12.78 (9.81) | APSD CU (Combined PR, TR) CU+ ≥ 67 (median split) CU− ≤ 63 | Facial | Anger Disgust Fear Happy Sad Surprise | Accuracy | Controlling age, sex, IQ and ADHD 1. Children with higher CU scores were less accurate in labelling sad affect than children with lower CU scores 2. Children with higher CU scores were more accurate in labelling fear than children with lower CU scores, but this main effect was qualified by the CP-CU interaction trend 3. Children with high CP but low CU traits were less accurate than other children in interpreting fearful facial emotions |
Study | Participants | Psychopathy measure (informant) | Emotion recognition | Main outcome specific to emotion recognition | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (female) | Sample type | Age range (M) | Type of stimuli | Emotions | Measurement | |||
Blair and Coles (2000) | n = 55 (24) PSD = 11 CG = 10 | Community | 11–14 (12.4) | PSD Total score (TR) | Facial | Happy Surprise Fear Sad Disgust Anger | Accuracy (Total correct answers) | 1. Higher scores in psychopathic traits were related with lower emotional recognition, as well as with an increased impairment in the recognition of anger, sadness and fearful expressions 2. At the dimensional level, Factor 1 (GM/CU) was inversely correlated with the ability to recognize sadness and fear. Factor 2 (INS) was inversely correlated with the ability to recognize fearful expressions 3. When comparing children high and low in psychopathic traits, results revealed a poorer recognition of sadness in the high group, controlling for mental age and sex |
Blair et al. (2001) | n = 51 (-) PP = 20 CG = 31 | At-risk | 9–17 (MPP = 12.93) (MCG = 12.84) | PSD Total score (TR) PP > 28 CG < 20 | Facial (morphed) | Happy Sad Surprise Fear Disgust Anger | Sensitivity (nº of stages needed to correctly identify the expression) Accuracy (% of errors) | 1.Children with psychopathic tendencies needed significantly more stages before they could successfully recognize sad expressions 2.Children with psychopathic tendencies made more errors when processing fearful expressions, being more likely to misclassify fear as one of the other five basic emotions 3. There were no group differences for the recognition of happy, anger, disgust and surprise |
Blair et al. (2005) | n = 43 (–) PP = 22 CG = 21 | At-risk | 11.8–15.5 (MPP = 13.5) (MCG = 12.87) | APSD Total score (TR) PP > 25 CG < 25 | Vocal | Happy Disgust Anger Sad Fear | Accuracy (% errors) | 1. Boys within the PP group overall made more errors than the CG 2. Boys with PP presented a selective impairment for the recognition of fearful vocal affect 3. Boys PP group did not show significant impairment in the recognition of sad vocal affect 4. When making errors on angry or disgusted expressions, children within the PP group were more likely to mistake these expressions for fearful expression 5. Within the PP group, the covariate age had a significant positive effect in the recognition of fearful affect, and the IQ had a significant effect in the recognition of fearful and disgusted vocal affects 6. At the dimensional level, there were a positive correlation between CU traits and the number of fearful and happy vocal affect recognition errors, and between GM traits and the number of fearful recognition errors. There were no significant correlations with INS |
Bowen et al. (2014) | n = 100 (–) Offenders = 63 CG = 37 | Forensic Community | 13–17 (Moff = 15.79) (MCG = 15.41) | YPI Total score and CU subscale (SR) High > 2.5 Low < 2.5 | Facial (morphed) | Happy Sad Fear Anger Disgust Surprise | Accuracy (% correct recognition scores at each intensity) | 1.Young offenders with high psychopathic traits were significantly worse at detecting 50% and 75% intensity of disgusted faces 2. Having high levels of psychopathic traits explained enhanced, rather than diminished, recognition of sad expressions 3. There was a positive correlation between CU traits and 25% and 100% anger recognition |
Dadds et al. (2006) | Study 1 = 33 (–) Study 2 = 65 (–) | Community | Study 1 = 8–15 (12.07) Study 2 = 9–17 (13.2) | APSD/SDQ CU and AB (GM/INS) subscales (Combined PR-SR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Disgust Fear Neutral | Accuracy | 1. AB (GM-INS) and CU traits were associated with different ER problems in young males 2. AB was uniquely associated with a tendency to over-interpret hostility in neutral faces 3. CU traits were uniquely related to poor recognition of fearful expressions, which are in part owing to visual neglect of the eye region of other people’s eyes 4. This deficit improved in the eye gaze condition, but returned in the mouth gaze condition |
Fairchild et al. (2009) | n = 121 (–) EO-CD = 42 AO-CD = 39 *21% comorbid ADHD CG = 40 | Community At-risk | 14–18 (EO-CD = 15.8) (AO-CD = 15.5) (CG = 15.8) | YPI Total score (SR) High > 2.5 | Facial (morphed) | Anger Disgust Fear Happy Sad Surprise | Accuracy (Total score) | 1. Relative to CG, recognition of anger, fear, disgust, and happiness was impaired in participants with EO-CD. Also, recognition of fear was impaired in participants with AO-CD. These results were replicated when removing participants with ADHD 2. Participants with CD who were high on psychopathic traits showed impaired fear, sadness and surprise recognition, as compared to those low in psychopathic traits |
Fairchild et al. (2010) | n = (55) CD = 25 *20% comorbid ADHD CG = 30 | Community At-risk | 14–18 | YPI Total score and CU (SR) High > 2.5 in Total score | Facial (morphed) | Anger Disgust Fear Happy Sad Surprise | Accuracy (Total score) | 1. Girls with CD showed impaired recognition of anger and disgust. These results were replicated when removing participants with ADHD 2. Participants with CD and high on psychopathic traits showed impaired recognition of sadness, as compared to those lower in psychopathic traits |
Gillen et al. (2018) | n = 144 (49) | Forensic | 11–18 (15.24) | PCL: YV subscales | Facial Vocal tone | Happy Sad Anger Fear | Accuracy (Total score) | 1. The PCL: YV Total score was related to lower accuracy of happy and sad faces 2. Interpersonal (GM) traits were positively related to fearful and angry facial emotion recognition accuracy, when controlling for the other two psychopathic factors 3. Affective (CU) traits were associated with poorer accuracy in identifying happy faces and sad, angry and fearful voices 4. Behavioral (INS) traits were not related to accuracy in recognizing emotional facial or vocal tones |
Kahn et al. (2016) | n = 141 (23) | Forensic | 14–18 (17.03) | PCL: YV ICU SR APSD SR CPS SR YPI SR All subscales | Facial | Happy Fear Surprise Disgust Excitement | Accuracy | 1. Controlling for IQ, there is no evidence of significant associations between psychopathic traits, measured by the PCL: YV, and Experiential EI 2. For self-reported measures, there was a negative association between the Callous-Disinhibited scale from the CPS and the experiential area, but it did not hold when controlling for multiple comparisons. The association was negative with the GM factor |
Lemos Vasconcellos et al. (2014) | n = 41 (–) (High PP = 20 Low PP = 21) | Forensic | 13–19 (M High PP = 16.3; M Low PP = 16.7) | PCL-YV Total score High PP ≥ 30 Low PP ≤ 20 | Facial | Fear Sad Happy Disgust Surprise Anger | Accuracy | 1. The High PP group was significantly poorer than the Low PP group in the recognition of fear in faces presented for 200 ms 2. No other differences between groups reached statistical significance; yet, effect sizes also demonstrated a moderate difference for fear recognition at 500 ms and 1 s, with worse performance for the High PP group, and small-to-moderate difference for sadness and surprise at 500 ms, with worse performance for the Low PP group |
Sharp et al. (2014) | n = 417 (187) | Community | 10–12 (11.33) | YPI subscales (SR) | Facial (eye region) | Happy Sad Anger Fear Disgust Surprise | Accuracy | 1. GM, CU and INS significantly (and inversely) correlated with emotion recognition 2. When the three dimensions were modeled together in a regression framework, only CU traits significantly predicted emotion recognition, but only in relation to complex emotions 3. No psychopathy dimension were predictive of basic emotion recognition |
Stevens et al. (2001) | n = 18 (–) (PSD+ = 9; PSD− = 9) | At-risk | 9–15 (11.7) | PSD Total score (TR) PSD+ > 25 PSD− < 20 | Facial Auditory | Happy Sad Anger Fear | Accuracy | 1. The PSD+ group was less likely to name the facial or vocal affect correctly than the PSD- group 2. Both groups were significantly more likely to name the facial expressions than the vocal affects 3. The PSD+ group showed selective impairments in the recognition of both sad and fearful facial expressions and sad vocal tone 4. The two groups did not differ in their recognition of happy of angry facial expressions, and fearful, happy and angry vocal tones |
Sylvers et al. (2011) | n = 88 (–) | At-risk | 7–11 (8.88) | APSD subscales (Combined PR-SR) | Facial | Neutral Happy Fear Disgust | RT Accuracy of face location | 1. Children with high CU scores exhibit preattentive or automatic preconscious fear-recognition deficits (large effect) and disgust recognition deficits (moderate effects) 2. CU traits provided an incremental contribution in predicting preattentive fear-recognition deficits (large effect) and disgust recognition deficits (moderate) above and beyond GM and INS traits 3. CU traits interacted with INS traits (moderate effect) in predicting preattentive fear-recognition deficits, with relation between CU traits and preattentive fear-recognition increasing as INS traits increased |
Study | Participants | CU/psychopathy measure (informant) | Emotion Recognition | Attention biases | Main outcome specific to attention biases and emotion recognition | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (female) | Sample type | Age range (M) | Type of stimuli | Emotion | Measurement | AOI | Measurement | |||
Bedford et al. (2017) | n = 206 (~ 105) | Community | Up to 7 years (MT1 = 6 m) (MT2 = 2.5) (MT3 = 6) (MT4 = 7) | ICU Total score (PR) | Facial | Happy Sad Mad Scared | Accuracy (Total score of correct/incorrect answers) | Face (not defined) | Proportion of the total valid interaction time | 1. Poorer ER at age 6 predicted higher CU trait at age 7, controlling for earlier CU traits (age 2.5) 2. Infants with low levels of gaze to the parent’s face showed higher CU traits at age 7, only when maternal sensitivity was low 3. Infant attention to the face was not related with later ER 4. ER appears to act as an independent predictor of later CU traits, rather than mediating their association with infant’s mother-directed gaze and maternal sensitivity |
Billeci et al. (2019) | n = 58 (–) CD = 16 ODD = 19 CG (HC) = 23 | Clinical | 7–10 (MDBD = 8.93) (MHC = 8.86) | APSD CU subscale (Combined PR, TR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear Disgust Neutral | Accuracy | Face Eyes Mouth | Number of fixations Average length of fixations Length of first fixation | 1. In the total sample, higher levels of CU were associated with a poorer recognition of sadness, and lower Nº fixations and Average length of fixations on the eye region of sad faces, even after controlling for covariates 2. In children with a DBD diagnosis, high levels of CU traits were associated with lower Average length of fixations on the eye areas of sad faces, which in turn predicts lower levels of sadness recognition (mediation process) |
Bours et al. (2018) | n = 122 (–) ASD = 50 ODD/CD = 44 *41% comorbid ADHD CG = 28 | Clinical | 12–19 (15.4) | ICU Total score (PR, SR) YPI Total score and CU subscale (SR) | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear Neutral | Accuracy (% correct answers) | Face Eyes Mouth | Total fixation duration Time to first fixation % Total fixation | 1. Higher scores in psychopathic traits within the ODD/CD group were related with a lower Time to first fixation to the eyes for the recognition of fear. This association did not survive when correcting for multiple comparisons 2. Sensitivity analyses revealed that results were not influenced by the presence of comorbid ADHD or by medication use |
Dadds et al. (2008) | n = 100 (–) | Community | 8–15 (12.4) | APSD/SDQ CU and AB (GM/INS) factors (Combined PR-SR) Low < 25th High > 75th | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Disgust Fear Neutral | Accuracy (Total score) | Free Eyes Mouth | Primacy Number & Duration of fixation | 1. High CU traits were associated with poorer fear recognition 2. High CU traits were related with lower number and duration of eye fixations, and fewer first foci to eye region, which would account for the deficit in recognizing fear 3. These results were not affected by the inclusion of antisocial problems (GM/INS), emotional problems, hyperactivity and peer problems as covariates 4. These deficits were no longer evident under the eye gaze condition, especially the number of times that subjects looked at the eyes first |
Dadds et al. (2011) | n = 92 (–) | Clinical | 5–16 (8.93) | APSD/SDQ CU and AB (GM/INS) subscale (Combined PR, TR, SR) High/low = combined 50th | Facial | Happy Sad Anger Fear Disgust Neutral | Accuracy (Total score of correct fear recognition) | Eyes | % Number of times eye contact was made divided by the number of intervals in which the dyad interacted | 1. Males high on CU traits showed consistent impairment in eye contact towards their parents 2. Fathers of high CU boys showed less eye contact with their child. Mothers of CU boys did not show impairments 3. Levels of eye contact were related with independent measures of fear recognition 4. Increased levels of eye contact in child-father and father-child dyads were related with increased recognition of fear 5.Levels of antisocial behavior (GM/INS), hyperactivity, anxiety/depression, peer problems, and autism type did not influence the results |
Demetriou and Fanti (2022) | n = 59 (27) CU+ = 31 (14) CU− (CG) = 28 (13) | Community | 5–10 (7.5) | CPTI CU subscale (PR: mothers & fathers) CU+ ≥ 1SD CU− ≤ 1SD | Facial | Happy Fear Sad Anger | Accuracy (Sum of missclassifications) | Eyes Mouth | Total fixation duration | 1. High CU children (CU +) made more accuracy errors, irrespective of the emotion expressed, compared to their CU− counterparts 2. High CU children showed lower rates of total fixation duration in the eye region for all the emotional expressions in both adult and peer pictures 3. High CU children showed an increased concentration to the mouth area compared to the CU− children, suggesting an ineffective pattern of processing emotions |
Hartmann and Schwenck (2020) | n = 94 (36) ODD = 29 (7) CD = 1 (1) *29% comorbid ADHD CG = 65 (30) | Clinical Community | 8–14 (10.4) | ICU Total score (PR) | Facial | Anger Sad Fear | Paradigm 1: Accuracy (error rates) RT Paradigm 2: Accuracy (error rates) | Eyes Mouth | Fixation count Total fixation duration | Paradigm 1: Recognition 1. Independent of CP (including ADHD), age and gender, CU traits were related with slower recognition of angry, sad and fearful facial expressions, but not with higher error rates Paradigm 2: Categorization 2. Higher levels of CU traits significantly predicted greater number of errors over all emotions 3. Higher levels of CU traits were related with higher mistakes during the anger trials, only when CP were high 4. Higher levels of CU traits predicted the number of mistakes in fear trials in which only the eyes were presented, but only when CP were low 5. Higher levels of CU traits predicted more mistakes in anger trials when only the eyes were presented. High levels of CP predicted low mistakes 6. High levels of CU traits predicted more mistakes in the sad trials when only the mouth of the stimuli was presented 7. There was no evidence that the association between CU traits and emotion processing could be explained by misguided attention |
Levantini et al., (2022a) | n = 116 (–) ODD = 94 CD = 22 *48.28% comorbid ADHD | Clinical | 7 –12 (9.0) | APSD subscales (Combined PR, TR) | Facial | Happy Sadness Anger Disgust Fear Neutral | Accuracy | Face Eyes Mouth | Number of fixations (FC), Average length of each fixation (FD), Length of first fixation (FFD) | Controlling for age, IQ, externalizing problems and SES: 1. CU traits were significantly and negatively associated with sadness recognition and narcissism was negatively associated with disgust recognition 2.Regarding gaze pattern, CU traits were negatively associated with length or first fixation to the mouth of angry faces, number of fixations to the eyes of sad faces and length of first fixation to the eyes of disgusted faces 3. Impulsivity (INS) was positively associated with the number of fixations and average length of each fixation to the eyes of angry faces and number of fixations to the eyes of fearful faces 4. Narcissism (GM) was negatively associated with the number of fixations to the eyes of angry faces, and positively associated with the number of fixations to the mouth of angry faces |
Martin-Key et al. (2018) | n = 101 (49) CD = 50 (24) *34% comorbid ADHD CG = 51 (25) | Forensic Community | 13–18 MCDmale = 15.94) MCDfemale = 16.21) MCGmale = 16.22) MCGfemale = 16.40) | ICU Total score (SR) | Facial static and dynamic | Anger Sad Fear Happy Surprise Disgust Neutral | Accuracy | Eyes Mouth | Initial eye preference Total eye preference | Controlling for subject, comorbidity and age: 1. Higher levels of CU traits were associated with poorer fear recognition across the whole sample 2. Within the CD group, those high on CU traits showed better fear recognition, and reduced attention to the eyes for surprise faces 3. CU and emotional intensity interacted to predict initial eye preference for surprise, which increased with emotional intensity and was larger for high CU participants 4.With exception of disgust, females showed greater total eye preferences than male for all emotions |
Martin-Key et al. (2021) | n = 96 (48) CD = 45 (23) *20% comorbid ADHD CG = 51 (25) | Forensic Community | 13–18 MCDmale = 15.80) MCDfemale = 16.36) MCGmale = 16.22) MCGfemale = 16.40) | ICU Total score (SR) | Body postures (dynamic and static) | Anger Fear Neutral | Accuracy | Arms | Arm preference score (% of time fixation) | Controlling for subject, age and psychiatric comorbidity: 1. There were no effects of CU traits on body posture recognition 2. The effects of CU traits varied according to CD status and sex, with CD males with lower levels of CU traits showing the most atypical fixation behavior More specifically: 3. Higher levels of CU traits predicted higher arm preference score across the entire sample, yet, CU traits were negatively associated with arm preference scores in females, but positively associated in males 4. For fearful and neutral body postures, CU traits were related with arm preference scores in the CD group. These association were negative for females, but positive for males (in the total sample) 5. This atypical fixation behavior did not explain the body posture recognition deficits observed in CD |
Muñoz et al. (2021) | n = 73 (12) *52% ADHD | Clinical | 11–16 (14.0) | ICU Total score and subscales APSD I/CP score (SR) | Facial | Fear Anger Happy Neutral | Accuracy | Eyes Mouth | Reflexive attentional orienting (number, direction and velocity of saccades) | 1.Children high on CU traits did not show a significant deficit in reflexive gaze to the eye region of fearful faces 2. Similar non-significant effects were observed for the other emotions 3. Children high on CU traits performed more poorly in labelling fearful faces accurately, only when the mouth region and not the eye region of the face was primed. Yet, there were no significant differences regarding the association between ICU scores and fear recognition across the mouth-fixation and the eye-fixation condition 4. Youths high on I/CP who are also high on CU traits shifted their gaze less toward fearful eyes when initially focused on the mouth. Only the Callousness facet was significantly associated with decreased gaze shift |