Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development 6/2023

Open Access 26-05-2022 | Original Article

Towards Preventative Psychiatry: Concurrent and Longitudinal Predictors of Postnatal Maternal-Infant Bonding

Auteurs: Frances L. Doyle, Sophie J. Dickson, Valsamma Eapen, Paul J. Frick, Eva R. Kimonis, David J. Hawes, Caroline Moul, Jenny L. Richmond, Divya Mehta, Mark R. Dadds

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 6/2023

Abstract

Maternal-infant bonding is important for children’s positive development. Poor maternal-infant bonding is a risk factor for negative mother and infant outcomes. Although researchers have examined individual predictors of maternal-infant bonding, studies typically do not examine several concurrent and longitudinal predictors within the same model. This study aimed to evaluate the unique and combined predictive power of cross-sectional and longitudinal predictors of maternal-infant bonding. Participants were 372 pregnant women recruited from an Australian hospital. Data were collected from mothers at antenatal appointments (T0), following their child’s birth (T1), and at a laboratory assessment when their child was 5-11-months-old (T2). Poorer bonding at T2 was predicted at T0 by younger maternal age, higher education, and higher antenatal depressive symptoms. Poorer bonding at T2 was predicted at T1 by younger maternal age, higher education, and higher postnatal depressive symptoms. Poorer bonding at T2 was predicted at T2 by younger maternal age, higher education, higher postnatal depression symptoms, higher concurrent perceived social support, and more difficult infant temperament, when controlling for child age at T2. To promote positive maternal-infant bonding, global and targeted interventions in the perinatal period may benefit from targeting maternal psychopathology, perceived lack of social support, and coping with difficult infant temperament.
Opmerkingen

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
A strong mother-infant bond is considered crucial for promoting positive parenting behaviours and optimal child psychosocial development [1, 2]. Maternal-infant bonding (MIB) refers to a mother’s perceived emotional connectedness to her infant [2, 3]. MIB often begins to develop antenatally, grows significantly through the first year of life, and continues to develop throughout the lifespan [46]. Impaired bonding, however, manifests in a range of behaviours, including irritability and hostility toward the infant, absence of positive feelings, as well as dismissal of the infant [1]. Impaired MIB is a risk factor for a range of negative infant and maternal outcomes; and, at the extremes, may manifest as severe clinical syndromes – or disorders – where there is emotional rejection of the infant and/or pathological anger toward the infant [1, 79]. It has been estimated that these severe clinical syndromes occur in about 1% of births in the general population [9], yet causes are still not well-known [7]. Since even sub-clinical levels of emotional rejection of the infant may be related to negative infant and maternal outcomes, warranting intervention or prevention efforts, it may be useful to understand what factors predict poorer MIB in the general population, particularly when these factors are considered in the same statistical model; that is, to determine which variables are uniquely predictive above and beyond other known variables in the model. Further, few studies provide a longitudinal analysis and thus little is known about when variables of interest may be more strongly related to poor MIB.
The detrimental effects of postnatal depression on poor MIB have been well documented cross-sectionally [5, 10], and longitudinally [1113]. Some mothers experiencing postnatal depression may not be sensitive to infant cues, or may be withdrawn, intrusive, apathetic or angry, which can sometimes result in infant rejection [14, 15]. While postnatal depression is understood to be a major contributor to poor bonding, not all mothers with postnatal depression experience impaired bonding nor do all mothers with impaired MIB have postnatal depression. Additionally, many other predictors of poor MIB appear to be interconnected with postnatal depression, such that they may influence, or be influenced by postnatal depression. Thus, it is yet to be discerned whether other predictors play a role above and beyond their interrelationship with postnatal depression.
Researchers examining poor MIB have tended to pay greater attention to postnatal depression than postnatal anxiety. To our knowledge, ten studies have investigated maternal anxiety and poor MIB in the early postpartum period [6, 1624]. Nine of these studies reported that maternal anxiety was associated with poorer bonding [1624], but five of these reported associations did not hold their significance after controlling for postnatal depression [17, 19, 2224]. One study reported that maternal anxiety was associated with better bonding scores [6]. Thus, the evidence for a relationship between maternal anxiety and poor MIB is sparse and mixed. The current study will be able to shed light on the relationship between postnatal anxiety and poor MIB, when controlling for postnatal depression.
Three further factors that may be related to poor MIB are antenatal attitudes towards the pregnancy, social support, and maternal history of maltreatment. Antenatal attitudes towards the pregnancy have been linked to poor MIB across clinical observations of severe clinical emotional rejection of the infant, yet have not been examined in cohort studies [7, 8]. Brockington [7] suggested that three groups should be assessed – namely: planned pregnancies, unplanned but accepted pregnancies, and unwanted pregnancies – with greater risk for poor MIB likely occurring in those with unwanted pregnancies. Additionally, poor satisfaction with social support systems during pregnancy has been associated with lower quality MIB [25, 26]. Further, maternal history of maltreatment has been associated with impaired MIB via maladaptive parenting practices and psychopathology in adulthood [11, 27].

Birth and Breastfeeding Experiences

Dissatisfaction with childbirth has been purported to disrupt some mothers’ abilities to engage in responsive and sensitive caregiving behaviours that are needed to facilitate positive MIB [28]. Although dissatisfaction with the childbirth experience (particularly due to mother-infant physical separations) was initially thought to be central to bonding impairments [2, 29], findings from past studies have been mixed and further research is therefore warranted [3, 28].
The typical preterm infant also has characteristics that are known to make bonding difficult, including responsiveness problems, irritability, and being harder to soothe [30, 31]. These infant characteristics have been associated with reduced maternal sensitivity, emotional detachment, and an elevated risk for child abuse and neglect [32]. Yet some studies have reported no differences in feelings of bonding, and even increased maternal investment in mothers of preterm infants compared to mothers of full-term infants [33, 34]. As antenatal depression has been identified as a risk factor for preterm birth [35], it is possible that the mixed findings reflect variations in maternal mental health, as none of the aforementioned studies controlled for maternal depression antenatally or postnatally.
While the positive effects of breastfeeding on poor MIB are often promoted by public health organisations and are often assumed in the literature, findings in this area are inconsistent. Some studies have found no relationship between breastfeeding status and bonding quality [3638]; whereas others have found that breastfeeding mothers show more affectionate responses during breastfeeding [39], lower maternal negative affect [40], and an overall higher MIB than non-breastfeeding mothers [4143]. It is evident, however, that not all studies control for depression symptoms. Depression has been shown to have a bidirectional effect on breastfeeding status (e.g., [44, 45]); with depressed mothers more likely to stop breastfeeding earlier than non-depressed mothers, and earlier cessation of breastfeeding has also been associated with depression. Thus, further investigation of the association between breastfeeding status and poor MIB is warranted, particularly controlling for depression symptoms. In line with previous research (e.g., [38]), breastfeeding status in this study will be a dichotomous variable that indicates whether or not the mother was breastfeeding at the time of the assessment.

Child Temperament

Temperament is defined as biologically based individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation (Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003; Rothbart, 1981; Rothbart et al., 2001). In this study, the term “difficult infant temperament” is used to describe infant temperaments that are characterized by high negative affect (Rothbart & Gartstein, 2000). Difficult infant temperament is known to impact bonding-related factors like negative maternal responses, reduced maternal responsiveness, parenting stress, and mutual rejection between the mother and infant [16, 4648] but has not often been studied in relation to MIB itself. While difficult infant temperament has been identified as a predictor of postnatal depression [49], it is also understood that depressed mothers are more likely to perceive bonding to their infant more negatively [50]. Nolvi and colleagues [16] found significant unique relationships between high mother-reported infant negative emotionality and low mother-reported infant positive emotionality with poor MIB, over and above maternal depression and anxiety. These findings highlight the importance of considering child and mother characteristics together within the same model.

Maternal Characteristics

Maternal characteristics, such as age and education, have been linked to poor MIB. While some studies find that younger women report higher quality bonding with their infants than older women [4, 38], other studies find no association between bonding and age [6, 51, 52]. In regard to education levels, findings are mixed. Some studies have shown that MIB does not differ by maternal education [6, 13, 21]; whereas, others have shown that women with lower levels of education have reported better bonding with their infant compared to women with higher levels of education [4, 5, 38]. Studies that link education with poor MIB have purported that they believe this to be due to those with more education being more inclined to answer more honestly about their relationship with their infant [4, 5, 38].

The Current Study

The aim of the current study was to examine whether postnatal depression, postnatal anxiety, antenatal attitudes towards the pregnancy, perceived social support, maternal history of maltreatment, dissatisfaction with childbirth, breastfeeding, difficult infant temperament, gestation, maternal age and maternal education were predictors of poor MIB in a general population sample. To date, no prior studies have empirically examined how these factors operate in relationship to poor MIB when considered together and, as noted above, this is particularly important in relation to maternal depression. Thus, this is the first study to prospectively investigate a comprehensive range of factors in relation to poor MIB. Based on previous research, it was hypothesised that all factors would be significantly bivariately correlated with poor MIB. Given previous research, it was expected that maternal postnatal depression would be the strongest predictor for poor MIB. While some previous studies control for maternal postnatal depression, not all do. It was therefore hypothesised that several of these variables would no longer correlate with poor MIB when examined in a model with other predictors, particularly postnatal depression.

Participants, Ethics, and Methods

Participants

The current paper reports on data from the ‘Watch Me Grow for REAL’ study [53], which follows 788 mother-child dyads prospectively from mother’s antenatal appointments until children turn 3-years-old. Mothers were recruited from the postnatal ward of an Australian hospital between October 2017 and December 2018. The inclusion criteria for the broader ‘Watch Me Grow for REAL’ study were having a child born at Liverpool hospital in Sydney, Australia during the recruitment period, maternal intention to remain in Sydney for four years, and maternal functional English (or a family member willing, and able, to translate throughout the project). The exclusion criteria for the broader ‘Watch Me Grow for REAL’ study was if the child was known to have congenital conditions at birth. Mothers were also excluded from the study if nursing staff reported to the research team that the mother will not be taking her child home from the hospital (e.g., infant death, removal of the child from maternal care at the hospital by child services).
Participants in this study were a sub-sample of mothers (n = 372) who completed the Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire (PBQ) at T2 [54, 55]. When both twins were recorded in the sample, we excluded the youngest twin (n = 11) to ensure statistical independence between mother reports. Mothers’ age at birth ranged from 18.28 to 44.54 years (M = 31.19, SD = 5.06). Mothers in the sample gave birth to 200 boys (53.8%) and 172 girls (46.2%). Mothers were of Caucasian (n = 109, 29.3%), Middle-Eastern (n = 77, 20.7%), South Asian (n = 62, 16.7%), South-East Asian (n = 57, 15.3%), African/African American (n = 17, 4.6%), Polynesian/Melanesian (n = 18, 4.8%), Hispanic/Latino (n = 14, 3.8%), East Asian (n = 12, 3.2%), and Aboriginal/Torres Strait Islander (n = 4, 1.1%) descent, and two mothers had missing ethnicity data (0.5%). At the child’s birth, 294 (79.0%) mothers were married, 66 (17.7%) were in a relationship, 9 (2.4%) were single, and three mothers had missing relationship status data (0.8%). Mothers were reimbursed for their participation in the T2 assessment with a $40 gift voucher.
To assess for selective attrition bias, comparisons were conducted between this sub-sample and the 396 mothers who did not complete the PBQ (i.e. did not attend the T2 assessment, or who attended T2 but did not have time to complete this questionnaire). Mothers of twins were counted once across measures. There were no significant differences on child gender, child gestation, maternal experience of child abuse, antenatal depression symptomology, childbirth satisfaction, or breastfeeding status at birth (all ps > 0.05). However, mothers who completed the PBQ were significantly older (M PBQ = 31.19 years vs. M No PBQ = 29.84 years, F(1, 767) = 12.00, p = .001), more educated (χ2 (df = 5, N = 757) = 36.14, p < .001), perceived less social support antenatally (χ2 (df = 1, N = 757) = 6.02, p = .014) and at birth (χ2 (df = 1, N = 762) = 6.04, p = .014), and more depressed following their child’s birth (M PBQ = 4.24 vs. M No PBQ = 3.54, F(1, 650) = 6.37, p = .012) than those who did not complete the PBQ at T2.

Ethics

This study involved human participants. It was reviewed and approved by The University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee (Project Number 2017/644), and the South Western Sydney Local Health District Human Research Ethics Committee (Local Project No HE17/115). Written informed consent was obtained from mothers for their participation and mothers provided written informed consent for the participation of their child.

Measures and Procedure

Data from antenatal records (when women were approximately 20 weeks gestation; T0), birth records and self-reports (T1), and a laboratory assessment when children were aged between 5- and 11-months (T2) are reported in the present study.
Antenatal data. At the time of recruitment, mothers gave consent for the researchers to access data from an antenatal interview [56] conducted with a midwife at approximately 20 weeks gestation. Variables extracted from this interview include maternal experience of child abuse (none reported, child abuse reported), lifetime history of anxiety and/or depression (no, yes), whether mothers’ perceived they will have good social support following birth (perceived not enough support following birth, perceived they would have good social support following birth). Antenatal attitudes towards pregnancy were captured using four categories (i.e. planned and happy; planned and unhappy; unplanned and happy; unplanned and unhappy). As no mothers endorsed the category ‘planned and unhappy’ and this category was not indicated in previous theoretical papers [7], only three categories were carried forward (i.e. planned and happy, unplanned and happy, unplanned and unhappy).
Mothers responded on the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression scale [EPDS; 57, 58]. The EPDS is a ten-item scale that has been validated in numerous trials and languages for the detection of postnatal depression (e.g., [59, 60]). The total score from the EPDS indicated mothers’ depression symptomatology during the past week, with higher scores indicating greater antenatal depressive symptoms and a score of 13 or more indicating antenatal depressive symptoms above the clinical threshold.
Demographic items were also self-reported on our Newborn Baseline Questionnaire (NBQ; [61]) at the time of recruitment (i.e. usually within one week of the child’s birth). These variables included mothers’ ethnicity, highest level of education completed (primary school, Year 10 or equivalent, Year 12 or equivalent, TAFE qualification or trade apprenticeship, undergraduate university degree, postgraduate university degree), and number of years living in Australia (0–5 years, 6–10 years, 11–15 years, 16–20 years, 21 + years).
Birth-related data. Routinely collected birth data (recorded by midwives and doctors) and postnatal Edinburgh Postnatal Depression scale [EPDS; 57, 58] scores (recorded by Community Health Nurses within the month following birth when conducting home visits) were extracted from mothers’ electronic medical records. Birth-related data included mother’s age at the birth of her child, birth type (vaginal unassisted birth, vaginal assisted birth, elective Caesarean section, emergency Caesarean section), and gestational age. Mothers also self-reported the following constructs on the NBQ: satisfaction with childbirth (see details below), current breastfeeding status following birth (not breastfeeding, currently breastfeeding), perceived social support (perceived not enough support, perceived good support), and overall relationship satisfaction (see details below).
Mothers responded to the 7-item Satisfaction With Childbirth scale [SWCh; 62] on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1, strongly disagree, to 7, strongly agree. Scores on the SWCh reflect mothers’ satisfaction with the childbirth experience, with higher scores indicating greater satisfaction. The current study demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83) in line with previous studies [62, 63].
Mothers were asked to respond to the Overall Relationship Satisfaction item from the Dyadic Adjustment scale [64]. Mothers reported, on a 7-point scale, ranging from 0, extremely unhappy, to 6, perfect, how happy they were with their relationship with the baby’s biological father.
T2 data from the laboratory assessment. Mothers self-reported the following constructs at a laboratory visit when their child was aged between 5- and 11-months-old: perceived social support perceived social support (perceived not enough support, perceived good support), current breastfeeding status (not breastfeeding, currently breastfeeding), and overall relationship satisfaction with the baby’s biological father.
Mothers also reported on their own mental health during the past week using the 21-item Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS21) [65]. The DASS21 has been validated in both research and clinical settings [66, 67]. Mothers rated on a four-point scale, ranging from 0, did not apply to me at all, to 3, applied to me very much, or most of the time. Higher scores indicate more frequent symptomology. In this study, only the depression subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.79) and the anxiety subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.71) are reported.
Further, mothers reported on their child’s temperament using the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ) Revised - Very Short Form [68]. Mothers reported, on a 7-point scale, ranging from 1, never, to 7, always, the frequency with which their child enacted specific behaviours over the past 7 days. Mothers were also given the option to select “does not apply”, and if selected, that item is excluded from the scale score. The validity of its’ predecessors – IBQ and IBQ Short form – has been extensively documented, with research demonstrating their convergent validity with observational measures [69]. In this study, only the negative affect subscale is examined with higher scores indicating greater difficult infant temperament. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.78 in the current study, which was consistent with earlier use of ‘Very short’ form of the IBQ- R (e.g., [70]).
Maternal-infant bonding. At T2, mothers completed the 23-items from the PBQ [54, 55]. Numerous studies have found the PBQ to be both a valid and reliable tool for identifying women at risk of impaired mother-infant bonding [5, 55, 71]. The questionnaire contains items addressing the mother’s feelings towards her infant such as “My baby irritates me”. The response categories range from 0, never, to 5, always on a six-point scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87), with higher scores indicating poorer MIB.

Analytic Plan

Bivariate correlations were examined. Three regression models were run with poor MIB as the outcome variable. First, a regression model was run with the antenatal (T0) variables predicting poor MIB. In Step 1, were the maternal characteristics (i.e. mothers’ age and education). In Step 2, was antenatal depression. In Step 3, were the antenatal mother-related variables (i.e. maternal experiences of child abuse, antenatal perceived social support, and attitudes towards pregnancy).
Second, a regression model was run with the birth-related (T1) variables predicting poor MIB. In Step 1, were the maternal characteristics (i.e. mothers’ age and education). In Step 2, was postnatal depression. In Step 3, were the T1 mother-related variables (i.e. maternal experiences of child abuse, T1 perceived social support, and attitudes towards pregnancy). In Step 4, were the birth and breastfeeding variables (i.e. satisfaction with childbirth, breastfeeding status, and gestation).
Third, a regression model was run with poor MIB as the outcome variable, and all T0, T1, and T2 variables predicting MIB. In Step 1, were the maternal characteristics (i.e. mothers’ age and education). In Step 2, were the depression variables at T0, T1, and T2. In Step 3 were the T1 mother-related variables (i.e. maternal experiences of child abuse, T0-T2 perceived social support, attitudes towards pregnancy, T2 anxiety). In Step 4, were the birth and breastfeeding variables (i.e. satisfaction with childbirth, breastfeeding status, and gestation). In Step 5, were the child characteristics (i.e. difficult infant temperament).

Results

Description of the Sample

Descriptions of the sample from the time of recruitment, the time of birth, and the time of the T2 assessment can be found in Tables 1 and 2, and 3, respectively.
Table 1
Description of the Sample from Antenatal Data
 
n (%)
Maternal Experience of Child Abuse
 
None reported
Child abuse reported
Missing
323 (86.8)
39 (10.5)
10 (2.7)
Lifetime History of Anxiety and/or Depression
 
No
Yes
Missing
276 (74.2)
84 (22.6)
12 (3.2)
Perceived Social Support
 
Not enough support
Good support
Missing
20 (5.4)
348 (93.5)
4 (1.1)
Antenatal Attitudes Towards Pregnancy
 
Planned and happy
Unplanned and happy
Unplanned and unhappy
Missing
236 (63.4)
128 (34.4)
2 (0.5)
6 (1.6%)
Depressive Symptoms
 
Depressive Symptoms Score (n = 357)
Missing
Scored Above Clinical Threshold
M = 5.22, SD = 4.18,
15
23 (6.4)
Education
 
Primary School
Year 10 or Equivalent
Year 12 or Equivalent
TAFE qualification or Trade Apprenticeship
Undergraduate University Degree
Postgraduate university Degree
Missing
5 (1.3)
27 (7.0)
53 (14.2)
83 (22.3)
131 (35.2)
72 (19.4)
2 (0.5)
Number of years Living in Australia
 
0–5 Years
6–10 Years
11–15 Years
16–20 Years
21 + Years
Missing
72 (19.4)
63 (16.9)
30 (8.2)
27 (7.3)
175 (47.0)
5 (1.3)
Table 2
Description of the Sample from Data Collected at Birth
 
n (%)
Depressive Symptoms
 
Depressive Symptoms Score (n = 343)
Scored above Clinical Threshold
M = 4.36, SD = 3.85
10 (2.9)
Birth Type
 
Vaginal Unassisted
Vaginal Assisted
Elective Caesarean Section
Emergency Caesarean Section
Missing
201 (54.0)
45 (12.1)
81 (21.8)
44 (11.8)
1 (0.3)
Gestational Age (n = 369)
M = 38.66 weeks, SD = 2.16, range: 28–42
Satisfaction with Child Birth (n = 361)
M = 36.79, SD = 9.51
Breastfeeding Status
 
Not Currently Breastfeeding
Currently Breastfeeding
34 (9.1)
338 (90.9)
Perceived Social Support
 
Not enough support
Good support
Missing
49 (13.2)
321 (86.3)
2 (0.5)
Relationship Satisfaction (n = 364)
M = 4.67, SD = 1.39

Univariate Associations Between MIB and Predictors

As can be seen in Table 6, poorer maternal-infant bonding was associated with higher maternal education status, antenatal and postnatal EPDS, T2 DASS-Depression and DASS-Anxiety, and T2 infant difficult infant temperament.

Unique and Combined Prediction of MIB from the Putative Predictors

Antenatal predictors. All predictors of MIB were examined using hierarchical stepwise linear regression (see Table 4). Poorer MIB at T2 was predicted by the following antenatal (T0) variables, which explained 8.4% variance: younger maternal age, higher maternal education, and higher antenatal depression symptoms (R 2 = 0.084, adjusted R 2 = 0.069, F(6, 351) = 5.39, p < .001). For each one-point change in maternal age, MIB changed by − 0.18. For each one-point change in mothers’ level of education, MIB changed by 1.32. For each one-point change in antenatal depression symptoms, MIB changed by 0.45.
Table 3
Description of the Sample from Data Collected at Laboratory Visit when the Child was Aged Between 5-11 Months
 
n (%)
Perceived Social Support
 
 Not enough support
 Good support
 Missing
254 (68.3)
118 (31.7)
0 (0.0)
Breastfeeding Status
 
 Not Currently Breastfeeding
 Currently Breastfeeding
181 (48.7)
191 (51.3)
Relationship Satisfaction (n = 371)
M = 4.11, SD = 1.54
Mother’s Mental Health During Past Week
 
 Depression Subscale (n = 372)
 Anxiety Subscale (n = 372)
M = 1.35, SD = 2.24
M = 1.51, SD = 2.29
Difficult Infant Temperament (n = 381)
M = 3.90, SD = 1.07
Maternal-Infant Bonding
M = 6.49, SD = 6.87
Table 4
Stepwise linear regression model predicting maternal-infant bonding at T2 from T0 antenatal predictors (N = 357)
 
Univariate correlation
B (SE B)
β
t
R2 (R2 adj)
ΔR2
Step 1
    
0.034 (0.028)**
0.034**
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
− 0.07
0.14**
− 0.18 (0.09)
1.18 (0.36)
− 0.119*
0.179***
-2.17
3.28
  
Step 2
    
0.082 (0.074)***
0.048***
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Antenatal depression
− 0.07
0.14**
0.21***
-0.19 (0.08)
1.26 (0.35)
0.43 (0.10)
− 0.121*
0.191***
0.220***
-2.28
3.58
4.31
  
Step 3
    
0.084 (0.069)***
0.003
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Antenatal depression
 Maternal experiences of child abuse
 Antenatal perceived social support
 Attitudes towards pregnancy
− 0.07
0.14**
0.21***
0.02
− 0.02
0.03
− 0.18 (0.08)
1.32 (0.36)
0.45 (0.11)
-0.46 (1.49)
1.63 (2.37)
0.63 (0.87)
− 0.117*
0.200***
0.227***
− 0.016
0.036
0.038
-2.17
3.67
4.26
-0.31
0.687
0.72
  
Note. Higher MIB scores indicate poorer bonding *p < .05; **p < .01, ***p < .001
Table 5
Stepwise linear regression model predicting maternal-infant bonding at T2 from T1 birth-related predictors (N = 333)
 
Univariate correlation
B (SE B)
β
t
R2 (R2 adj)
ΔR2
Step 1
    
0.034 (0.028)**
0.034**
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
− 0.07
0.14**
− 0.18 (0.09)
1.18 (0.37)
− 0.119*
0.179**
-2.10
3.28
  
Step 2
    
0.107 (0.099)***
0.073***
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Postnatal depression
− 0.07
0.14**
0.28***
-0.19 (0.08)
1.02 (0.36)
0.64 (0.12)
− 0.124*
0.154**
0.272***
-2.28
2.83
5.20
  
Step 3
    
0.108 (0.092)***
0.002
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Postnatal depression
 Maternal experiences of child abuse
 Postnatal perceived social support
 Attitudes towards pregnancy
− 0.07
0.14**
0.28***
0.02
− 0.06
0.03
-0.19 (0.09)
1.05 (0.37)
0.63 (0.13)
-0.35 (1.52)
-0.42 (1.48)
0.60 (0.89)
− 0.124*
0.160**
0.269***
− 0.012
− 0.015
0.036
-2.24
2.86
5.03
-0.23
-0.29
0.67
  
Step 4
    
0.118 (0.094)***
0.010
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Postnatal depression
 Maternal experiences of child abuse
 Postnatal perceived social support
 Attitudes towards pregnancy
 Satisfaction with childbirth
 Breastfeeding status
 Gestation
− 0.07
0.14**
0.28***
0.02
− 0.06
0.03
0.03
− 0.01
0.06
− 0.20 (0.09)
1.07 (0.37)
0.66 (0.13)
-0.01 (1.53)
-0.46 (1.48)
0.49 (0.89)
0.07 (0.05)
-0.83 (1.46)
0.25 (0.22)
− 0.126*
0.163**
0.281***
0.000
− 0.016
0.030
0.076
− 0.030
0.058
-2.27
2.89
5.19
-0.01
0.31
0.56
1.42
-0.57
1.11
  
Note. Higher MIB scores indicate poorer bonding *p < .05; **p < .01, ***p < .001
Table 6
Stepwise linear regression model predicting maternal-infant bonding at T2 from T0, T1 and T2 predictors (N = 329)
 
Univariate correlation
B (SE B)
β
t
R2 (R2 adj)
ΔR2
Step 1
    
0.070 (0.002)
0.005
 Child Age at T2
0.07
0.02 (0.02)
0.070
1.29
  
Step 2
    
0.040 (0.032)**
0.035**
 Child Age at T2
 Maternal Age
0.07
− 0.06
0.02 (0.02)
− 0.18 (0.09)
0.081
− 0.117*
1.52
-2.10
  
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
0.15**
1.22 (0.37)
0.185**
3.31
  
Step 3
    
0.160 (0.145)***
0.120***
 Child Age at T2
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Antenatal depression
 Postnatal depression
 Concurrent depression
0.07
− 0.06
0.15**
0.22***
0.29***
0.30***
0.02 (0.01)
-0.18 (0.08)
1.16 (0.35)
0.16 (0.12)
0.34 (0.14)
0.72 (0.20)
0.06
− 0.115*
0.175**
0.083
0.148*
0.204***
1.24
-2.18
3.31
1.42
2.42
3.59
  
Step 4
    
0.187 (0.156)***
0.027
 Child Age at T2
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Antenatal depression
 Postnatal depression
 Concurrent depression
 Maternal experiences of child abuse
 Antenatal perceived social support
 Postnatal perceived social support
 Concurrent perceived social support
 Attitudes towards pregnancy
 Concurrent anxiety
0.07
− 0.06
0.15**
0.22***
0.29***
0.30***
0.02
− 0.05
− 0.07
− 0.15**
0.06
0.26***
0.02 (0.01)
-0.19 (0.08)
1.24 (0.35)
0.17 (0.12)
0.35 (0.14)
0.48 (0.26)
-1.13 (1.47)
3.52 (2.34)
0.92 (1.45)
-2.50 (0.93)
0.53 (0.85)
0.36 (0.25)
0.056
− 0.123*
0.188**
0.086
0.153*
0.136
− 0.039
0.080
0.034
− 0.139**
0.032
0.102
1.11
-2.32
3.51
1.44
2.50
1.85
-0.77
1.51
0.63
-2.69
0.64
1.44
  
Step 5
    
0.198 (0.158)***
0.011
 Child Age at T2
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Antenatal depression
 Postnatal depression
 Concurrent depression
 Maternal experiences of child abuse
 Antenatal perceived social support
 Postnatal perceived social support
 Concurrent perceived social support
 Attitudes towards pregnancy
 Concurrent anxiety
 Satisfaction with childbirth
 Breastfeeding status at birth
 Breastfeeding status at T2
 Gestation
0.07
− 0.06
0.15**
0.22***
0.29***
0.30***
0.02
− 0.05
− 0.07
− 0.15**
0.06
0.26***
0.03
− 0.03
0.05
0.07
0.01 (0.01)
-0.19 (0.08)
1.26 (0.37)
0.18 (0.12)
0.38 (0.14)
0.44 (0.26)
-0.84 (1.48)
3.52 (2.34)
0.85 (1.46)
-2.66 (0.95)
0.44 (0.85)
0.38 (0.26)
0.05 (0.05)
-0.33 (1.43)
-0.19 (0.92)
0.36 (0.20)
0.053
− 0.125*
0.191**
0.090
0.165**
0.123
− 0.029
0.081
0.031
− 0.148**
0.027
0.107
0.055
− 0.012
− 0.011
0.090
1.04
-2.33
3.46
1.50
2.66
1.65
-0.57
1.50
0.58
-2.81
0.52
1.50
1.07
-0.23
-0.21
1.77
  
Step 6
    
0.234 (0.193)***
0.036***
 Child Age at T2
 Maternal Age
 Mothers’ Highest Level of Education Achieved
 Antenatal depression
 Postnatal depression
 Concurrent depression
 Maternal experiences of child abuse
 Antenatal perceived social support
 Postnatal perceived social support
 Concurrent perceived social support
 Attitudes towards pregnancy
 Concurrent anxiety
 Satisfaction with childbirth
 Breastfeeding status at birth
 Breastfeeding status at T2
 Gestation
 Difficult Infant Temperament
0.07
− 0.06
0.15**
0.22***
0.29***
0.30***
0.02
− 0.05
− 0.07
− 0.15**
0.06
0.26***
0.03
− 0.03
0.05
0.07
0.31***
0.01 (0.01)
-0.17 (0.09)
1.07 (0.36)
0.14 (0.11)
0.30 (0.14)
0.46 (0.26)
-0.57 (1.45)
3.58 (2.29)
0.84 (1.43)
-2.85 (0.93)
0.10 (0.83)
0.29 (0.25)
0.03 (0.46)
-0.44 (1.40)
-0.60 (0.908)
0.33 (0.20)
1.62 (0.42)
0.043
− 0.108*
0.162**
0.071
0.130*
0.130
− 0.020
0.082
0.031
− 0.158**
0.005
0.082
0.037
− 0.016
− 0.035
0.081
0.205***
0.86
-2.05
2.98
1.21
2.12
1.78
-0.39
1.56
0.59
-3.07
0.12
1.16
0.73
-0.32
-0.66
1.63
3.90
  
Note. Higher MIB scores indicate poorer bonding *p < .05; **p < .01, ***p < .001
Birth-related predictors. All predictors of MIB were examined using hierarchical stepwise linear regression (see Table 5). Poorer MIB at T2 was predicted by the following birth-related (T1) variables, which explained 11.8% variance: younger maternal age, higher maternal education, and higher postnatal depression symptoms (R 2 = 0.118, adjusted R 2 = 0.094, F(6, 351) = 5.39, p < .001). For each one-point change in mothers’ level of education, MIB changed by 1.07. For each one-point change in postnatal depression symptoms, MIB changed by 0.66.
All possible predictors. All predictors of MIB were examined, controlling for infant age at T2, using hierarchical stepwise linear regression (see Table 6). Poorer MIB at T2 was predicted by the following variables, which explained 23.4% variance: younger maternal age, higher maternal education, higher postnatal (T1) depression symptoms, lower concurrent perceived social support (T2), and higher concurrent (T2) difficult infant temperament (R 2 = 0.234, adjusted R 2 = 0.193, F(17, 323) = 5.80, p < .001). For each one-point change in maternal age, MIB changed by − 0.17. For each one-point change in mothers’ level of education, MIB changed by 1.07. For each one-point change in T1 postnatal depression symptoms, MIB changed by 0.30. For each one-point change in T2 concurrent perceived social support, MIB changed by -2.85. For each one-point change in difficult infant temperament, MIB changed by 1.62.

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to test the role of depression, anxiety, antenatal attitudes towards the pregnancy, perceived social support, maternal history of maltreatment, dissatisfaction with childbirth, breastfeeding, difficult infant temperament, gestation, maternal age and maternal education as unique predictors of poor MIB. As expected, it was found that poorer MIB was associated with higher maternal education level (T0), higher depression symptoms (T0, T1, and T2), anxiety symptoms (T2), and difficult infant temperament (T2), and lower concurrent perceived social support (T2). Contrary to expectations, MIB was not significantly correlated with maternal age, maternal experience of childhood abuse (T0), perceived social support at T0 and T1, attitudes toward pregnancy (T0), satisfaction with childbirth (T1), breastfeeding status (T1 and T2), or the infants’ gestation (T1).
Three models were run to examine the unique longitudinal and concurrent predictors of poor MIB. Regression analyses revealed that poorer MIB when infants were in their first year of life was predicted antenatally by younger maternal age, higher maternal education and higher antenatal depression symptoms. Results showed that maternal experiences of child abuse, attitudes towards pregnancy, and perceived social support did not significantly predict poor MIB above and beyond maternal age, maternal education and antenatal depression symptoms. These results indicate that when mothers are being screened antenatally for risk factors for poor MIB that those mothers who are younger with higher education and higher antenatal depression symptoms should be the primary targets for further screening, and possibly intervention.
Second, a regression model was run with the birth-related (T1) variables predicting poor MIB. Regression analyses revealed that poorer MIB when infants were in their first year of life was predicted at birth by younger maternal age, higher maternal education and higher postnatal depression symptoms. Results showed that models that included maternal experiences of child abuse, perceived social support at birth, attitudes towards pregnancy, breastfeeding status, and gestation did not significantly predict poor MIB over and above younger maternal age, higher maternal education and higher postnatal depression symptoms. Similar to the antenatal results, the postnatal model indicates that when mothers are being screened following the birth of their child for poor MIB risk factors that younger mothers with higher education and higher postnatal depression symptoms should be the primary targets for further assessment, and possibly intervention.
Third, a regression model was run with poor MIB as the outcome variable, and all T0, T1, and T2 variables predicting poor MIB, controlling for the age of the child at the T2 assessment. Regression analyses revealed that poorer MIB when infants were in their first year of life was predicted by younger maternal age, higher maternal education (T0), higher postnatal depression (T2), lower concurrent perceived social support (T2), and higher difficult infant temperament (T2) when controlling for child age at the T2 assessment. As expected, several variables were not significant predictors of poor MIB during the first year of an infant’s life once other predictors were accounted for; specifically: antenatal and concurrent depression (T0 and T2), maternal experiences of child abuse, perceived social support antenatally and at birth (T0 and T1), attitudes towards pregnancy, concurrent anxiety (T2), satisfaction with childbirth, breastfeeding status (T1 and T2), and infant gestation. The current study extends previous research by considering a range of antenatal (i.e. T0), birth (i.e. T1), and postnatal (i.e. T2) factors together within a sample of socio-demographically diverse families who were recruited as part of a birth-cohort study.
These findings support the notion that perinatal mental health is an important factor in the sequelae of poor MIB. Although concurrent anxiety and concurrent depression symptoms were significantly bivariately correlated with poor MIB, they were not significant predictors in the final multivariate regression models. Antenatally, higher depression symptoms were independently predictive of poorer MIB. Similarly, following birth, postnatal depression symptoms were independently predictive of poorer MIB. Thus, these results indicate that mothers who are showing symptoms of depression antenatally or postnatally are at a higher risk of bonding problems within the first year of their child’s life. Interestingly, antenatal and concurrent depression symptoms were not significant predictors in the model with all predictors; however, postnatal depression symptoms remained a significant predictor. It is possible that this may be due to the stability of depression symptoms in this sample (i.e. depression measures were significantly correlated – albeit, they did not appear to show high collinearity). Overall, our maternal depression and poor MIB findings echo those found by various researchers cross-sectionally [5, 10, 16], and longitudinally [11, 12, 72], and indicate the important role of depression in the sequelae of bonding problems.
This study reiterated the importance of examining child characteristics alongside mother characteristics and experiences when examining poor MIB. This study showed that negative infant temperament influenced poor MIB above and beyond all other predictors in the model. Our findings support those of Nolvi et al. [16] who found a significant unique relationship of mother- and father-reported child temperament with poor MIB, over and above maternal depression or anxiety. It is increasingly acknowledged that the infant is not a passive participant in mother-infant interactions [16], and the bidirectional nature of mother-infant relationships is likely reflected in our findings. In particular, our results show that the mother-infant bond is influenced by what the infant brings to the interaction, particularly in terms of his/her temperament.
Concurrent perceived social support was independently associated with poor MIB above and beyond several other factors, with lower levels statistically predicting more bonding problems. This is interesting because it was not a significant independent predictor antenatally at T0 or postnatally at T1, contradicting previous findings that poor satisfaction with social support systems during pregnancy were associated with lower quality mother-infant bonding [25, 26]. When examining our three perceived social support variables, it appears that the majority of mothers reported that they perceived their support networks to be good antenatally and postnatally (i.e. within a month following the birth of their child). At T2 (when their child was 5- to 11-months old), however, more mothers perceived they were not getting enough support, at which timepoint low social support emerged as a significant predictor of poor MIB. It is possible that this change in ratings may be occurring as some mothers’ anticipated social support does not meet their reality, thus creating more variance within this measure. Alternatively, social support may be reducing in the second half of the first year of life for some mothers, which may be impacting their ability to bond with their infant. For example, fathers may have parental leave to provide support in the postnatal days (i.e. T1) and be back to work in these later months (i.e. T2), leaving mothers with less support. Similarly, mothers may have returned to work, in part or full, by T2 and may be juggling work alongside caregiving demands. Regardless of the exact mechanism, this study shows that low perceived social support is linked to poor MIB independent of postnatal depression. Clinicians and researchers, therefore, need to be mindful of when in the perinatal period they ask mothers about their social support, and that perceptions of social support may change over time.
In relation to demographic factors, we found that maternal age and education level were significant predictors of poor MIB. Previous research has shown that women with lower levels of education reported better bonding with their infant compared to women with higher education levels [4, 5, 38], and these findings were replicated in this study. It is possible that these findings may relate to the amount of time that the mother spends with her infant. For example, it is possible that when compared to less educated mothers, more educated mothers may be more likely to be working and therefore have less time to bond with their infant. Future research may wish to investigate this possibility further. Moreover, some studies have shown that younger women have reported higher quality bonding with their infants than older women [4, 38], whereas other studies have found no association between poor MIB and maternal age [6, 13, 51]. In the present study, however, these findings were not replicated as younger mothers reported more bonding problems than older mothers. It is unclear why age-related findings are so mixed. Researchers have proposed that social desirability relating to MIB may differ according to age [4, 38]. To our knowledge, only one study has included a measure of social desirability in relation to MIB [5], finding that mothers scoring higher on social desirability also reported stronger MIB than mothers who scored lower on social desirability. It is also possible that older mums may be more likely to be employed in workplaces with good parental leave policies (i.e. that allow for longer leave entitlements than the standard leave provided by the Australian government), affording them more opportunity to bond with their infants. Future research could include a measure of social desirability and length of leave alongside age and MIB to further elucidate these relationships.
Although this study has many strengths, it also has a number of noteworthy limitations. First, this study employed a range of self-report measures, including our measure of MIB. Researchers have emphasized that MIB needs to be self-reported, given that it is measuring self-perceptions [3, 38]; however, future research may employ clinician ratings of predictors including mental health symptomology, and observer/father/other caregiver ratings of difficult infant temperament. As individuals with depression have been theorised and shown to have a negative information processing biases [e.g., 73, 74–76], these filters may bias interpretations of infants’ difficult temperament. By employing an independent rater (either observer or fathers/other caregivers) the shared method variance between mother ratings could be reduced, which in turn would reduce the likelihood that mothers are skewing ratings of infant temperament. Second, T2 measures (including DASS-Depression and Anxiety, T2 perceived social support, and difficult infant temperament) were collected at the same time as the PBQ, so that temporal ordering and causality cannot be determined. Third, in this study, we were unable to examine anxiety separately until the T2 assessment because the antenatal and postnatal data used in this study were routinely collected in hospitals during antenatal midwife visits and postnatal nursing visits. Thus, we were unable to examine any additional predictors of poor MIB, such as antenatal anxiety. It is therefore possible that anxiety could play an earlier role in the development of MIB, but we were unable to examine this in the present study. Fourth, it would have been useful to more broadly investigate difficulties with infants’ sleep and/or feeding (not just whether the mother was breastfeeding at the time of the assessment), as these tend to be major issues present at this age (e.g., [77]). Further, infant sleeping and feeding problems are linked with maternal psychopathology and perceptions of infant temperament [78]; yet have not been extensively investigated with poor MIB. Due to testing load limitations, further information on sleeping and feeding could not be ascertained. Fifth, there was a possible attrition bias from those initially recruited for the larger ‘Watch Me Grow for REAL’ study. Our sample was likely to be older, more educated, to have perceived lower social support, and experienced more postnatal depression symptoms following the birth of their child than the general population. Thus, findings should be interpreted with these limitations in mind.
This study also had several strengths and there are a range of clinical practice implications. A key strength is that this study includes prospective longitudinal data from an ethnically diverse birth-cohort sample. Further, another notable strength is that we were able to consider many variables concurrently within the same statistical model unlike the majority of the MIB literature. To our knowledge, this is also the first birth cohort study to report attitudes towards pregnancy in relation to MIB. It must be noted that there were a small number of mothers who reported that they had an unplanned pregnancy that they were unhappy about. It was shown that attitudes towards pregnancy were not significantly correlated with MIB in this sample. It is possible that this factor may be more pertinent in clinical cohorts that have been referred to specialist services than it is in general population cohorts. In regard to clinical practice implications, practitioners may be able to better support mothers in developing healthy MIB by addressing modifiable factors, such as maternal depression and social support. Two approaches to intervention may be useful to consider: first, the importance of positive MIB to healthy child development and the factors that can impede this relationship could be communicated within universal interventions (such as antenatal classes and postnatal parenting groups); second, targeted interventions for maternal depression in the perinatal period could also address poor MIB. Interventions could include parenting strategies to promote the maternal-infant bond (e.g., increasing maternal sensitivity, and reducing intrusiveness), and teach cognitive strategies to assist mothers to realistically appraise their bonding. Mothers’ cognitions are a reasonable intervention target as they have been shown to influence mothers’ interactions with their infants [11]. Further, interventions addressing poor MIB may also address how to respond to infants with difficult temperaments. Midwives, nurses, obstetricians, and psychologists may be well-placed to have these conversations and to help facilitate the environment to be able to promote the development of bonding between mothers and their infants.

Summary

Maternal-infant bonding is important for children’s positive development. Poor maternal-infant bonding is a risk factor for negative mother and infant outcomes. Although researchers have examined individual predictors of maternal-infant bonding, studies typically do not examine several concurrent and longitudinal predictors within the same model. This study aimed to evaluate the unique and combined predictive power of cross-sectional and longitudinal predictors of maternal-infant bonding. Participants were 372 pregnant women recruited from an Australian hospital. Data were collected at antenatal appointments (T0), following their child’s birth (T1), and at a laboratory assessment when the child was 5-11-months-old (T2). Overall, this study aligns with previous research on maternal bonding by demonstrating the strong link between poor MIB and maternal depression symptomology. Uniquely, our findings have uncovered that higher maternal education, lower perceived social support (when infants were, on average, 7-months-old), and higher difficult infant temperament also independently contribute to poor MIB within the infant’s first year of life. Moreover, although prior research has linked maternal experiences of child abuse, satisfaction with childbirth, infant gestation, and breastfeeding status to poor MIB, our findings suggest that these factors do not uniquely predict poor MIB during the critical first year of an infant’s life when other factors are considered. To promote positive MIB, global and targeted interventions in the perinatal period would be beneficial, particularly in relation to maternal psychopathology, social support, and difficult infant temperament.

Acknowledgements

This study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees of the South Western Sydney Local Health District and The University of Sydney. We would like to express our gratitude to Dr. Antonio Mendoza Diaz, our ‘Watch Me Grow for REAL’ research assistants (including Charlotte Burman, Annie Chapman, Isabel Lopez, Bronte G. Morgan, Scott O’Loughlin, and Sinia Sareen), our research and clinical students, our ‘Watch Me Grow for REAL’ volunteers, the team at the Ingham Institute, as well as the antenatal and postnatal staff at Liverpool Hospital in Sydney, NSW Australia.

Declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical Statement

This study involved human participants. It was reviewed and approved by The University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee (Project Number 2017/644), and the South Western Sydney Local Health District Human Research Ethics Committee (Local Project No HE17/115). Written informed consent was obtained from mothers for their participation and mothers also provided written informed consent for the participation of their child.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Brockington I (1996) Motherhood and mental health. Oxford University Press, Oxford Brockington I (1996) Motherhood and mental health. Oxford University Press, Oxford
2.
go back to reference Klaus MH, Kennell JH (1982) Parent-infant bonding. Mosby, St. Louis, MO Klaus MH, Kennell JH (1982) Parent-infant bonding. Mosby, St. Louis, MO
3.
go back to reference Bicking Kinsey C, Hupcey JE (2013) State of the science of maternal–infant bonding: A principle-based concept analysis. Midwifery 29:1314–1320PubMedCrossRef Bicking Kinsey C, Hupcey JE (2013) State of the science of maternal–infant bonding: A principle-based concept analysis. Midwifery 29:1314–1320PubMedCrossRef
4.
go back to reference Reck C, Klier CM, Pabst K, Stehle E, Steffenelli U, Struben K et al (2006) The german version of the postpartum bonding instrument: Psychometric properties and association with postpartum depression. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:265–271PubMedCrossRef Reck C, Klier CM, Pabst K, Stehle E, Steffenelli U, Struben K et al (2006) The german version of the postpartum bonding instrument: Psychometric properties and association with postpartum depression. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:265–271PubMedCrossRef
5.
go back to reference van Bussel JC, Spitz B, Demyttenaere K (2010) Three self-report questionnaires of the early mother-to-infant bond: Reliability and validity of the dutch version of the mpas, pbq and mibs. Arch Womens Ment Health 13:373–384PubMedCrossRef van Bussel JC, Spitz B, Demyttenaere K (2010) Three self-report questionnaires of the early mother-to-infant bond: Reliability and validity of the dutch version of the mpas, pbq and mibs. Arch Womens Ment Health 13:373–384PubMedCrossRef
6.
go back to reference Edhborg M, Nasreen H-E, Kabir ZN (2011) Impact of postpartum depressive and anxiety symptoms on mothers’ emotional tie to their infants 2–3 months postpartum: A population-based study from rural bangladesh. Arch Womens Ment Health 14:307PubMedCrossRef Edhborg M, Nasreen H-E, Kabir ZN (2011) Impact of postpartum depressive and anxiety symptoms on mothers’ emotional tie to their infants 2–3 months postpartum: A population-based study from rural bangladesh. Arch Womens Ment Health 14:307PubMedCrossRef
7.
go back to reference Brockington IF (2016) Emotional rejection of the infant: Status of the concept. Psychopathology 49:247–260PubMedCrossRef Brockington IF (2016) Emotional rejection of the infant: Status of the concept. Psychopathology 49:247–260PubMedCrossRef
8.
go back to reference Brockington IF (2011) Maternal rejection of the young child: Present status of the clinical syndrome. Psychopathology 44:329–336PubMedCrossRef Brockington IF (2011) Maternal rejection of the young child: Present status of the clinical syndrome. Psychopathology 44:329–336PubMedCrossRef
9.
go back to reference Brockington IF, Aucamp H, Fraser C (2006) Severe disorders of the mother–infant relationship: Definitions and frequency. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:243–251PubMedCrossRef Brockington IF, Aucamp H, Fraser C (2006) Severe disorders of the mother–infant relationship: Definitions and frequency. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:243–251PubMedCrossRef
10.
go back to reference Afolabi O, Bunce L, Lusher J, Banbury S (2020) Postnatal depression, maternal–infant bonding and social support: A cross-cultural comparison of nigerian and british mothers. J Ment Health 29:424–430PubMedCrossRef Afolabi O, Bunce L, Lusher J, Banbury S (2020) Postnatal depression, maternal–infant bonding and social support: A cross-cultural comparison of nigerian and british mothers. J Ment Health 29:424–430PubMedCrossRef
11.
go back to reference Muzik M, Bocknek EL, Broderick A, Richardson P, Rosenblum KL, Thelen K et al (2013) Mother-infant bonding impairment across the first 6 months postpartum: The primacy of psychopathology in women with childhood abuse and neglect histories. Arch Womens Ment Health 16:29–38PubMedCrossRef Muzik M, Bocknek EL, Broderick A, Richardson P, Rosenblum KL, Thelen K et al (2013) Mother-infant bonding impairment across the first 6 months postpartum: The primacy of psychopathology in women with childhood abuse and neglect histories. Arch Womens Ment Health 16:29–38PubMedCrossRef
12.
go back to reference Halligan SL, Murray L, Martins C, Cooper PJ (2007) Maternal depression and psychiatric outcomes in adolescent offspring: A 13-year longitudinal study. J Affect Disord 97:145–154PubMedCrossRef Halligan SL, Murray L, Martins C, Cooper PJ (2007) Maternal depression and psychiatric outcomes in adolescent offspring: A 13-year longitudinal study. J Affect Disord 97:145–154PubMedCrossRef
13.
go back to reference Moehler E, Brunner R, Wiebel A, Reck C, Resch F (2006) Maternal depressive symptoms in the postnatal period are associated with long-term impairment of mother–child bonding. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:273–278PubMedCrossRef Moehler E, Brunner R, Wiebel A, Reck C, Resch F (2006) Maternal depressive symptoms in the postnatal period are associated with long-term impairment of mother–child bonding. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:273–278PubMedCrossRef
14.
go back to reference Lyons-Ruth K, Lyubchik A, Wolfe R, Bronfman E (2002) Parental depression and child attachment: Hostile and helpless profiles of parent and child behavior among families at risk Lyons-Ruth K, Lyubchik A, Wolfe R, Bronfman E (2002) Parental depression and child attachment: Hostile and helpless profiles of parent and child behavior among families at risk
15.
go back to reference Kitamura T, Ohashi Y, Kita S, Haruna M, Kubo R (2013) Depressive mood, bonding failure, and abusive parenting among mothers with three-month-old babies in a japanese community Kitamura T, Ohashi Y, Kita S, Haruna M, Kubo R (2013) Depressive mood, bonding failure, and abusive parenting among mothers with three-month-old babies in a japanese community
16.
go back to reference Nolvi S, Karlsson L, Bridgett DJ, Pajulo M, Tolvanen M, Karlsson H (2016) Maternal postnatal psychiatric symptoms and infant temperament affect early mother-infant bonding. Infant Behav Dev 43:13–23PubMedCrossRef Nolvi S, Karlsson L, Bridgett DJ, Pajulo M, Tolvanen M, Karlsson H (2016) Maternal postnatal psychiatric symptoms and infant temperament affect early mother-infant bonding. Infant Behav Dev 43:13–23PubMedCrossRef
17.
go back to reference Tietz A, Zietlow A-L, Reck C (2014) Maternal bonding in mothers with postpartum anxiety disorder: The crucial role of subclinical depressive symptoms and maternal avoidance behaviour. Arch Womens Ment Health 17:433–442PubMedCrossRef Tietz A, Zietlow A-L, Reck C (2014) Maternal bonding in mothers with postpartum anxiety disorder: The crucial role of subclinical depressive symptoms and maternal avoidance behaviour. Arch Womens Ment Health 17:433–442PubMedCrossRef
18.
go back to reference Doster A, Wallwiener S, Müller M, Matthies LM, Plewniok K, Feller S et al (2018) Reliability and validity of the german version of the maternal–fetal attachment scale. Arch Gynecol Obstet 297:1157–1167PubMedCrossRef Doster A, Wallwiener S, Müller M, Matthies LM, Plewniok K, Feller S et al (2018) Reliability and validity of the german version of the maternal–fetal attachment scale. Arch Gynecol Obstet 297:1157–1167PubMedCrossRef
19.
go back to reference Dubber S, Reck C, Müller M, Gawlik S (2014) Postpartum bonding: The role of perinatal depression, anxiety and maternal–fetal bonding during pregnancy. Arch Womens Ment Health 18:187–195PubMedCrossRef Dubber S, Reck C, Müller M, Gawlik S (2014) Postpartum bonding: The role of perinatal depression, anxiety and maternal–fetal bonding during pregnancy. Arch Womens Ment Health 18:187–195PubMedCrossRef
20.
go back to reference Fallon V, Silverio SA, Halford JCG, Bennett KM, Harrold JA (2019) Postpartum-specific anxiety and maternal bonding: Further evidence to support the use of childbearing specific mood tools.J Reprod Infant Psychol:1–11 Fallon V, Silverio SA, Halford JCG, Bennett KM, Harrold JA (2019) Postpartum-specific anxiety and maternal bonding: Further evidence to support the use of childbearing specific mood tools.J Reprod Infant Psychol:1–11
21.
go back to reference Figueiredo B, Costa R (2009) Mother’s stress, mood and emotional involvement with the infant: 3 months before and 3 months after childbirth. Arch Womens Ment Health 12:143–153PubMedCrossRef Figueiredo B, Costa R (2009) Mother’s stress, mood and emotional involvement with the infant: 3 months before and 3 months after childbirth. Arch Womens Ment Health 12:143–153PubMedCrossRef
22.
go back to reference Lutkiewicz K, Bieleninik Ł, Cieślak M, Bidzan M (2020) Maternal-infant bonding and its relationships with maternal depressive symptoms, stress and anxiety in the early postpartum period in a polish sample. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17:5427PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lutkiewicz K, Bieleninik Ł, Cieślak M, Bidzan M (2020) Maternal-infant bonding and its relationships with maternal depressive symptoms, stress and anxiety in the early postpartum period in a polish sample. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17:5427PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
23.
go back to reference Tolja R, Nakić Radoš S, Anđelinović M (2020) The role of maternal mental health, infant temperament, and couple’s relationship quality for mother-infant bonding. J Reprod Infant Psychol 38:395–407PubMedCrossRef Tolja R, Nakić Radoš S, Anđelinović M (2020) The role of maternal mental health, infant temperament, and couple’s relationship quality for mother-infant bonding. J Reprod Infant Psychol 38:395–407PubMedCrossRef
24.
go back to reference Lehnig F, Nagl M, Stepan H, Wagner B, Kersting A (2019) Associations of postpartum mother-infant bonding with maternal childhood maltreatment and postpartum mental health: A cross-sectional study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 19:1–10CrossRef Lehnig F, Nagl M, Stepan H, Wagner B, Kersting A (2019) Associations of postpartum mother-infant bonding with maternal childhood maltreatment and postpartum mental health: A cross-sectional study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 19:1–10CrossRef
25.
go back to reference Ohara M, Okada T, Aleksic B, Morikawa M, Kubota C, Nakamura Y et al (2017) Social support helps protect against perinatal bonding failure and depression among mothers: A prospective cohort study. Sci Rep 7:1–8CrossRef Ohara M, Okada T, Aleksic B, Morikawa M, Kubota C, Nakamura Y et al (2017) Social support helps protect against perinatal bonding failure and depression among mothers: A prospective cohort study. Sci Rep 7:1–8CrossRef
26.
go back to reference Ohashi Y, Kitamura T, Kita S, Haruna M, Sakanashi K, Tanaka T (2014) Mothers’ bonding attitudes towards infants: Impact of demographics, psychological attributes, and satisfaction with usual clinical care during pregnancy. Int J Nurs Health Sci 1:16–21 Ohashi Y, Kitamura T, Kita S, Haruna M, Sakanashi K, Tanaka T (2014) Mothers’ bonding attitudes towards infants: Impact of demographics, psychological attributes, and satisfaction with usual clinical care during pregnancy. Int J Nurs Health Sci 1:16–21
27.
go back to reference Zuravin SJ, Fontanella C (1999) Parenting behaviors and perceived parenting competence of child sexual abuse survivors. Child Abuse Negl 23:623–632PubMedCrossRef Zuravin SJ, Fontanella C (1999) Parenting behaviors and perceived parenting competence of child sexual abuse survivors. Child Abuse Negl 23:623–632PubMedCrossRef
28.
go back to reference Smorti M, Ponti L, Ghinassi S, Rapisardi G (2020) The mother-child attachment bond before and after birth: The role of maternal perception of traumatic childbirth. Early Hum Dev 142:104956PubMedCrossRef Smorti M, Ponti L, Ghinassi S, Rapisardi G (2020) The mother-child attachment bond before and after birth: The role of maternal perception of traumatic childbirth. Early Hum Dev 142:104956PubMedCrossRef
29.
go back to reference Klaus MH, Kennell JH (1976) Maternal-infant bonding: The impact of early separation or loss on family development Klaus MH, Kennell JH (1976) Maternal-infant bonding: The impact of early separation or loss on family development
30.
go back to reference Crnic KA, Greenberg MT, Ragozin AS, Robinson NM, Basham RB (1983) Effects of stress and social support on mothers and premature and full-term infants. Child Dev 54:209PubMedCrossRef Crnic KA, Greenberg MT, Ragozin AS, Robinson NM, Basham RB (1983) Effects of stress and social support on mothers and premature and full-term infants. Child Dev 54:209PubMedCrossRef
31.
go back to reference Hoffenkamp HN, Tooten A, Hall RAS, Croon MA, Braeken J, Winkel FW et al (2012) The impact of premature childbirth on parental bonding. Evol Psychol 10:147470491201000CrossRef Hoffenkamp HN, Tooten A, Hall RAS, Croon MA, Braeken J, Winkel FW et al (2012) The impact of premature childbirth on parental bonding. Evol Psychol 10:147470491201000CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Forcada-Guex M, Pierrehumbert B, Borghini A, Moessinger A, Muller-Nix C (2006) Early dyadic patterns of mother-infant interactions and outcomes of prematurity at 18 months. Pediatr 118:e107–e114CrossRef Forcada-Guex M, Pierrehumbert B, Borghini A, Moessinger A, Muller-Nix C (2006) Early dyadic patterns of mother-infant interactions and outcomes of prematurity at 18 months. Pediatr 118:e107–e114CrossRef
33.
go back to reference Greene JG, Fox NA, Lewis M (1983) The relationship between neonatal characteristics and three-month mother-infant interaction in high-risk infants. Child Dev 54:1286PubMedCrossRef Greene JG, Fox NA, Lewis M (1983) The relationship between neonatal characteristics and three-month mother-infant interaction in high-risk infants. Child Dev 54:1286PubMedCrossRef
34.
go back to reference Korja R, Latva R, Lehtonen L (2012) The effects of preterm birth on mother-infant interaction and attachment during the infant’s first two years. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 91:164–173PubMedCrossRef Korja R, Latva R, Lehtonen L (2012) The effects of preterm birth on mother-infant interaction and attachment during the infant’s first two years. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 91:164–173PubMedCrossRef
35.
go back to reference Anderson C, Cacola P (2017) Implications of preterm birth for maternal mental health and infant development. MCN Am J Matern Child Nurs 42:108–114PubMedCrossRef Anderson C, Cacola P (2017) Implications of preterm birth for maternal mental health and infant development. MCN Am J Matern Child Nurs 42:108–114PubMedCrossRef
36.
go back to reference Hairston IS, Handelzalts JE, Lehman-Inbar T, Kovo M (2019) Mother-infant bonding is not associated with feeding type: A community study sample. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 19:125–125PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hairston IS, Handelzalts JE, Lehman-Inbar T, Kovo M (2019) Mother-infant bonding is not associated with feeding type: A community study sample. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 19:125–125PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
37.
go back to reference Martone DJ, Nash BR (1988) Initial differences in postpartum attachment behavior in breastfeeding and bottle-feeding mothers. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 17:212–213PubMedCrossRef Martone DJ, Nash BR (1988) Initial differences in postpartum attachment behavior in breastfeeding and bottle-feeding mothers. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 17:212–213PubMedCrossRef
38.
go back to reference Bicking Kinsey C, Baptiste-Roberts K, Zhu J, Kjerulff KH (2014) Birth-related, psychosocial, and emotional correlates of positive maternal–infant bonding in a cohort of first-time mothers. Midwifery 30:e188–e194CrossRef Bicking Kinsey C, Baptiste-Roberts K, Zhu J, Kjerulff KH (2014) Birth-related, psychosocial, and emotional correlates of positive maternal–infant bonding in a cohort of first-time mothers. Midwifery 30:e188–e194CrossRef
39.
go back to reference McKee MD, Zayas LH, Jankowski KRB (2004) Breastfeeding intention and practice in an urban minority population: Relationship to maternal depressive symptoms and mother–infant closeness. J Reprod Infant Psychol 22:167–181CrossRef McKee MD, Zayas LH, Jankowski KRB (2004) Breastfeeding intention and practice in an urban minority population: Relationship to maternal depressive symptoms and mother–infant closeness. J Reprod Infant Psychol 22:167–181CrossRef
40.
go back to reference Mezzacappa ES, Katkin ES (2002) Breast-feeding is associated with reduced perceived stress and negative mood in mothers. Health Psychol 21:187–193PubMedCrossRef Mezzacappa ES, Katkin ES (2002) Breast-feeding is associated with reduced perceived stress and negative mood in mothers. Health Psychol 21:187–193PubMedCrossRef
41.
go back to reference Brandt KA, Andrews CM, Kvale J (1998) Mother-infant interaction and breastfeeding outcome 6 weeks after birth. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 27:169–174PubMedCrossRef Brandt KA, Andrews CM, Kvale J (1998) Mother-infant interaction and breastfeeding outcome 6 weeks after birth. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 27:169–174PubMedCrossRef
42.
go back to reference Johnson K (2013) Maternal-infant bonding: A review of literature. Int J Childbirth Educ 28:17–22 Johnson K (2013) Maternal-infant bonding: A review of literature. Int J Childbirth Educ 28:17–22
43.
go back to reference Kim P, Feldman R, Mayes LC, Eicher V, Thompson N, Leckman JF et al (2011) Breastfeeding, brain activation to own infant cry, and maternal sensitivity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 52:907–915CrossRef Kim P, Feldman R, Mayes LC, Eicher V, Thompson N, Leckman JF et al (2011) Breastfeeding, brain activation to own infant cry, and maternal sensitivity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 52:907–915CrossRef
44.
go back to reference Ystrom E (2012) Breastfeeding cessation and symptoms of anxiety and depression: A longitudinal cohort study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 12:1–6CrossRef Ystrom E (2012) Breastfeeding cessation and symptoms of anxiety and depression: A longitudinal cohort study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 12:1–6CrossRef
45.
go back to reference Figueiredo B, Canário C, Field T (2014) Breastfeeding is negatively affected by prenatal depression and reduces postpartum depression Figueiredo B, Canário C, Field T (2014) Breastfeeding is negatively affected by prenatal depression and reduces postpartum depression
46.
go back to reference Campbell SB, Cohn JF, Meyers T (1995) Depression in first-time mothers: Mother-infant interaction and depression chronicity. Dev Psychol 31:349–357CrossRef Campbell SB, Cohn JF, Meyers T (1995) Depression in first-time mothers: Mother-infant interaction and depression chronicity. Dev Psychol 31:349–357CrossRef
47.
go back to reference Oddi KB, Murdock KW, Vadnais S, Bridgett DJ, Gartstein MA (2013) Maternal and infant temperament characteristics as contributors to parenting stress in the first year postpartum. Infant Child Dev 22:553–579CrossRef Oddi KB, Murdock KW, Vadnais S, Bridgett DJ, Gartstein MA (2013) Maternal and infant temperament characteristics as contributors to parenting stress in the first year postpartum. Infant Child Dev 22:553–579CrossRef
48.
go back to reference van den Bloom DC, Hoeksma JB (1994) The effect of infant irritability on mother-infant interaction: A growth-curve analysis. Dev Psychol 30:581–590CrossRef van den Bloom DC, Hoeksma JB (1994) The effect of infant irritability on mother-infant interaction: A growth-curve analysis. Dev Psychol 30:581–590CrossRef
50.
go back to reference Hornstein C, Trautmann-Villalba P, Hohm E, Rave E, Wortmann-Fleischer S, Schwarz M (2006) Maternal bond and mother–child interaction in severe postpartum psychiatric disorders: Is there a link? Arch Womens Ment Health 9:279–284PubMedCrossRef Hornstein C, Trautmann-Villalba P, Hohm E, Rave E, Wortmann-Fleischer S, Schwarz M (2006) Maternal bond and mother–child interaction in severe postpartum psychiatric disorders: Is there a link? Arch Womens Ment Health 9:279–284PubMedCrossRef
51.
go back to reference Figueiredo B, Costa R, Pacheco A, Pais Á (2008) Mother-to-infant emotional involvement at birth. Matern Child Health J 13:539–549PubMedCrossRef Figueiredo B, Costa R, Pacheco A, Pais Á (2008) Mother-to-infant emotional involvement at birth. Matern Child Health J 13:539–549PubMedCrossRef
52.
go back to reference Kokubu M, Okano T, Sugiyama T (2012) Postnatal depression, maternal bonding failure, and negative attitudes towards pregnancy: A longitudinal study of pregnant women in japan. Arch Womens Ment Health 15:211–216PubMedCrossRef Kokubu M, Okano T, Sugiyama T (2012) Postnatal depression, maternal bonding failure, and negative attitudes towards pregnancy: A longitudinal study of pregnant women in japan. Arch Womens Ment Health 15:211–216PubMedCrossRef
53.
go back to reference Doyle FL, Mendoza Diaz A, Eapen V, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Hawes DJ et al (2020) Mapping the specific pathways to early-onset mental health disorders: The “watch me grow for real” study protocol. Front Psychiatry 11:553PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Doyle FL, Mendoza Diaz A, Eapen V, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Hawes DJ et al (2020) Mapping the specific pathways to early-onset mental health disorders: The “watch me grow for real” study protocol. Front Psychiatry 11:553PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
54.
go back to reference Brockington IF, Oates J, George S, Turner D, Vostanis P, Sullivan M et al (2001) A screening questionnaire for mother-infant bonding disorders. Arch Womens Ment Health 3:133–140CrossRef Brockington IF, Oates J, George S, Turner D, Vostanis P, Sullivan M et al (2001) A screening questionnaire for mother-infant bonding disorders. Arch Womens Ment Health 3:133–140CrossRef
55.
go back to reference Brockington IF, Fraser C, Wilson D (2006) The postpartum bonding questionnaire: A validation. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:233–242PubMedCrossRef Brockington IF, Fraser C, Wilson D (2006) The postpartum bonding questionnaire: A validation. Arch Womens Ment Health 9:233–242PubMedCrossRef
56.
go back to reference Austin M-P, Colton J, Priest S, Reilly N, Hadzi-Pavlovic D (2013) The antenatal risk questionnaire (anrq): Acceptability and use for psychosocial risk assessment in the maternity setting. Women Birth 26:17–25PubMedCrossRef Austin M-P, Colton J, Priest S, Reilly N, Hadzi-Pavlovic D (2013) The antenatal risk questionnaire (anrq): Acceptability and use for psychosocial risk assessment in the maternity setting. Women Birth 26:17–25PubMedCrossRef
57.
go back to reference Cox JL, Holden JM (2003) Perinatal mental health: A guide to the edinburgh postnatal depression scale (epds). Royal College of Psychiatrists, London Cox JL, Holden JM (2003) Perinatal mental health: A guide to the edinburgh postnatal depression scale (epds). Royal College of Psychiatrists, London
58.
go back to reference Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R (1987) Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br J Psychiatry 150:782–786PubMedCrossRef Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R (1987) Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br J Psychiatry 150:782–786PubMedCrossRef
59.
go back to reference Hewitt CE, Gilbody SM, Mann R, Brealey S (2010) Instruments to identify post-natal depression: Which methods have been the most extensively validated, in what setting and in which language? Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract 14:72–76PubMedCrossRef Hewitt CE, Gilbody SM, Mann R, Brealey S (2010) Instruments to identify post-natal depression: Which methods have been the most extensively validated, in what setting and in which language? Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract 14:72–76PubMedCrossRef
60.
go back to reference Kernot J, Olds T, Lewis LK, Maher C (2015) Test-retest reliability of the english version of the edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Arch Womens Ment Health 18:255–257PubMedCrossRef Kernot J, Olds T, Lewis LK, Maher C (2015) Test-retest reliability of the english version of the edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Arch Womens Ment Health 18:255–257PubMedCrossRef
61.
go back to reference Doyle FL, Mendoza Diaz A, Eapen V, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Hawes DJ et al (2020) Mapping the specific pathways to early-onset mental health disorders: The “watch me grow for real” study protocol. Front Psychiatry 11:553–553PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Doyle FL, Mendoza Diaz A, Eapen V, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Hawes DJ et al (2020) Mapping the specific pathways to early-onset mental health disorders: The “watch me grow for real” study protocol. Front Psychiatry 11:553–553PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
62.
go back to reference Stevens NR, Wallston KA, Hamilton NA (2012) Perceived control and maternal satisfaction with childbirth: A measure development study. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol 33:15–24PubMedCrossRef Stevens NR, Wallston KA, Hamilton NA (2012) Perceived control and maternal satisfaction with childbirth: A measure development study. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol 33:15–24PubMedCrossRef
63.
go back to reference Townsend ML, Brassel AK, Baafi M, Grenyer BF (2020) Childbirth satisfaction and perceptions of control: Postnatal psychological implications. Br J Midwifery 28:225–233CrossRef Townsend ML, Brassel AK, Baafi M, Grenyer BF (2020) Childbirth satisfaction and perceptions of control: Postnatal psychological implications. Br J Midwifery 28:225–233CrossRef
64.
go back to reference Spanier GB (1976) Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads.J Marriage Fam:15–28 Spanier GB (1976) Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads.J Marriage Fam:15–28
65.
go back to reference Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH (1995) The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (dass) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther 33:335–343PubMedCrossRef Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH (1995) The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (dass) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther 33:335–343PubMedCrossRef
66.
go back to reference Page AC, Hooke GR, Morrison DL (2007) Psychometric properties of the depression anxiety stress scales (dass) in depressed clinical samples. Br J Clin Psychol 46:283–297PubMedCrossRef Page AC, Hooke GR, Morrison DL (2007) Psychometric properties of the depression anxiety stress scales (dass) in depressed clinical samples. Br J Clin Psychol 46:283–297PubMedCrossRef
67.
go back to reference Ng F, Trauer T, Dodd S, Callaly T, Campbell S, Berk M (2007) The validity of the 21-item version of the depression anxiety stress scales as a routine clinical outcome measure. Acta Neuropsychiatr 19:304–310PubMedCrossRef Ng F, Trauer T, Dodd S, Callaly T, Campbell S, Berk M (2007) The validity of the 21-item version of the depression anxiety stress scales as a routine clinical outcome measure. Acta Neuropsychiatr 19:304–310PubMedCrossRef
68.
go back to reference Rothbart M, Gartstein M (2000) Infant behavior questionnaire–revised. Items by scale Rothbart M, Gartstein M (2000) Infant behavior questionnaire–revised. Items by scale
69.
go back to reference Gartstein MA, Bridgett DJ, Rothbart MK, Robertson C, Iddins E, Ramsay K et al (2010) A latent growth examination of fear development in infancy: Contributions of maternal depression and the risk for toddler anxiety. Dev Psychol 46:651PubMedCrossRef Gartstein MA, Bridgett DJ, Rothbart MK, Robertson C, Iddins E, Ramsay K et al (2010) A latent growth examination of fear development in infancy: Contributions of maternal depression and the risk for toddler anxiety. Dev Psychol 46:651PubMedCrossRef
70.
go back to reference Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E (2014) Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire–revised. J Pers Assess 96:445–458PubMedCrossRef Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E (2014) Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire–revised. J Pers Assess 96:445–458PubMedCrossRef
71.
go back to reference Wittkowski A, Wieck A, Mann S (2007) An evaluation of two bonding questionnaires: A comparison of the mother-to-infant bonding scale with the postpartum bonding questionnaire in a sample of primiparous mothers. Arch Womens Ment Health 10:171–175PubMedCrossRef Wittkowski A, Wieck A, Mann S (2007) An evaluation of two bonding questionnaires: A comparison of the mother-to-infant bonding scale with the postpartum bonding questionnaire in a sample of primiparous mothers. Arch Womens Ment Health 10:171–175PubMedCrossRef
72.
go back to reference Seng JS, Sperlich M, Low LK, Ronis DL, Muzik M, Liberzon I (2013) Childhood abuse history, posttraumatic stress disorder, postpartum mental health, and bonding: A prospective cohort study. J Midwifery Womens Health 58:57–68PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Seng JS, Sperlich M, Low LK, Ronis DL, Muzik M, Liberzon I (2013) Childhood abuse history, posttraumatic stress disorder, postpartum mental health, and bonding: A prospective cohort study. J Midwifery Womens Health 58:57–68PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
73.
go back to reference Beck AT, Haigh EA (2014) Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: The generic cognitive model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 10:1–24PubMedCrossRef Beck AT, Haigh EA (2014) Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: The generic cognitive model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 10:1–24PubMedCrossRef
74.
go back to reference Mehu M, Scherer KR (2015) The appraisal bias model of cognitive vulnerability to depression. Emot Rev 7:272–279CrossRef Mehu M, Scherer KR (2015) The appraisal bias model of cognitive vulnerability to depression. Emot Rev 7:272–279CrossRef
75.
go back to reference Gotlib IH, Krasnoperova E, Yue DN, Joormann J (2004) Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. J Abnorm Psychol 113:127CrossRef Gotlib IH, Krasnoperova E, Yue DN, Joormann J (2004) Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. J Abnorm Psychol 113:127CrossRef
76.
go back to reference Peckham AD, McHugh RK, Otto MW (2010) A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depress Anxiety 27:1135–1142PubMedCrossRef Peckham AD, McHugh RK, Otto MW (2010) A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depress Anxiety 27:1135–1142PubMedCrossRef
77.
go back to reference Fisher J, Feekery C, Rowe H (2004) Treatment of maternal mood disorder and infant behaviour disturbance in an australian private mothercraft unit: A follow-up study. Arch Womens Ment Health 7:89–93PubMedCrossRef Fisher J, Feekery C, Rowe H (2004) Treatment of maternal mood disorder and infant behaviour disturbance in an australian private mothercraft unit: A follow-up study. Arch Womens Ment Health 7:89–93PubMedCrossRef
78.
go back to reference Hairston IS, Solnik-Menilo T, Deviri D, Handelzalts JE (2016) Maternal depressed mood moderates the impact of infant sleep on mother–infant bonding. Arch Womens Ment Health 19:1029–1039PubMedCrossRef Hairston IS, Solnik-Menilo T, Deviri D, Handelzalts JE (2016) Maternal depressed mood moderates the impact of infant sleep on mother–infant bonding. Arch Womens Ment Health 19:1029–1039PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Towards Preventative Psychiatry: Concurrent and Longitudinal Predictors of Postnatal Maternal-Infant Bonding
Auteurs
Frances L. Doyle
Sophie J. Dickson
Valsamma Eapen
Paul J. Frick
Eva R. Kimonis
David J. Hawes
Caroline Moul
Jenny L. Richmond
Divya Mehta
Mark R. Dadds
Publicatiedatum
26-05-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 6/2023
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-022-01365-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2023

Child Psychiatry & Human Development 6/2023 Naar de uitgave