Schools and classes are considered socialization contexts for adolescents’ views about others, but class-level analyses on the role of contextual school-related factors are very limited (Mitchell,
2019), and studies in the Italian context are almost missing. Notably, most studies addressed adolescent’s prejudice toward immigrants in terms of unidimensional attitudinal measures (Eckstein et al.,
2021; Van Zalk & Kerr,
2014). This study aimed to tackle these gaps by applying a neglected model in the literature on adolescents’ prejudice (i.e., the stereotype content model) to highlight the specific contents of adolescents’ prejudice toward immigrants in terms of warmth and competence, the two fundamental dimensions of social judgment (Abele & Wojciszke,
2014; Fiske et al.,
2002). This study employed a large and representative sample of Italian adolescents. By focusing on the effects of classroom-related factors over and above what can be explained by individual features, it underlined how the contents of adolescents’ prejudice toward immigrants were related to different types of contextual antecedents, disentangling the role of background given factors and of malleable school-related factors. Overall, the evidence of this study suggests that – by combining an educational and social-psychological analysis of class-related processes – the knowledge of antecedents of adolescents’ prejudice toward immigrants can be advanced.
The Contents of Adolescents’ Prejudice toward Immigrants Depending on School-Related Contextual Factors
Compared to the limited available evidence, the study provided a more thorough analysis of factors that can affect native adolescents’ prejudice toward immigrants and lead to more or less desirable class-level evaluations of such outgroup in terms of perceived warmth and competence. Background less malleable factors related to the composition of classes did not directly affect perceived warmth or competence (Hypothesis 1a; Hypothesis 1b; Hypothesis 2). Immigrant density was negatively associated with the perceived competition of immigrants. In line with the intergroup contact theory (Pettigrew & Tropp,
2006), the perceived competitiveness of immigrants was lower in classes with high immigrant density. In this vein, findings seem to provide more support for the beneficial role of contact with immigrants (Tropp & Pettigrew,
2005) rather than the complementary theorization stressing that the higher the presence of immigrants, the higher the natives’ perception of immigrants’ threat and competition for resources (Esses et al.,
2001; Scheepers et al.,
2002).
Besides this, on the one side, higher ethnic diversity in the classroom also emerged as a facilitator of class-level achievement in civic knowledge, pairing evidence that more socio-economically diverse classes develop higher civic knowledge (Collado et al.,
2015). On the other side, it worked as an obstacle to the classroom perceived and experienced democratic functioning (i.e., classroom open to discussion climate). This finding adds to available evidence mainly considering open to discussion as a moderator of the effects of classroom diversity on youth prejudice (e.g., Miklikowska et al.,
2021) by showing that also immigrant density might limit school-related outcomes.
As for classroom SES, classes with low SES perceived immigrants as more competitive than classes with high SES. In turn, and in line with expectations (Hypothesis 7), competitiveness mediated the effect of classroom SES on judgments of immigrants’ warmth and competence: if classes with higher SES also perceived lower immigrants’ competitiveness, leading to better classroom evaluation of immigrants in terms of warmth and competence, low classroom SES was associated with more downward attribution
– at the class level
– of warmth and competence to immigrants through higher perceived competitiveness. Such evidence can be conceived as coherent with theorizations stressing that natives can perceive immigrants as a threat to the ingroup’s welfare (e.g., Scheepers et al.,
2002; Stephan & Stephan,
2000), representing
resource stress (i.e., the perception that within a society, access to desired resources is limited) that leads to perceived group competition for resources (Esses et al.,
2001). In this vein, native adolescents in classes with low SES perceive higher stress regarding access to resources due to immigrants, a very salient outgroup in one’s social environment (Bergamaschi,
2013). This, in turn, leads to attributing lesser warmth and competence to immigrants. Also a direct association between classroom SES and more malleable contextual features such as classroom civic knowledge and educational achievement emerged. The attainment gap depending on low SES is confirmed by the findings that classes with high SES also had better educational achievement (for a review, see Sirin,
2005) and civic knowledge attainments (Collado et al.,
2015).
Overall, and most importantly, the evidence mentioned above widens the understanding of the role that such background, less malleable factors related to classroom compositions have on a specific antecedent of prejudice toward immigrants: their perceived competitiveness. Low immigrant density and low classroom SES emerged as factors that can promote a competitive view of immigrants at the class level. This is a crucial point since warmth traits are also shown to have a primacy in person/others’ perception (Cuddy et al.,
2009) because, from an evolutionary account, there are potentially greater costs for dealing with someone who is not warm versus not competent (Cuddy et al.,
2009; Wojciszke,
2005). This, in turn, might be predictive of the potential behavioral correlates of prejudice, as theorized in the stereotype content model, in terms of active behaviors, both harmful and facilitative, that can target immigrants (Cuddy et al.,
2009).
By adding to the minimal evidence on the stereotype content model in the domain of adolescents’ prejudice, this study points to the critical role of experiencing a classroom democratic climate in shaping class-level perceptions of immigrants’ warmth and competence. In particular, even if the predicted direct effect of open to discussion classroom climate on immigrants’ warmth and competence was not found, evidence revealed an indirect effect at the class level through the mediating role of perceived competition and status (Hypothesis 7). Compared to classes with a high open to discussion climate, low classroom open to discussion enhanced the perceived competitiveness of immigrants – leading to lower class-level ratings in terms of warmth and competence – and led to the attribution of lower social status – leading to lower perceived warmth of immigrants. Interestingly – even if it is not in line with the assumptions of the stereotype content model – attributions of status to immigrants were not associated with class-level judgments of immigrants’ competence.
These findings suggest that actual experiences in contexts of democratic and tolerant interactions can be transferred to the views that adolescents develop regarding immigrants (Bandura,
1977; Eckstein et al.,
2021). Those who experience less democratic and participative classroom environments are exposed to risk of perceiving higher competitiveness by immigrants and attributing them lower social status, thus perceiving immigrants as lower in warmth and competence. This corresponds to an increased tendency toward a contemptuous-like pattern of prejudice at the class level. Those who experience democratic values and interactions on their own skin are more prone to attribute higher status and low competitiveness to immigrant attributing them higher warmth and higher competence, and displaying a tendency toward an increase in the admiration-like pattern of prejudice (i.e., the most desirable one) that is reserved for the ingroup or admired groups.
The unexpected finding (related to Hypothesis 6) that classroom perceived status of immigrants was not associated with their perceived competence, but with their warmth should not be conceived as a limitation to replication of the stereotype content model (Fiske et al.,
2002) given that this was the first time that immigrants’ perception was treated at the group level of shared representations, a very specific context. The analytical approach of the study highlighted the group processes that might act at the class level by controlling for individual perceptions. Thus, it can be argued that in contexts/classes where the perception of immigrants’ competitiveness was high, the acknowledgment of their competence was driven by competition rather than status, leading to lower judgments in terms of warmth and competence than in classes with low perceived competition. Instead, when adolescents were exposed to a class-level perception of immigrants as having high status, the perception of their warmth was also driven by their perceived status.
Also, classroom achievement emerged as a factor affecting adolescents’ prejudice toward immigrants. In particular, classes with high achievements attributed higher competence to immigrants (Hypothesis 4), but not warmth. Educational achievement indirectly affected immigrants’ perceived warmth and competence through the enhanced perception of immigrants’ competitiveness. These findings can be interpreted with the lens of the social identity approach of intragroup and intergroup relations (Ellemers et al.,
2002; Sani & Bennett,
2011). In classes with high mean achievements individuals might have difficulties obtaining a positive self-image if they rely on interpersonal, intragroup comparisons within the class, given that the average of students has high academic performances. Consequently, they might enhance the salience of intergroup comparisons and set ingroup-favorable intergroup differentiation by perceiving the outgroup as more competitive and, in turn, less warm. This result can be better understood considering the so-called big-fish-little-pond phenomenon. Evidence of this effect stresses that individual self-concept is weaker in classes with high average achievement (Marsh et al.,
2012). Thus, in such classes, intergroup comparisons might become more salient in order to restore self-esteem through ingroup’s evaluation (Brown,
2011; Ellemers et al.,
2002).
Finally, classroom civic knowledge seemed to be unrelated to stereotypical classroom perceptions of immigrants in terms of warmth and competence. That is, students with equal levels of civic knowledge tended to have the same prejudices toward immigrants even if they were in classes with very different average levels of civic knowledge. This suggests that formal knowledge about society’s functioning does not relate to prejudices, which are social cognitive products of social interaction (Ellemers et al.,
2002) that mere cognition or knowledge seems not strong enough to challenge, at least at the class level. This result matches those observed in a previous study in which civic knowledge alone was insufficient to promote adolescents’ civic engagement (Manganelli et al.,
2014).
Limitations and Future Directions
This study consisted of a large-scale survey conducted with native Italian adolescents. In this vein, it did not consider local differences in immigrants’ presence outside school (besides interethnic classroom composition) and the different facets of contact with immigrants (e.g., direct or extended; positive or negative; Tropp & Pettigrew,
2005). Even though the study’s evidence stressed a positive association between high immigrant classroom density and low perceived immigrants’ competition, future contributions should tackle more closely the quality (either positive or negative) of contact to clarify controversial evidence of contact effects in schools (e.g., Vervoort et al.,
2011).
Moreover, in order to get a clear picture of how school-related contextual factors affect adolescents’ prejudice toward a salient outgroup, the study focused only on the majority group of natives; thus, it could not describe the antecedents of minority adolescents’ prejudice toward the majority outgroup (Tropp & Pettigrew,
2005). Besides this, the study endorsed a social identity approach to explain how class-related phenomena/features affect prejudice, relying on the assumption that classes are social groups and are subjected to the same processes explaining interpersonal and intergroup relations (Ellemers et al.,
2002). Nonetheless, future studies are needed to directly tackle the motivational bases (e.g., the need to differentiate the ingroup and the outgroup positively) of prejudice toward immigrants displayed by adolescents in classes with high mean achievements.
A further step toward thoroughly understanding the processes leading adolescents to show specific patterns of prejudice toward the outgroup of immigrants could be considering the adolescents’ evaluation of their ingroup (i.e., natives) since it has been consistently shown that compensation effects appear: if a group is attributed one of the two fundamental dimensions of social judgment, the other group involved in the social comparison process would be attributed mainly the other dimension (Yzerbyt et al.,
2008). Also, the expected behavioral correlates of the patterns of prejudice could be directly examined (Cuddy et al.,
2009).
If the study’s main finding is that malleable school-related classroom features can affect the shared representations that classes hold about immigrants – which is a finding that has field relevance – implementation strategies to achieve this goal have to be designed. A more thorough analysis of why civic knowledge did not affect the class-level contents of prejudice in terms of groups’ warmth and competence is needed. Future studies should also test the effectiveness of interventions aimed at changing the classroom democratic climate and achievements to change adolescents’ prejudice and promote their harmonious intergroup relations with minority outgroups later in life.